Siga este enlace para ver otros tipos de publicaciones sobre el tema: Cognitive control processes.

Artículos de revistas sobre el tema "Cognitive control processes"

Crea una cita precisa en los estilos APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard y otros

Elija tipo de fuente:

Consulte los 50 mejores artículos de revistas para su investigación sobre el tema "Cognitive control processes".

Junto a cada fuente en la lista de referencias hay un botón "Agregar a la bibliografía". Pulsa este botón, y generaremos automáticamente la referencia bibliográfica para la obra elegida en el estilo de cita que necesites: APA, MLA, Harvard, Vancouver, Chicago, etc.

También puede descargar el texto completo de la publicación académica en formato pdf y leer en línea su resumen siempre que esté disponible en los metadatos.

Explore artículos de revistas sobre una amplia variedad de disciplinas y organice su bibliografía correctamente.

1

Jarrett, Matthew A., Ansley Tullos Gilpin, Jillian M. Pierucci, and Ana T. Rondon. "Cognitive and reactive control processes." International Journal of Behavioral Development 40, no. 1 (March 10, 2015): 53–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025415575625.

Texto completo
Resumen
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) can be identified in the preschool years, but little is known about the correlates of ADHD symptoms in preschool children. Research to date suggests that factors such as temperament, personality, and neuropsychological functioning may be important in understanding the development of early ADHD symptomatology. The current study sought to extend this research by examining how cognitive and reactive control processes predict ADHD symptoms. Data were drawn from a larger study that measured the cognitive, social, and emotional functioning of preschool
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
2

Whitehead, Peter S., Gene A. Brewer, and Chris Blais. "Are cognitive control processes reliable?" Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 45, no. 5 (May 2019): 765–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000632.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
3

Kopp, Claire B. "Cognitive processes and effortful control." Infant Behavior and Development 21 (April 1998): 190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(98)91405-9.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
4

Cho, Yon Joo. "Learner Control, Cognitive Processes, and Hypertext Learning." Journal of Educational Technology 11, no. 2 (December 30, 1995): 103–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.17232/kset.11.2.103.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
5

Manza, Peter, Ehsan Shokri-Kojori, and Nora D. Volkow. "Reduced Segregation Between Cognitive and Emotional Processes in Cannabis Dependence." Cerebral Cortex 30, no. 2 (June 18, 2019): 628–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz113.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract Addiction is characterized by an erosion of cognitive control toward drug taking that is accentuated by negative emotional states. Here we tested the hypothesis that enhanced interference on cognitive control reflects a loss of segregation between cognition and emotion in addiction. We analyzed Human Connectome Project data from 1206 young adults, including 89 with cannabis dependence (CD). Two composite factors, one for cognition and one for emotion, were derived using principal component (PC) analyses. Component scores for these PCs were significantly associated in the CD group, suc
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
6

Altmann, Erik M. "Fine-Grain Episodic Memory Processes in Cognitive Control." Zeitschrift für Psychologie 221, no. 1 (January 2013): 23–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000127.

Texto completo
Resumen
Previous task-switching research raises a question concerning the role of episodic memory processes in cognitive control. The question is framed by the contrast between two procedures, explicit cuing and randomized runs, one of which presents a task cue perceptually on every trial and the other of which involves uncued trials. The present study compares performance across these procedures. Performance errors sensitive to errors in focusing on the correct task were higher under explicit-cuing conditions than under randomized-runs conditions, consistent with a high level of proactive interferenc
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
7

Dreher, J. C., and K. F. Berman. "Fractionating the neural substrate of cognitive control processes." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99, no. 22 (October 21, 2002): 14595–600. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.222193299.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
8

LENIOR, T. M. J. "Analyses of cognitive processes in train traffic control." Ergonomics 36, no. 11 (November 1993): 1361–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00140139308968005.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
9

Rubinstein, Joshua S., David E. Meyer, and Jeffrey E. Evans. "Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27, no. 4 (2001): 763–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.763.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
10

Smith, Edward E., Teal S. Eich, Deniz Cebenoyan, and Chariklia Malapani. "Intact and impaired cognitive-control processes in schizophrenia." Schizophrenia Research 126, no. 1-3 (March 2011): 132–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2010.11.022.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
11

Erb, Christopher D., Jeff Moher, David M. Sobel, and Joo-Hyun Song. "Reach tracking reveals dissociable processes underlying cognitive control." Cognition 152 (July 2016): 114–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.015.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
12

Amorim, Jéssica Florinda, and Sarah Cassimiro Marques. "Cognitive Processes on Tradicional, Pentecostal and Neopentecostal Views on Protestant Religion." Fragmentos de Cultura 27, no. 4 (February 27, 2018): 544. http://dx.doi.org/10.18224/frag.v27i4.6028.

Texto completo
Resumen
This article produces a relative research accomplished to the gospel population as experimental group, and, as control group, subjects that doesn’t attend any religious institution. Based on neuropsychology and psychology of religion, its purpose to evaluate the flexibility cognition of subjects that are immersed in several protestant contexts. This results pointed significant differences among researched groups.
 
 Processos Cognitivos nas Vertentes Tradicional, Pentecostal e Neopentecostal da Religião Protestante
 
 O presente artigo apresenta uma pesquisa comparativa rea
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
13

Scherbaum, Stefan, Simon Frisch, Anna-Maria Holfert, Denis O'Hora, and Maja Dshemuchadse. "No evidence for common processes of cognitive control and self-control." Acta Psychologica 182 (January 2018): 194–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.11.018.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
14

Eisenreich, Benjamin R., Rei Akaishi, and Benjamin Y. Hayden. "Control without Controllers: Toward a Distributed Neuroscience of Executive Control." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29, no. 10 (October 2017): 1684–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01139.

Texto completo
Resumen
Executive control refers to the regulation of cognition and behavior by mental processes and is a hallmark of higher cognition. Most approaches to understanding its mechanisms begin with the assumption that our brains have anatomically segregated and functionally specialized control modules. The modular approach is intuitive: Control is conceptually distinct from basic mental processing, so an organization that reifies that distinction makes sense. An alternative approach sees executive control as self-organizing principles of a distributed organization. In distributed systems, control and con
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
15

Haykin, Simon. "Cognitive Dynamic Systems." International Journal of Cognitive Informatics and Natural Intelligence 5, no. 4 (October 2011): 33–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jcini.2011100103.

Texto completo
Resumen
The main topics covered in this paper address the following four issues: 1) Distinction between how adaptation and cognition are viewed with respect to each other, 2) With human cognition viewed as the framework for cognition, the following cognitive processes are identified: the perception-action cycle, memory, attention, intelligence, and language. With language being outside the scope of the paper, detailed accounts of the other four cognitive processes are discussed, 3) Cognitive radar is singled out as an example application of cognitive dynamic systems that “mimics” the visual brain; exp
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
16

KERNS, JOHN G. "Experimental manipulation of cognitive control processes causes an increase in communication disturbances in healthy volunteers." Psychological Medicine 37, no. 7 (January 16, 2007): 995–1004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291706009718.

Texto completo
Resumen
Background. Although communication disturbances (CD) have been associated with poor cognitive control, it is unclear whether they are associated specifically with poor cognitive control or with poor cognition in general. The current research examined whether (a) two specific components of cognitive control, working memory and interference resolution, were associated with CD, and (b) associations between CD and cognitive control could be accounted for by generalized poor cognitive performance.Method. In this study, as healthy volunteers spoke, the level of cognitive demands was experimentally i
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
17

Shende, Shraddha A., Lydia T. Nguyen, Elizabeth A. Lydon, Fatima T. Husain, and Raksha A. Mudar. "Cognitive Flexibility and Inhibition in Individuals with Age-Related Hearing Loss." Geriatrics 6, no. 1 (March 5, 2021): 22. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/geriatrics6010022.

Texto completo
Resumen
Growing evidence suggests alterations in cognitive control processes in individuals with varying degrees of age-related hearing loss (ARHL); however, alterations in those with unaided mild ARHL are understudied. The current study examined two cognitive control processes, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition, in 21 older adults with unaided mild ARHL and 18 age- and education-matched normal hearing (NH) controls. All participants underwent comprehensive audiological and cognitive evaluations including Trail Making Test-B, Verbal Fluency, Stroop, and two Go/NoGo tasks. Group differences in cogn
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
18

Kweitel, Ruth, and Felicity C. L. Allen. "Cognitive Processes Associated with Gambling Behaviour." Psychological Reports 82, no. 1 (February 1998): 147–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1998.82.1.147.

Texto completo
Resumen
Gambling behaviours can be pathological if positive response is extreme, but very little is known about the psychological precursors of pathological gambling in Australia. This study examined the relationships between self-reported gambling behaviours and scores on locus of control measures. The sample of 80 male and 75 female undergraduate students completed the South Oaks Gambling Screen and Levenson's multidimensional Locus of Control Scale. No significant association was found for the self-reported gambling behaviours with scores on the Internal scale but a positive one obtained between sc
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
19

Wylie, G. R., D. C. Javitt, and J. J. Foxe. "Cognitive control processes during an anticipated switch of task." European Journal of Neuroscience 17, no. 3 (January 31, 2003): 667–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02474.x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
20

Schneider, Darryl W., and Gordon D. Logan. "Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: Switching tasks in sequences." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135, no. 4 (2006): 623–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.623.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
21

Davelaar, Eddy J. "Processes Versus Representations: Cognitive Control as Emergent, Yet Componential." Topics in Cognitive Science 3, no. 2 (March 17, 2011): 247–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01138.x.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
22

Postigo-Alonso, B., M. Hofmann, A. Kühn, and W. J. Neumann. "P21. Neural correlates of cognitive control in motor processes." Clinical Neurophysiology 129, no. 8 (August 2018): e75-e76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2018.04.663.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
23

Chevalier, Nicolas, Shaina Bailey Martis, Tim Curran, and Yuko Munakata. "Metacognitive Processes in Executive Control Development: The Case of Reactive and Proactive Control." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27, no. 6 (June 2015): 1125–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00782.

Texto completo
Resumen
Young children engage cognitive control reactively in response to events, rather than proactively preparing for events. Such limitations in executive control have been explained in terms of fundamental constraints on children's cognitive capacities. Alternatively, young children might be capable of proactive control but differ from older children in their metacognitive decisions regarding when to engage proactive control. We examined these possibilities in three conditions of a task-switching paradigm, varying in whether task cues were available before or after target onset. RTs, ERPs, and pup
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
24

Agelink van Rentergem, Joost A., Ivar E. Vermeulen, Philippe R. Lee Meeuw Kjoe, and Sanne B. Schagen. "Computational Modeling of Neuropsychological Test Performance to Disentangle Impaired Cognitive Processes in Cancer Patients." JNCI: Journal of the National Cancer Institute 113, no. 1 (April 1, 2020): 99–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jnci/djaa039.

Texto completo
Resumen
Abstract There is a need to better identify impaired cognitive processes to increase our understanding of cognitive dysfunction caused by cancer and cancer treatment and to improve interventions. The Trail Making Test is frequently used for evaluating information-processing speed (part A) and executive function (part B), but interpretation of its outcomes is challenging because performance depends on many cognitive processes. To disentangle processes, we collected high-resolution data from 192 non–central nervous system cancer patients who received systemic therapy and 192 cancer-free control
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
25

Bechtel, William. "Resituating cognitive mechanisms within heterarchical networks controlling physiology and behavior." Theory & Psychology 29, no. 5 (October 2019): 620–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0959354319873725.

Texto completo
Resumen
Cognitive science has traditionally focused on mechanisms involved in high-level reasoning and problem-solving processes. Such mechanisms are often treated as autonomous from but controlling underlying physiological processes. I offer a different perspective on cognition which starts with the basic production mechanisms through which organisms construct and repair themselves and navigate their environments and then I develop a framework for conceptualizing how cognitive control mechanisms form a heterarchical network that regulates production mechanisms. Many of these control mechanisms perfor
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
26

KRYLOV, SERGEY M. "FORMAL TECHNOLOGY AND COGNITIVE PROCESSES." International Journal of General Systems 24, no. 3 (February 1996): 233–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03081079608945119.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
27

Carey, David. "Book Review: Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 55, no. 4 (October 2002): 1385–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000251.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
28

van Driel, Joram, Ilja G. Sligte, Jara Linders, Daniel Elport, and Michael X. Cohen. "Frequency Band-Specific Electrical Brain Stimulation Modulates Cognitive Control Processes." PLOS ONE 10, no. 9 (September 25, 2015): e0138984. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138984.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
29

Dreisbach, Gesine, and Svenja Böttcher. "How the social-evaluative context modulates processes of cognitive control." Psychological Research 75, no. 2 (July 4, 2010): 143–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0298-z.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
30

Ozgoren, Murat, Onur Bayazit, Sibel Kocaaslan, Necati Gokmen, and Adile Oniz. "Probing into cognitive control: Auditory processes in different conscious states." International Journal of Psychophysiology 77, no. 3 (September 2010): 225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.06.321.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
31

Golub, Matthew D., Steven M. Chase, Aaron P. Batista, and Byron M. Yu. "Brain–computer interfaces for dissecting cognitive processes underlying sensorimotor control." Current Opinion in Neurobiology 37 (April 2016): 53–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.12.005.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
32

Higgs, S. "Interactions Between Metabolic, Reward And Cognitive Processes In Appetite Control." Appetite 132 (January 2019): 204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2018.09.025.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
33

Harada, Etsuko, Satoru Suto, Ryuta Takawaki, and Eriko Ankyu. "Cognitive control under choking occurred by unstable task rhythm: Investigating dual processes cognitive control with healthy older adults." Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 84 (September 8, 2020): PI—028—PI—028. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.84.0_pi-028.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
34

Suto, Satoru, Ryuta Takawaki, Eriko Ankyu, and Etsuko Harada. "Cognitive control under choking occurred by unstable task rhythm (1): Investigating dual processes cognitive control with AX-CPT70." Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 84 (September 8, 2020): PI—027—PI—027. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.84.0_pi-027.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
35

Leschziner, Vanina, and Gordon Brett. "Beyond Two Minds: Cognitive, Embodied, and Evaluative Processes in Creativity." Social Psychology Quarterly 82, no. 4 (August 20, 2019): 340–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0190272519851791.

Texto completo
Resumen
Scholars in sociology and social psychology typically represent creativity as an imaginative and deliberate mental activity. Such a perspective has led to a view of creativity as disconnected from the body and the senses as well as from nonanalytic cognition. In this article, we demonstrate that creativity is more grounded in bodily and sensory experience and more reliant on a combination of cognitive processes than has been typically recognized. We use literature on social cognition and embodiment to build our arguments, specifically, the embodied simulation perspective and tripartite process
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
36

Kramer, Arthur F., and Sherry L. Willis. "Enhancing the Cognitive Vitality of Older Adults." Current Directions in Psychological Science 11, no. 5 (October 2002): 173–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00194.

Texto completo
Resumen
Aging is associated with decline in a multitude of cognitive processes and brain functions. However, a growing body of literature suggests that age-related decline in cognition can sometimes be reduced through experience, cognitive training, and other interventions such as fitness training. Research on cognitive training and expertise has suggested that age-related cognitive sparing is often quite narrow, being observed only on tasks and skills similar to those on which individuals have been trained. Furthermore, training and expertise benefits are often realized only after extensive practice
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
37

Drigas, Athanasios, and Maria Karyotaki. "Attentional Control and other Executive Functions." International Journal of Emerging Technologies in Learning (iJET) 12, no. 03 (March 27, 2017): 219. http://dx.doi.org/10.3991/ijet.v12i03.6587.

Texto completo
Resumen
Current article aims to shed light on the reciprocal relation between attentional control and emotional regulation. More specifically, there is a verified relation between attention and cognitive, metacognitive and emotional processes, such as memory, perception, reasoning as well as inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring and positive moods. In addition, positive mood has been already reciprocally related to a broad attentional scope as well as to an increased cognitive flexibility. Future research should focus on the effects of attentional control on cognitive control proc
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
38

Kızılırmak, J. M., F. Rösler, and P. H. Khader. "Control processes during selective long-term memory retrieval." NeuroImage 59, no. 2 (January 2012): 1830–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.041.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
39

de Barros, Graça Monteiro, Filipe Melo, Josefa Domingos, Raul Oliveira, Luís Silva, Júlio Belo Fernandes, and Catarina Godinho. "The Effects of Different Types of Dual Tasking on Balance in Healthy Older Adults." Journal of Personalized Medicine 11, no. 9 (September 18, 2021): 933. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jpm11090933.

Texto completo
Resumen
Numerous of our daily activities are performed within multitask or dual task conditions. These conditions involve the interaction of perceptual and motor processes involved in postural control. Age-related changes may negatively impact cognition and balance control. Studies identifying changes related to dual-task actions in older people are need. This study aimed to determine the effects of different types of dual-tasking on the balance control of healthy older adults. The sample included 36 community-living older adults, performing two tests—a sway test and a timed up-and-go test—in three co
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
40

Furman, Daniella J., Robert L. White, Jenna Naskolnakorn, Jean Ye, Andrew Kayser, and Mark D'Esposito. "Effects of Dopaminergic Drugs on Cognitive Control Processes Vary by Genotype." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32, no. 5 (May 2020): 804–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01518.

Texto completo
Resumen
Dopamine (DA) has been implicated in modulating multiple cognitive control processes, including the robust maintenance of task sets and memoranda in the face of distractors (cognitive stability) and, conversely, the ability to switch task sets or update the contents of working memory when it is advantageous to do so (cognitive flexibility). In humans, the limited specificity of available pharmacological probes has posed a challenge for understanding the mechanisms by which DA, acting on multiple receptor families across the PFC and striatum, differentially influences these cognitive processes.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
41

Stephan, K. E. "Lateralized Cognitive Processes and Lateralized Task Control in the Human Brain." Science 301, no. 5631 (July 18, 2003): 384–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1086025.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
42

Cooper, Patrick S., Aaron S. W. Wong, W. Ross Fulham, Renate Thienel, Elise Mansfield, Patricia T. Michie, and Frini Karayanidis. "Theta frontoparietal connectivity associated with proactive and reactive cognitive control processes." NeuroImage 108 (March 2015): 354–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.028.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
43

Mecklinger, Axel. "The control of long-term memory: Brain systems and cognitive processes." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34, no. 7 (June 2010): 1055–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.020.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
44

Westerhausen, René, Susanne Passow, and Kristiina Kompus. "Reactive cognitive-control processes in free-report consonant–vowel dichotic listening." Brain and Cognition 83, no. 3 (December 2013): 288–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.006.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
45

Xiao, Yuchen, Chien-Chen Chou, Garth Rees Cosgrove, Nathan E. Crone, Scellig Stone, Joseph R. Madsen, Ian Reucroft, et al. "Cross-task specificity and within-task invariance of cognitive control processes." Cell Reports 42, no. 1 (January 2023): 111919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111919.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
46

Tsotsos, John K., Omar Abid, Iuliia Kotseruba, and Markus D. Solbach. "On the control of attentional processes in vision." Cortex 137 (April 2021): 305–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2021.01.001.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
47

Boag, Russell J., Niek Stevenson, Roel van Dooren, Anne C. Trutti, Zsuzsika Sjoerds, and Birte U. Forstmann. "Cognitive Control of Working Memory: A Model-Based Approach." Brain Sciences 11, no. 6 (May 28, 2021): 721. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11060721.

Texto completo
Resumen
Working memory (WM)-based decision making depends on a number of cognitive control processes that control the flow of information into and out of WM and ensure that only relevant information is held active in WM’s limited-capacity store. Although necessary for successful decision making, recent work has shown that these control processes impose performance costs on both the speed and accuracy of WM-based decisions. Using the reference-back task as a benchmark measure of WM control, we conducted evidence accumulation modeling to test several competing explanations for six benchmark empirical pe
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
48

Baumeister, Sarah, Sarah Hohmann, Isabella Wolf, Michael M. Plichta, Stefanie Rechtsteiner, Maria Zangl, Matthias Ruf, et al. "Sequential inhibitory control processes assessed through simultaneous EEG–fMRI." NeuroImage 94 (July 2014): 349–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.023.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
49

Schmajuk, Mariana, Mario Liotti, Laura Busse, and Marty G. Woldorff. "Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults." Neuropsychologia 44, no. 3 (January 2006): 384–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.06.005.

Texto completo
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
50

Holloway, Ralph L. "Language and tool making are similar cognitive processes." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35, no. 4 (June 15, 2012): 226. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11002019.

Texto completo
Resumen
AbstractDesign features for language and stone toolmaking (not tool use) involve similar if not homologous cognitive processes. Both are arbitrary transformations of internal “intrinsic” symbolization, whereas non-human tool using is mostly an iconic transformation. The major discontinuity between humans and non-humans (chimpanzees) is language. The presence of stone tools made to standardized patterns suggests communicative and social control skills that involved language.
Los estilos APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, etc.
Ofrecemos descuentos en todos los planes premium para autores cuyas obras están incluidas en selecciones literarias temáticas. ¡Contáctenos para obtener un código promocional único!