Tesi sul tema "Figure-ground perception"
Cita una fonte nei formati APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard e in molti altri stili
Vedi i top-20 saggi (tesi di laurea o di dottorato) per l'attività di ricerca sul tema "Figure-ground perception".
Accanto a ogni fonte nell'elenco di riferimenti c'è un pulsante "Aggiungi alla bibliografia". Premilo e genereremo automaticamente la citazione bibliografica dell'opera scelta nello stile citazionale di cui hai bisogno: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver ecc.
Puoi anche scaricare il testo completo della pubblicazione scientifica nel formato .pdf e leggere online l'abstract (il sommario) dell'opera se è presente nei metadati.
Vedi le tesi di molte aree scientifiche e compila una bibliografia corretta.
Adamo, Stephen Hunter. "Semantic Suppression in Figure-Ground Perception and Binocular Rivalry". Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/146907.
Testo completoTurner, Maureen Cassidy. "The Role of Working Memory in Bistable Figure-Ground Perception". Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/146696.
Testo completoAnderson, John A. E., M. Karl Healey, Lynn Hasher e Mary A. Peterson. "Age-related deficits in inhibition in figure-ground assignment". ASSOC RESEARCH VISION OPHTHALMOLOGY INC, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/617415.
Testo completoWhite, Hannah, Rachel Jubran, Alison Heck, Alyson Chroust e Ramesh S. Bhatt. "The Role of Shape Recognition in Figure/Ground Perception in Infancy". Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2018. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/2729.
Testo completoLass, Jordan W., Patrick J. Bennett, Mary A. Peterson e Allison B. Sekuler. "Effects of aging on figure-ground perception: Convexity context effects and competition resolution". ASSOC RESEARCH VISION OPHTHALMOLOGY INC, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/623274.
Testo completoTrujillo, Logan Thomas. "Electrophysiological Correlates of the Influences of Past Experience on Conscious and Unconscious Figure-Ground Perception". Diss., The University of Arizona, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/194981.
Testo completoCacciamani, Laura M. "Beyond Conscious Object Perception: Processing and Inhibition of the Groundside of a Figure". Diss., The University of Arizona, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/332846.
Testo completoSanguinetti, Joseph LaCoste. "The Dynamics Of Perceptual Organization In The Human Visual System; Competition In Time". Diss., The University of Arizona, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/333347.
Testo completoCampbell, Elizabeth Marie Salvagio, e Elizabeth Marie Salvagio Campbell. "How do 5.5-month-old Infants Learn to Segment Objects from their Backgrounds?" Diss., The University of Arizona, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/623064.
Testo completoRAZ, DANNAH GECLEWICZ. "VISUAL PERCEPTION AND INFANT DEVELOPMENT: CAN INFANTS USE CONVEXITY AS A CUE FOR FIGURE/GROUND SEGREGATION". Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/192206.
Testo completoBarnes, Timothy. "Visual depth perception from texture accretion and deletion: a neural model of figure-ground segregation and occlusion". Thesis, Boston University, 2012. https://hdl.handle.net/2144/31504.
Testo completoPLEASE NOTE: Boston University Libraries did not receive an Authorization To Manage form for this thesis or dissertation. It is therefore not openly accessible, though it may be available by request. If you are the author or principal advisor of this work and would like to request open access for it, please contact us at open-help@bu.edu. Thank you.
Freezing is an effective defense strategy for some prey, because their predators rely on visual motion to distinguish objects from their surroundings. An object moving over a background progressively covers (deletes) and uncovers (accretes) background texture while simultaneously producing discontinuities in the optic flow field. These events unambiguously specify kinetic occlusion and can produce a crisp edge, depth perception, and figure-ground segregation between identically textured surfaces -- percepts which all disappear without motion. Given two abutting regions of uniform random texture with different motion velocities, one region will appear to be situated farther away and behind the other (i.e., the ground), if its texture is accreted or deleted at the boundary between the regions, irrespective of region and boundary velocities. Consequently, a region with moving texture appears farther away than a stationary region if the boundary is stationary, but it appears closer (i.e. the figure) if the boundary is moving coherently with the moving texture. The perception of kinetic occlusion requires the detection of an unexpected onset or offset of otherwise predictably moving or stationary contrast patches. A computational model of directional selectivity in visual cells is here extended to also detect motion onsets and offsets. The connectivity of these model cells not only affords the detection of local texture accretion and deletion events but also explains results showing that human reaction times differ for motion onsets versus offsets. These theorized cells are placed into a larger computational model of visual areas V1 and V2 to show how interactions between orientation- and direction-selective cells first create a motion-defined boundary and then signal texture accretion or deletion at that boundary. A weak speed-depth bias brings faster-moving texture regions forward in depth. This is consistent with percepts: the faster of two surfaces appears closer when moving parallel to the resulting emergent boundary between them (shearing motion). Activation of model occlusion detectors tuned to a particular velocity results in the model assigning the adjacent surface with a matching velocity to the far depth. These processes together reproduce human psychophysical reports of depth ordering for a representative set of all kinetic occlusion displays.
2031-01-01
Yang, Weilu. "Moments of Absence Phenomenology of Perception in Representing the Absent Moments". University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1397737336.
Testo completoGoksun-Yoruk, Tilbe. "The 'Who' and 'Where' of Events: Infants' Processing of Figures and Grounds in Events". Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2010. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/100828.
Testo completoPh.D.
Learning relational terms such as verbs and prepositions is fundamental to language development. To learn relational words, children must first dissect and process dynamic event components, and then uncover how the particular language they are learning encodes these constructs. Building on a new area of research, this dissertation investigated two event components, figure (i.e., the moving entity) and ground (i.e., the stationary setting) that are central to learning relational words. In particular, we examine how English- and Japanese-reared infants process figures and grounds in nonlinguistic events and how language learning interacts with their conceptualization of these constructs. Four studies were designed to probe our questions. Study 1 examined English-reared infants' ability to form nonnative ground categories encoded only in Japanese. For example, "crossing a road," which extends in a line and is bounded, is expressed differently than "crossing a field" that extends in a plane and is unbounded. We found that infants can detect the geometry of the ground and form a nonnative ground category. Study 2 indicated that the path of an action plays a role in construing these categorical ground distinctions such that without the bounded paths infants do not differentiate between grounds. Study 3 demonstrated that even though infants notice figures and grounds in static representations of the dynamic events (even earlier for the ground discrimination), the Japanese categorical ground differentiation no longer emerged. In the last set of studies, we showed that despite the sensitivity to the event structure and categorical ground distinctions in dynamic events by both English- and Japanese-reared infants (Study 4a), only Japanese toddlers retained these categorical distinctions (Study 4b). Overall, these results suggest that 1) infants distinguish between figures and grounds in events with differential attention to static and dynamic displays; 2) before learning much about their native language infants form nonnative event categories; and 3) the process of learning language appears to shift earlier formed categorical boundaries.
Temple University--Theses
Chang, Ssu-chien, e 張絲茜. "A Study of figure-ground perception in Chinese typography design". Thesis, 2013. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/50653961867275968705.
Testo completo朝陽科技大學
工業設計系碩士班
101
Applying figure-ground perception to Chinese typography design is a way that makes connection between character strokes, and further reverses its perceived hollow/full spatiality. This kind of typography design conveys the meaning of the character, while at the same time brings attractiveness to the design. Figure-ground illustrations that are originally admired for its graphic presentations became more intriguing because of the distinguishing feature of Chinese characters. This paper reviewed cases of Chinese typography design and figure-ground perception, sorted out their correlativity, and generalized several ways of applying figure-ground perception to Chinese typography design. In this paper, we found that “strokes of the characters” and “combination of shape” are the two critical elements of applying figure-ground perception to Chinese typography design. Furthermore, there are four general methods to correlate the two, including “strokes connecting between characters”, “characters deconstructing and surrounding”, “strokes sharing and reversing”, and “perspective stroke contours and reversing”. These methods can be adopted as the basic design concept, and further apply to posters, identification products and T-shirts. While making actual designs, we faced difficulties and limitations of applying figure-ground perception to Chinese typography design. We concluded that the character should be appropriately chosen to carry out the theme; also, the graphic shouldn’t be too complicated to steal the visual focus of the characters; last but not least, the design should be presenting both the full structure of a Chinese character and the idea of figure-ground perception.
Kim, Sung-Ho. "Globally inconsistent figure/ground relations induced by a negative part". 2009. http://hdl.rutgers.edu/1782.2/rucore10001600001.ETD.000052253.
Testo completoHecht, Lauren Nicole Vecera Shaun P. "Temporal processing of figures and grounds". 2009. http://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/298.
Testo completoKuo, Ying-Miao, e 郭盈妙. "Generating Ambiguous Figure-Ground Images and Pop-up Design via 2D Shape Perception and Analysis". Thesis, 2017. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/28hg79.
Testo completoLayton, Oliver W. "Neural models of inter-cortical networks in the primate visual system for navigation, attention, path perception, and static and kinetic figure-ground perception". Thesis, 2013. https://hdl.handle.net/2144/15224.
Testo completoKrigolson, Olave Edouard. "Facilitation or interference? the influence of visual cues on the accuracy and control of visually-guided and memory-dependent reaches /". 2003. http://www.oregonpdf.org.
Testo completoSmit, Susanna Margrietha. "Instabilities of visual perception in the 'Bath Series' of Jasper Johns (1983-1988)". Thesis, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/10539/12049.
Testo completoThe ‘Bath Series’ (1983-1988) of Jasper Johns shows the artist’s meditation on his oeuvre of the past thirty years, and the examples of his previous works demonstrate his interest in instabilities of visual perception. The latter are activated when the viewer’s expectation to see conventional representational strategies are destabilized, and figure/ground pictorial space, particularly, becomes ambiguous. This first recorded academic study focusing exclusively on the series as a unit, discovers that figure/ground switching, an ‘Ur-Gestalt’ (Gandelman 1989: 209), appears to be a core energy motivating ambiguous pictorial space in Johns’ art, and constitutes the theoretical component of the research. The practical component is a site specific installation which shows some visual and verbal processes and meditates on the perpetual interaction between the eye and the mind, which is a fundamental concern of Johns (Varnedoe 1996b: 245, 257), as well as of myself. The work invites viewers to experience destabilized conventional visual perceptions and to explore, as Johns said, ‘something new’ (Varnedoe 1996a: 17).