Tesi sul tema "Avoidance motivation"
Cita una fonte nei formati APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard e in molti altri stili
Vedi i top-50 saggi (tesi di laurea o di dottorato) per l'attività di ricerca sul tema "Avoidance motivation".
Accanto a ogni fonte nell'elenco di riferimenti c'è un pulsante "Aggiungi alla bibliografia". Premilo e genereremo automaticamente la citazione bibliografica dell'opera scelta nello stile citazionale di cui hai bisogno: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver ecc.
Puoi anche scaricare il testo completo della pubblicazione scientifica nel formato .pdf e leggere online l'abstract (il sommario) dell'opera se è presente nei metadati.
Vedi le tesi di molte aree scientifiche e compila una bibliografia corretta.
Hamamura, Takeshi. "Approach-avoidance motivation across cultures". Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/1012.
Testo completoWarburton, Victoria Emily. "Approach-avoidance motivation in physical education". Thesis, Loughborough University, 2008. https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/13962.
Testo completoHammill, Amanda C. "Approach/avoidance motivation extensions of the congruency effect /". Cleveland, Ohio : Cleveland State University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=csu1216741968.
Testo completoAbstract. Title from PDF t.p. (viewed on Nov. 6, 2008). Includes bibliographical references (p. 57-64) and appendices. Available online via the OhioLINK ETD Center. Also available in print.
Testa, Rylan Jay. "Motivation for Weight Loss Maintenance: Approach and Avoidance". Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2011. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/101058.
Testo completoPh.D.
Currently, two-thirds of adults and one-half of children and adolescents in the United States meet criteria for overweight or obese weight status (Wang & Beydoun, 2007). While weight loss often is desired among these individuals, few achieve sustained weight loss maintenance. A model of motivation, affect, and long-term weight loss maintenance is proposed, based on Carver's model of behavior change (2004b) and related research. This investigation evaluated the model's proposed associations between approach and avoidance motivation, depressive and anxious affect, and caloric consumption within the context of a dietary lapse condition in the laboratory. Findings did not support the hypotheses that avoidance motivation would be negatively related to calorie consumption or that approach motivation would be positively associated with calorie consumption. Conversely, higher avoidance motivation was found to predict higher caloric consumption. Finally, this relationship was not mediated by depressive or anxious affect. Instead, consumption of high calorie foods alone was shown to fully mediate this relationship. Recommendations for future research are presented in the context of these findings.
Temple University--Theses
Daman, Stuart Jenkins. "The Influence of Humor on Approach and Avoidance Motivation". Cleveland State University / OhioLINK, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=csu1210866811.
Testo completoEerde, Wendelien van. "Work motivation and procrastination : self-set goals and action avoidance /". Inhaltsverzeichnis, 1998. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=008711103&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.
Testo completoKahn, Lauren. "Boredom and the Need for Agency". Thesis, University of Oregon, 2018. http://hdl.handle.net/1794/23755.
Testo completoScott, Mark David. "Measuring Approach-Avoidance Motivation: Expanding the Dimensionality and the Implied Outcomes Problem". Diss., Virginia Tech, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/40404.
Testo completoPh. D.
Hicks, Joshua A. King Laura A. "The effects of approach-avoidance behaviors on goal appraisals". Diss., Columbia, Mo. : University of Missouri--Columbia, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10355/6138.
Testo completoBateman, Tanner Alan. "Construct Deficiency in Avoidance Motivation: Development and Validation of a Scale Measuring Vigilance". Diss., Virginia Tech, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/74003.
Testo completoPh. D.
Marchand, Gwen Catherine. "Motivational processes involved in academic help seeking and help avoidance". PDXScholar, 2004. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/3685.
Testo completoJarvis, Sharon. "Work avoidance as a manifestation of anger, helplessness, and boredom". Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/ftp01/MQ36140.pdf.
Testo completoRock, Mindi S. "Where Do We Draw Our Lines?: Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Political Orientation, and Cognitive Rigidity". Amherst, Mass. : University of Massachusetts Amherst, 2009. http://scholarworks.umass.edu/theses/230/.
Testo completoBrodbeck, Patricia Marie. "The Relations of Conscientiousness, Risk Avoidance, and Growth Motivation with Facebook Usage and Regret". University of Dayton / OhioLINK, 2018. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=dayton1544114921490422.
Testo completoMay, Christine. "Effects of Acute Exercise on Automatic Action-Tendencies and Self-Reported Affect". Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1427979148.
Testo completoKarakus, Melissa. "UNDERSTANDING THE MASTERY-AVOIDANCE GOALS CONSTRUCT: AN INVESTIGATION AMONG MIDDLE SCHOOL STUDENTS IN TWO DOMAINS". Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2016. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/373902.
Testo completoPh.D.
This dissertation addressed knowledge gaps concerning “mastery-avoidance goals”—a construct within the prominent motivational perspective Achievement Goal Theory. Mastery-avoidance goals refer to students’ engagement in an achievement task with the purpose of avoiding failure to develop competence. While it was introduced to the achievement goal literature over a decade and a half ago, the construct of mastery-avoidance goals still lacks intuitive relevance, conceptual clarity, and evidence of prevalence among young students. In addition, so far, research has not established clear patterns of relations of mastery-avoidance goals with the other personal achievement goals (mastery-approach, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance), with contextual motivational emphases, or with adaptive and maladaptive educational outcomes. This dissertation aimed to contribute to knowledge in these gaps by investigating mastery-avoidance goals among middle school students in two subject domains that concern different types of competence: science and instrumental music. The dissertation describes two studies. In Study 1, I administered a self-report measure to middle school students (N=126) that included summated scales to investigate the empirical distinction between mastery-avoidance goals and other achievement goals, the components of its conceptual definition, its prevalence of adoption by young students in the two different domains, as well as its relations with contextual emphases and adaptive and maladaptive educational outcomes. Multidimensional scaling analysis indicated that while students in both science and instrumental music made a distinction between mastery and performance goals, these students did not make a complete distinction between mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals, at least according to the conceptual definition investigated in these studies. Regression analyses indicated that students’ perceptions of their teachers’ emphasis on mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals were significantly related to their reports of mastery-avoidance goals. Cluster analysis suggested a pattern of two general motivational profiles in the sample of more and less motivated students that differed on their simultaneous and respective high and low endorsements of both mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, sense of academic efficacy, and also academic achievement. In Study 2, I aimed to further knowledge of the meaning that students make of mastery-avoidance goals by examining students’ (N=79) qualitative responses to questions asking them to interpret items from the summated-scales self-report measure. Findings from a qualitative content analysis supported the findings from Study 1 about students’ lack of distinction between mastery-approach and mastery-avoidance goals, and indicated that students interpreted mastery-avoidance goals items in ways that were different from those intended by the researchers. These findings suggested that students form meanings of mastery-avoidance goals that are potentially different from the formal conceptual definition in the literature. The findings are interpreted as suggesting that students’ meaning-making about mastery-avoidance goals in both science and instrumental music may be contextualized by their personal characteristics (e.g., age), by characteristics of their school and classroom environments, and by situational characteristics (e.g., proximity of evaluative tasks). Further research should investigate systematically the different personal and contextual factors that may contribute to the meaning students make of mastery-avoidance goals.
Temple University--Theses
Winterheld, Heike A. "Motivations in romantic relationships: a regulatory focus perspective". Thesis, Texas A&M University, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1969.1/2260.
Testo completoMotro, Daphna, e Daniel Sullivan. "Could two negative emotions be a positive? The effects of anger and anxiety in enemyship". ACADEMIC PRESS INC ELSEVIER SCIENCE, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/622804.
Testo completoZamanpour, Haleh. "The mediational roles of performance-avoidance goals pursuit and procrastination in the hierarchical model of achievement motivation". Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 2000. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/ftp01/MQ52375.pdf.
Testo completoZamanpour, Haleh Carleton University Dissertation Psychology. "The Mediational roles of performance-avoidance goals pursuit and procrastination in the hierarchical model of achievement motivation". Ottawa, 2000.
Cerca il testo completoBlackie, Laura E. R. "A test of dual-existential systems : exploring approach and avoidance motivation as a function of mortality awareness". Thesis, University of Essex, 2012. https://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.549304.
Testo completoZhao, Jinling. "The Effects of Fear and Happiness on Intertemporal Decision Making: The Proposed Approach/Avoidance (Inhibition) Motivation Model". Ohio University / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ohiou1426718437.
Testo completoAvdic, Alen. "The Roles of Personality and Attachment in Relation to Academic Motivation". Available to subscribers only, 2009. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1967908791&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=1509&RQT=309&VName=PQD.
Testo completoOuyang, Li. "Motivation, cultural values, learning processes, and learning in Chinese students". Thesis, Kingston, Ont. : [s.n.], 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/1974/1340.
Testo completoDurón, Kelly M. "A Multi-method Approach to Examining Stress and Anxiety Among Mexican American College Students". Thesis, University of North Texas, 2013. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc500090/.
Testo completoAbd-El-Fattah, Sabry M. "Egyptian high school students' perception of parental involvement and its relationship to approach and avoidance achievement motivation : a multidimensional ecological approach". Virtual Press, 2003. http://liblink.bsu.edu/uhtbin/catkey/1264228.
Testo completoDepartment of Educational Psychology
Rawley, Jo Ann K. "Factors perceived to contribute to mathematics avoidance or mathematics confidence in non-traditional age women attending a community college". [Tampa, Fla.] : University of South Florida, 2007. http://purl.fcla.edu/usf/dc/et/SFE0002047.
Testo completoFechtner, Julia [Verfasser], e Ewald [Akademischer Betreuer] Naumann. "The Role of Cognitive Control and Approach-Avoidance Motivation in the Relationship between Stress and Aggression - A Psychophysiological Investigation / Julia Fechtner ; Betreuer: Ewald Naumann". Trier : Universität Trier, 2013. http://d-nb.info/1197698922/34.
Testo completoRichard, Augustin. "Implication du striatum et du pallidum ventral dans le traitement de l'information aversive : approche électrophysiologique et pharmacologique chez le primate non-humain". Thesis, Lyon 1, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015LYO10318/document.
Testo completoStriatum and globus pallidus belong to the basal ganglia, which are a group of subcortical structures involved in motor, cognitive and motivational functions. They are also involved in the reward system which enables one’s motivation to initiate approach behaviors in order to get a reward and then consolidate activities that have produced these behaviors. This represents instrumental learning basis. However, in some situations, the behavior to be initiated depends on a motivation to escape or avoid an aversive situation. For a long time, it was thought that aversive information processing system and reward system depended on different networks and cerebral regions. Yet, a growing number of studies tend to show that basal ganglia certainly play an important role in aversive information processing. In this thesis, we recorded neuronal activity and performed local pharmacological perturbations in non-human primates, in two structures of the reward system, anterior striatum and ventral pallidum, while performing a behavioral task requiring them to initiate alternatively approach behaviors toward a reward and avoidance behaviors from an aversive event. We showed aversive information coding for the predictive stimulus, preparation and initiation of the avoidance behavior and anticipation and response to the aversive events. Furthermore, local perturbation experiments demonstrated that a functional impairment of the anterior striatum and the ventral pallidum affects the behaviors usually initiated by the animals in aversive context. Put together, these results clearly show that these two cerebral structures are involved in both appetitive and aversive motivations
LaBuda, Jessica E. "Accuracy in Perceptions of Partner Sacrifice Motives: An Examination of Individual Differences and Daily Outcomes". Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent155609593349748.
Testo completoAbraham-Smith, Kelly Michelle. "Experiences of mothers who disclose symptoms of postnatal depression". Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2299/17182.
Testo completoTeboul, Anne. "La dynamique des motivations d'approche et d'évitement en contexte d'accomplissement". Thesis, Montpellier, 2020. http://www.theses.fr/2020MONT4003.
Testo completoA dynamical model of approach and avoidance motivation in achievement contexts was recently proposed by Gernigon et al. (2015) as a parsimonious alternative to contemporary models of achievement goals (e.g., Elliot, 2005). The purpose of this doctoral dissertation was to provide the first empirical evidences of the relevance of this model. A first series of studies consisted in developing and validating a questionnaire measuring states of achievement goal involvement according to Elliot et al.'s (2011) 6-goal model: the QIBA-6 (Study 1), as well as a questionnaire measuring the three key social-cognitive variables of the model to be tested (competence expectancies, benefit to the self, and threat to the self that are conveyed by a goal), the interactions of which determine the value of the control parameter k of approach and avoidance motivational attractors: the QSAE (Study 2). A second series of studies was then carried out to test the model and its bases. The first of them (Study 3) led to support some conceptual bases of the model by showing that self- and norm-referenced goals impact self-esteem differently. The last two studies (studies 4 and 5) aimed to validate the dynamic properties of the model. Thus, Study 4 led to support the properties of non-linearity and resistance to change in motivational states, under the influence of gradual variations in the parameter k and in the resulting approach and avoidance attractor landscape. Finally, based on longitudinal data collected in natural achievement contexts (academic and sports), Study 5 enabled the detection of the prevalence of two clusters corresponding to the motivational attractors of approach and avoidance, as well as the characterization of the dynamics of motivational states as mainly modelable in terms of moving average processes, which reflects their environmental sensitivity. The results of this doctoral work provide the first empirical validations of the dynamical model of approach and avoidance in achievement contexts. By showing the utility of the dynamical systems approach, they also invite to continue—via agent-based modeling—the investigation of the emergence of approach and avoidance motivational states as they result from self-organization processes. The model, as currently validated, already offers opportunities for practical applications for anyone (coach, teacher, educator, coach) in charge of the development and maintenance of individuals' motivation in achievement contexts
Cox, Christie M. "Approach and Avoidance Motivations: Implications for Organizational Justice". Akron, OH : University of Akron, 2009. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=akron1239567858.
Testo completo"May, 2009." Title from electronic dissertation title page (viewed 12/2/2009) Advisor, Aaron M. Schmidt; Committee members, Rosalie J. Hall, Paul E. Levy, Yang Lin, Robert G. Lord; Department Chair, Paul E. Levy; Dean of the College, Chand Midha; Dean of the Graduate School, George R. Newkome. Includes bibliographical references.
Krpan, Dario. "Approach and avoidance motivations and the perception of valenced stimuli". Thesis, University of Cambridge, 2015. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.709411.
Testo completoSmith, Daniel R. "Implicit personality and leadership in stressful and dangerous situations: a first step". Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/43693.
Testo completoFroemming, Maren Wright. "The Relation of Unwanted Consensual Sex to Mental Health and Relationship Variables: The Role of Motivations". Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1594345616905041.
Testo completoDunn, Emily Justine. "Interpersonal outcomes and motivational factors associated with overcontrol". The Ohio State University, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1589019723586709.
Testo completoKuschel, Stefanie [Verfasser]. "Going beyond information given : how approach versus avoidance motivational cues influence encoding of meaning and details / Stefanie Kuschel". Bremen : IRC-Library, Information Resource Center der Jacobs University Bremen, 2008. http://d-nb.info/1034893718/34.
Testo completoGendre, Manon. "Influence des émotions sur l’organisation biomécanique des mouvements volontaires d’approche et d’évitement : cas de l’initiation du pas et de l’élévation latérale de la jambe". Thesis, Université Paris-Saclay (ComUE), 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015SACLS202/document.
Testo completoThe aim of this thesis is to investigate the influence of emotions on the planning and execution of whole body voluntary movement. The effect of emotions was test thanks to the motivational direction hypothesis which supposed that 1) pleasant stimuli should facilitate approach and perturb avoidance behaviors. 2) Unpleasant stimuli should facilitate avoidance and perturb approach behaviors. Two experimental protocols have been chosen to pursue the investigation: Forward and backward gait initiation (study 1) and lateral elevation of the leg (studies 2-4). The emotions were manipulated by exposing the subjects to pictures of the International Affective Pictures System (inducing pleasant and unpleasant emotions, study 1) and by placing the subjects at the edge of an elevated platform (inducing fear of falling). Movements were performed on force platform (studies 1-3). Kinematic parameters of leg movement were recorded by the system of motion capture VICON (studies 2 and 3). Psychological parameters (valence and arousal dimensions of emotions fear of falling) were assessed by questionnaires. Results of the first study showed that pleasant pictures facilitated approach behaviors (as observed in the reactivity) and perturbed avoidance behaviors (as observed in the velocity of centre of mass during anticipatory postural adjustment (APA) prior to the execution of a backward step), compared to unpleasant or neutral pictures. However, these pleasant pictures also perturbed approach behaviors (as observed in the peak of centre of mass velocity during the execution of a forward step). Results of the second study showed that approach behaviors toward the postural threat (i.e, the void) were perturbed (as observed in the amplitude and duration of APA), compared to avoidance behaviors. The third study focused on the inter-individual differences in the relation between postural threat and organization of movement. Results revealed that balance confidence of the participants dealing with increased postural threat. In other terms, participants reported a high confidence in their own balance control were least perturbed than those reported a low balance confidence and vice versa. Finally, in the fourth study we tried to explain these inter-individual differences. The self-esteem seems to explain these differences. In conclusion, the results of this thesis seem to partially confirm the hypothesis of motivational direction. Put together, the results help improve our general understanding of the influence of emotions on the planning and execution of voluntary movements.Keywords: Anticipatory Postural Adjustments, Approach-Avoidance, Emotions, Motivational Direction Hypothesis, Motor Control
Dejitthirat, Kullaya Wicker Frank W. Neff Kristin D. "Avoidance motivation". 2004. http://repositories.lib.utexas.edu/bitstream/handle/2152/1916/dejitthiratk042.pdf.
Testo completoArnett, Peter Andrew. "Approach and avoidance motivation in incarcerated psychopaths during passive avoidance". 1992. http://catalog.hathitrust.org/api/volumes/oclc/28444575.html.
Testo completoTypescript. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 73-80).
Su, Tsung-yu, e 蘇琮瑜. "The Correlation between College Students\' Interests Adaptation, Avoidance Motivation and Learning Motivation". Thesis, 2019. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/r89xup.
Testo completo國立彰化師範大學
輔導與諮商學系
107
This study was to explore the relationship between college students' interest congruence, achievement motivation and learning motivation, and the degree of difference between different background variables. The sample were surveying for the “University Career and Competency Assessment Network Career Interest survey” test . The sample were distributed 514 questionnaires in total , 493 of which are valid .The study use three measurement, inclusive of UCAN Interest survey test results, learning motivation and achievement motivation. The data of the questionnaires was analyzed by descriptive statistics, one-way analysis of variance, Pearson product-moment correlation and Multiple regression analysis. The study has the following conclusions. 1.There is no significant difference between college students of different genders and grades in learning motivation and departmental interest congruence. 2.College students from different colleges have significant differences in learning motivation and departmental interest congruence. 3.There are significant differences in the achievement motivation of college students of different grades. 4.Learning motivation is highly related to department interest congruence and achievement motivation. Based on the above research results, suggestions for teaching, counseling work and future research are proposed.
Dejitthirat, Kullaya. "Avoidance motivation : its manifestation in goals across cultures". Thesis, 2004. http://hdl.handle.net/2152/1916.
Testo completoElliot, Andrew James. "Approach and avoidance achievement goals an intrinsic motivation analysis /". 1994. http://catalog.hathitrust.org/api/volumes/oclc/32512421.html.
Testo completoTypescript. eContent provider-neutral record in process. Description based on print version record. Includes bibliographical references (leaves 88-115).
Kordts-Freudinger, Robert. "Relief: Approach Behavior and Avoidance Goals". Doctoral thesis, 2010. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-55366.
Testo completoDie Arbeit behandelt die Frage, mit welcher motivationalen Richtung, Annäherung oder Vermeidung, die Emotion Erleichterung verbunden ist - Erleichterung ist dabei definiert als positive Emotion niedriger Erregung, die durch eine erwartete oder unerwartete motiv-konsistente Veränderung zum Besseren, also durch die Abwesendheit aversiver Stimulation, verursacht wird. Basierend auf der in der Literatur vertretenen Idee, mehrere Ebenen der Annäherungs- Vermeidungs-Motivation zu postulieren, wird das Reflektiv-Impulsiv Modell menschlichen Verhaltens (RIM, Strack & Deutsch, 2004) auf die Erleichterung und Annäherung-Vermeidung angewendet. Im RIM wird zwischen einem impulsiven und einem reflektiven System der Informationsverarbeitung unterschieden, beide Systeme arbeiten dabei relativ unabhängig voneinander. Zwei zentrale Variablen moderieren danach den Zusammenhang zwischen Erleichterung und Annäherungs-Vermeidung. Der erste Moderator ist das psychologische System, in dem Annäherung-Vermeidung verarbeitet und gemessen wird. Hierzu werden zwei Ebenen von Annäherung-Vermeidung unterschieden: eine impulsive Distanzveränderungsorientierung (Distanzveränderung bezüglich spezifischen Reizes) und eine reflektive Zielorientierung (Erreichung positiver oder Vermeidung negativer Endzustände). Der zweite Moderator ist das psychologische System, in dem die Erleichterung entsteht: Im impulsiven System entsteht Erleichterung als an die Abwesenheit negativer Zustände konditionierter Affekt; im reflektiven System entsteht Erleichterung als Resultat zielgesteuerten Verhaltens bei der Kontrolle oder Verhinderung einer negativen Stimulation. Die Arbeit betrachtet dabei beide Moderatoren (Ebene der Annäherung-Vermeidung und Entstehungssystem der Erleichterung) gleichzeitig. Für die impulsive Distanzorientierung werden daraus die folgenden zentralen Vorhersagen abgeleitet: Erleichterung löst eine Annäherungs-Distanzorientierung (Distanzverringerung) aus, unabhängig davon, ob Erleichterung im impulsiven oder im reflektiven System entsteht. Für die reflektive Zielorientierung wird abgeleitet: Erleichterung löst eine Vermeidungs-Zielorientierung (Vermeidung negativer Endzustände) aus. Diese letzte Vorhersage gilt allerdings nur dann, wenn die Erleichterung im reflektiven System entsteht, also durch eigenes, zielgeleitetes Verhalten entsteht; außerdem ist es für eine Vermeidungs-Zielorientierung nötig, dass die Erleichterung nicht sicher erreicht werden kann, sondern immer ein Anteil Unsicherheit bei der Vermeidung negativer Zustände bestehen bleibt. Die in der Arbeit verwendete Methodik basiert auf Studien zur aversiven Konditionierung. In der Arbeit wird mehrheitlich ein Differenzierungsparadigma gewählt. Die im impulsiven System entstehende Erleichterung wird dabei mit einer klassischen Konditionierung hergestellt (als aversiver CS-), die im reflektiven System entstehende Erleichterung wird mit einem aktiven Vermeidungs-Paradigma hergestellt, das die methodische Vergleichbarkeit der „reflektiven Erleichterung“ mit der „impulsiven Erleichterung“ sicherstellt. Die Vorhersagen sind wie folgt: Vorhersage A: Sowohl impulsive als auch reflektive Erleichterung löst eine positive affektive Valenz und eine impulsive Annäherungs-Distanzorientierung aus (Experimente 2-3). Vorhersage B: Die positivere Valenz von Erleichterung, die durch eine größere affektive Veränderung verursacht wird, verstärkt die Annäherungs-Distanzorientierung (Experiment 4). Vorhersage C: Impulsive Erleichterung beeinflusst die Zielorientierung nicht (Experiment 5). Vorhersage D: Unsichere reflektive Erleichterung löst eine Vermeidungs-Zielorientierung aus (Experimente 6-7). Zusätzlich validiert Experiment 1 das Konditionierungsparadigma, das für die Erzeugung der Erleichterung benutzt wird. Die vorliegenden Experimente unterstützen alle im Theoretischen Teil gemachten Vorhersagen. Die vorliegende Arbeit hat Implikationen für die Annahmen über das impulsive System sowie das reflektive System im RIM (Strack & Deutsch, 2004). Im impulsiven System ist die affektive Valenz entscheidend für die Annäherungs-Vermeidungs-Orientierung (z.B. R. Neumann & Strack, 2000), das reflektive Ziel spielt dabei keine Rolle. Impulsiv löst Erleichterung eine Annäherungs-Orientierung aus. Im reflektiven System dagegen ist das aktive Ziel entscheidend für die Annäherungs-Vermeidungs-Orientierung. Reflektiv löst nur unsichere selbst bewirkte Erleichterung eine Vermeidungsorientierung aus. Die vorliegenden Studien unterstützen und validieren damit die Annahmen des RIM (Strack & Deutsch, 2004) auf dem spezifischen Feld der motivationalen Richtung
Schomberg, Jessica. "Motivation and the brain: How do appetitive versus aversive states relate to electroencephalographic activity?" Doctoral thesis, 2016. https://repositorium.ub.uni-osnabrueck.de/handle/urn:nbn:de:gbv:700-2016010713781.
Testo completoFerris, Douglas Lance. "Core Self-Evaluations and the Hierarchical Model of Approach/Avoidance Motivation". Thesis, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10012/3808.
Testo completoSzymaniak, Kinga. "Cognitive consequences of trait anger: the role of motivation". Doctoral thesis, 2021. https://depotuw.ceon.pl/handle/item/4083.
Testo completoGłównym celem projektu było zweryfikowanie, w jakim stopniu motywacja dążeniowa odpowiada za związek cechy gniewu z tendencyjnością poznawczą na trzech poziomach przetwarzania informacji: sądami na temat bodźców zewnętrznych (reprezentowanymi przez preferencję bodźców o charakterze nagradzającym, powiązanych z wysokim lub niskim poziomem pobudzenia), sądami na temat siebie (reprezentowanymi przez nadmiernie pozytywny obraz własnej zdolności do bycia sarkastycznym), oraz sądami dotyczącymi świata społecznego (reprezentowanymi przez wiarę w teorie spiskowe). Ponadto, postawiono pytanie czy cecha gniewu może również, oprócz motywacji dążeniowej, wiązać się z motywacją unikającą i czy oba aspekty cechy gniewu mają odrębne efekty poznawcze. W tym celu sprawdzono jak oba warianty gniewu wiążą się z wiarą w teorie spiskowe. Weryfikację tych relacji poprzedzono opracowaniem i walidacją Skali Gniewu o Motywacji Unikającej (the Avoidance Motivational Anger Scale, AMAS) – samo- opisowego narzędzia do pomiaru cechy gniewu powiązanego z motywacją unikającą. Badania 1a i 1b ujawniły pozytywny związek cechy gniewu z lubieniem bodźców nagradzających, oraz że pobudzenie powiązane z bodźcem ma znaczenie dla siły tej relacji. Ponadto, badanie 1b wykazało, że wrażliwość na nagrodę (ang. reward responsiveness), czyli wymiar motywacji dążeniowej, odpowiada za związek między gniewem a lubieniem bodźców nagradzających, zarówno tych o wysokim jak i niskim pobudzeniu. Pośredni efekt (ang. indirect effect) orientacji hedonistycznej okazał się być istotny tylko dla lubienia bodźców nagradzających o wysokim pobudzeniu. Badania 2a i 2b wykazały istotny związek między gniewem jako cechą a subiektywnie, ale nie obiektywnie, szacowaną zdolnością do stosowania sarkazmu. Sugeruje to, że osoby o wysokiej cesze gniewu mogą przejawiać tendencję do wydawania zbyt pozytywnych sądów na temat swojej zdolności do bycia sarkastycznym. Jak pokazują wyniki 6 badania 2b, tendencja ta może być związana ze zwiększonym pragnieniem stymulacji i poszukiwaniem nowych nagradzających doświadczeń (reprezentowanych przez wysokie wyniki na wymiarze poszukiwania przyjemności, ang. BAS-Fun seeking). Kolejne cztery badania (3a, 3b, 3c i 4c) wykazały pozytywne powiązania między gniewem zorientowanym dążeniowo a wiarą w szeroki zakres teorii spiskowych (w tym przekonań o charakterze ogólnym, teorii związanych z pandemią koronawirusa i postawami anty-szczepionkowymi). W badaniach 3a, 3b i 3c motywacja zorientowana na działanie (ang. BAS-Drive) okazała się wyjaśniać te relacje. Ponadto, badanie 4c ujawniło negatywny kierunek relacji między wiarą w określone przekonania spiskowe, a cechą gniewu zorientowanego unikająco (przeciwnie niż w przypadku gniewu zorientowanego dązeniowo), dodatkowo podkreślając rolę motywacji w badanych relacjach. Wreszcie, badania 4a i 4b poświęcone były stworzeniu i walidacji skali AMAS w dwóch różnych wersjach językowych. Wyniki obu badań zgodnie wskazały, że AMAS jest rzetelną i trafną miarą gniewu jako cechy zorientowanej na unikanie, sugerując tym samym możliwość stosowania tego narzędzia do celów badawczych. Podsumowując, przedstawione badania dostarczają istotnych dowodów na to, że motywacyjny aspekt odgrywa ważną rolę w kształtowaniu tendencyjności poznawczych związanych z gniewem. Ponadto, jednoznacznie podważają wciąż popularne i uproszczone założenia przedstawiające gniew jako emocję związaną z wyłącznie szkodliwymi aspektami ludzkiego poznania.
Hong, Yi-Hong, e 洪翊鴻. "Interactions in Close Relationships: The Exploration of the Associations among Rejection Sensitivity, Approach Motivation and Avoidance Motivation". Thesis, 2018. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/ux9gk3.
Testo completo國立政治大學
心理學系
106
Previous research shows that motivations can predict action and response. These findings can maintain quality of close relationships.There are two types of motivation in close relationships:approach and avoidance motivations. Approach motivations can predict positive response and avoidance motivation can predict negative response in close relationships, but how strong the prediction is depend on individual difference. Previous research shows that compared to individuals with low rejection sensitivity, avoidance motivations are more associated with negative response in individuals with high rejection sensitivity. And there are different cognition and affect responses between people with low and high rejection sensitivity. Main goals of this research are to investigate if motivations (approach, avoidance) can predict emotion response in close relationship and if these associations are modulated by rejection sensitivity. There are two studies in this research. Study1 was a survey study (longitudinal) that measured participants’ approach motivation, avoidance motivations, rejection sensitivity, and response in close relationship (positive and negative emotion, relationship satisfaction). Study1 found that avoidance motivations could predict negative emotions, approach motivations could predict positive emotions and relationship satisfaction. Study2 was an experiment that contained two manipulations of motivations and it used a scenario story to manipulate ambiguous rejection cues. Study2 mainly found that manipulations of motivations could predict change of state motivation, state approach motivation could predict positive emotions, state avoidance motivation could predicted negative emotions, and association between avoidance motivation and negative emotions was modulated by participants’ rejection sensitivity. Results in study2 suggested that manipulations of motivations could predict change of emotions in close relationships, and that is helpful to maintain quality of close relationships. Two studies provided convergent evidence to this research to suggest that motivations can predict emotions in close relationships, and association between avoidance motivations and negative emotions is modulated rejection sensitivity.
Maillet, Myles A. "Self-regulation of healthy eating: the role of motivation and approach-avoidance goals". Thesis, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/1828/8306.
Testo completoGraduate
2018-06-11