Auswahl der wissenschaftlichen Literatur zum Thema „Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)“

Geben Sie eine Quelle nach APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard und anderen Zitierweisen an

Wählen Sie eine Art der Quelle aus:

Machen Sie sich mit den Listen der aktuellen Artikel, Bücher, Dissertationen, Berichten und anderer wissenschaftlichen Quellen zum Thema "Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)" bekannt.

Neben jedem Werk im Literaturverzeichnis ist die Option "Zur Bibliographie hinzufügen" verfügbar. Nutzen Sie sie, wird Ihre bibliographische Angabe des gewählten Werkes nach der nötigen Zitierweise (APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver usw.) automatisch gestaltet.

Sie können auch den vollen Text der wissenschaftlichen Publikation im PDF-Format herunterladen und eine Online-Annotation der Arbeit lesen, wenn die relevanten Parameter in den Metadaten verfügbar sind.

Zeitschriftenartikel zum Thema "Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)"

1

Ash, Jude, und K. Lira Yoon. „Negative emotions and emotion avoidance in victim derogation✰“. Personality and Individual Differences 154 (Februar 2020): 109674. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109674.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Mitmansgruber, Horst, Thomas N. Beck und Gerhard Schüßler. „“Mindful helpers”: Experiential avoidance, meta-emotions, and emotion regulation in paramedics“. Journal of Research in Personality 42, Nr. 5 (Oktober 2008): 1358–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.012.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Dewey, Daniel, David Schuldberg und Renee Madathil. „Do Peritraumatic Emotions Differentially Predict PTSD Symptom Clusters? Initial Evidence for Emotion Specificity“. Psychological Reports 115, Nr. 1 (August 2014): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/16.02.pr0.115c11z7.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This study investigated whether specific peritraumatic emotions differentially predict PTSD symptom clusters in individuals who have experienced stressful life events. Hypotheses were developed based on the SPAARS model of PTSD. It was predicted that the peritraumatic emotions of anger, disgust, guilt, and fear would significantly predict re-experiencing and avoidance symptoms, while only fear would predict hyperarousal. Undergraduate students ( N = 144) participated in this study by completing a packet of self-report questionnaires. Multiple regression analyses were conducted with PCL-S symptom cluster scores as dependent variables and peritraumatic fear, guilt, anger, shame, and disgust as predictor variables. As hypothesized, peritraumatic anger, guilt, and fear all significantly predicted re-experiencing. However, only fear predicted avoidance, and anger significantly predicted hyperarousal. Results are discussed in relation to the theoretical role of emotions in the etiology of PTSD following the experience of a stressful life event.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Marotta, Angela, Mirta Fiorio, Marianna Riello, Benedetta Demartini, Ginevra Tecilla, Carlo Dallocchio und Michele Tinazzi. „Attentional avoidance of emotions in functional movement disorders“. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 133 (Juni 2020): 110100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2020.110100.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Nes, Lise Solberg, und Suzanne C. Segerstrom. „Dispositional Optimism and Coping: A Meta-Analytic Review“. Personality and Social Psychology Review 10, Nr. 3 (August 2006): 235–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_3.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
The relation between dispositional optimism and better adjustment to diverse stressors may be attributable to optimism's effects on coping strategies. A meta-analytic review ( K = 50, N = 11,629) examined the impact of dispositional optimism on coping. Dispositional optimism was found to be positively associated with approach coping strategies aiming to eliminate, reduce, or manage stressors or emotions ( r = .17), and negatively associated with avoidance coping strategies seeking to ignore, avoid, or withdraw from stressors or emotions ( r = -.21). Effect sizes were larger for the distinction between approach and avoidance coping strategies than for that between problem and emotion-focused coping. Meta-analytic findings also indicate that optimists may adjust their coping strategies to meet the demands of the stressors at hand, and that the optimism-coping relationship is strongest in English-speaking samples.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

van Middendorp, Henriët, Mark A. Lumley, Johannes W. G. Jacobs, Lorenz J. P. van Doornen, Johannes W. J. Bijlsma und Rinie Geenen. „Emotions and emotional approach and avoidance strategies in fibromyalgia“. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 64, Nr. 2 (Februar 2008): 159–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.08.009.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
7

Cova, Florian, Julien Deonna und David Sander. „The emotional shape of our moral life: Anger-related emotions and mutualistic anthropology“. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36, Nr. 1 (Februar 2013): 86–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x12000763.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
AbstractThe evolutionary hypothesis advanced by Baumard et al. makes precise predictions on which emotions should play the main role in our moral lives: morality should be more closely linked to “avoidance” emotions (like contempt and disgust) than to “punitive” emotions (like anger). Here, we argue that these predictions run contrary to most psychological evidence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
8

Lennarz, Hannah K., Tom Hollenstein, Anna Lichtwarck-Aschoff, Emmanuel Kuntsche und Isabela Granic. „Emotion regulation in action: Use, selection, and success of emotion regulation in adolescents’ daily lives“. International Journal of Behavioral Development 43, Nr. 1 (05.02.2018): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025418755540.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Successful emotion regulation (ER) is a central aspect of psychosocial functioning and mental health and is thought to improve and be refined in adolescence. Past research on ER has mainly focused on one-time measurements of habitual ER. Linking regulatory strategies to emotions in daily lives is key to understanding adolescents’ emotional lives. Using an Experience Sampling Method with 78 adolescents ( Mage = 13.91, SDage = .95, 66% girls), we investigated the use, selection, and success in down-regulating negative emotions of eight ER strategies across 44 assessments. Acceptance was the strategy employed most often followed by problem-solving, rumination, distraction, avoidance, reappraisal, social support, and suppression. Interestingly, negativity of the event influenced the use of ER strategies: With low intensity negative emotions, acceptance was more likely to be used, and with high intensity negative emotions, suppression, problem-solving, distraction, avoidance, social support, and rumination were more likely to be used. With regard to success, multilevel models revealed that problem-solving, reappraisal, and acceptance were more successful in down-regulating negative emotions than rumination. Further, among girls, no relations between the momentary use of ER strategies and depressive symptoms was found. Among boys, a negative relation between acceptance and depressive symptoms emerged. Results from this study suggest that there is a reciprocal relationship between the intensity of negative emotions and ER strategies and that gender differences may exist. Taken together, this study showed which ER strategies are used by a healthy adolescent sample, and these results are discussed with regard to their theoretical and practical importance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
9

Robazza, Claudio, Laura Bortoli, Attilio Carraro und Maurizio Bertollo. „Approach-Avoidance Individual Differences in Changing Students' Responses to Physical Education“. Perceptual and Motor Skills 104, Nr. 3 (Juni 2007): 937–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.104.3.937-946.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
The purpose of this report was to examine the effects of physical education acrobatic activities as a function of individual differences on approach-avoidance tendencies for acrobatics. The data of a study conducted by Robazza, Bortoli, Carraro, and Bertollo (2006) were analyzed after having classified students as high- or low-avoiders. Approach-avoidance tendencies and idiosyncratic emotions related to acrobatic tasks and adventurous sports were originally assessed for 72 Italian male high school students. Experimental participants engaged in acrobatic tasks of physical education for 12 lessons, while control participants were involved in team sports. Analysis showed that high-avoiders changed their emotions positively toward physical education tasks more than low-avoiders, whereas the latter modified their attitudes for adventurous sports. Approach-avoidance tendencies can be expected to moderate involvement in challenging physical activities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
10

Znoj, Hans J., und Peter Lude. „Regulation of emotion and psychological symptoms in people with spinal cord injury“. Swiss Journal of Psychology 61, Nr. 4 (Dezember 2002): 203–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.61.4.203.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This cross-sectional study examined the contribution of regulation of emotion on depression, symptoms of traumatic and general distress, perceived disability, and somatic symptoms following spinal cord injury. Depressed and non-depressed participants (N = 264, mean age = 46 years) differed in their use of adaptive and maladaptive ways of regulating emotions. In addition to somatic symptoms and symptoms of traumatic stress, maladaptive emotional regulation (avoidance and distortion of awareness) accounted for 8% of the total variance of depression. It is concluded that adaptive and maladaptive ways of regulating emotions are important factors in dealing with negative feelings. Regulation of emotion appears to influence perceived disability mainly via depression. Limitations of the study and implications for rehabilitation are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Mehr Quellen

Dissertationen zum Thema "Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)"

1

Polifroni, Mark. „The development and testing of a multi-component emotion induction method“. Columbus, Ohio : Ohio State University, 2006. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=osu1141669741.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Burris, Elizabeth D. „Emotions in the classroom /“. View online, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10090/5871.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Boulie, Elizabeth M. „The Impact of Friendships and Mutual Antipathies on Children's Social Behavior and Social Cognition“. TopSCHOLAR®, 2009. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/89/.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Andersson, K. Pernille. „Changing the servicescape : The influence of music, self-disclosure and eye gaze on service encounter experience and approach-avoidance behavior“. Doctoral thesis, Karlstads universitet, Institutionen för sociala och psykologiska studier, 2016. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:kau:diva-46417.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
The purpose of this thesis is to investigate and understand the effect of a servicescape’s ambient and social conditions on consumers’ service encounter experience and their approach/avoidance behavior in a retail context. In three papers, with a total sample of over 1600 participants (including 550 actual consumers) and seven experiments, the author investigates the effect of music (ambient stimuli), employees’ self-disclosure (verbal social stimuli) and employees’ gazing behavior (nonverbal social stimuli) on consumers’ service encounter experience and approach/avoidance behavior in a retail store. Paper I comprised two experiments, and the aim was to investigate the influence of music on emotions, approach/avoidance behavior. Paper II comprised two experiments, and the aim was to investigate the effect of frontline employees’ personal self-disclosure on consumers’ reciprocal behavior. Paper III comprised three experiments, and the aim was to investigate the influence of employee’s direct eye gaze/ averted eye gaze on consumer emotions, social impression of the frontline employee and encounter satisfaction in different purchase situations. The results in this thesis show that music affects consumers in both positive and negative ways (Paper I). Self-disclosure affects consumers negatively, in such a way that it decreases encounter satisfaction  (Paper II) and, finally, eye gaze affects consumers by regulating both positively – and in some cases also negatively – consumers’ social impression of the frontline employee and their encounter satisfaction (Paper III). The conclusions of this thesis are that both ambient and social stimuli in a servicescape affect consumers’ internal responses, which in turn affect their behavior. Depending on the purchase situation, type of retail, and stimuli, the internal and behavioral responses are different.
The purpose of this thesis is to investigate and understand the effect of a servicescape’s ambient and social conditions on consumers’ service encounter experience and their approach/avoidance behavior in a retail context. In three papers, with a total sample of over 1600 participants (including 550 actual consumers) and seven experiments, the author investigates the effect of music, employees’ self-disclosure and employees’ gazing behavior on consumers’ service encounter experience and approach/avoidance behavior in a retail store. The results in this thesis show that music affects consumers in both positive and negative ways (Paper I). Self-disclosure affects consumers negatively, in such a way that it decreases encounter satisfaction (Paper II) and, finally, eye gaze affects consumers by regulating both positively – and in some cases also negatively – consumers’ social impression of the frontline employee and their encounter satisfaction (Paper III). The conclusions of this thesis are that both ambient and social stimuli in a servicescape affect consumers’ internal responses, which in turn affect their behavior. Depending on the purchase situation, type of retail, and stimuli, the internal and behavioral responses are different.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Shea, Amanda Marie. „Attachment Avoidance and Depressive Symptoms: A Test of Moderation by Cognitive Abilities“. Thesis, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/1805/4982.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Indiana University-Purdue University Indianapolis (IUPUI)
The substantial interpersonal and economic costs of depression make it imperative to better understand the predictors and moderators of depressive symptoms. The ability to use social support protects people from depressive symptoms, but individuals high in attachment avoidance tend not to use others as sources of support. Research has found that attachment avoidance is related to depressive symptoms in some samples but not in others (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Shea, 2011). Thus, there appear to be factors that moderate the relationship between attachment avoidance and depressive symptoms. The present study examined if cognitive abilities that facilitate effective emotion regulation strategies moderate the relationship between attachment avoidance and depressive symptoms. Using a sample of college students, attachment avoidance, cognitive abilities, depressive symptoms, and other indices of psychological distress and well-being were measured and examined for evidence of moderation via hierarchical linear regression. The hypothesis that cognitive abilities moderate the relationship between attachment avoidance and depressive symptoms was not supported (ΔR2 = 0.02, p = .68). Factors contributing to the null findings are discussed and conceptual and methodological suggestions are offered for future research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
6

Vorster, Anja. „I feel for you, therefore, I respond on your behalf: Social psychological processes leading to and consequences of vicarious humiliation“. Thesis, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/10500/27714.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Vicarious humiliation as a devaluing intergroup event is a rather common experience, which has the potential to adversely influence present and future intergroup relations. Based on an extensive literature review and previous research, we hypothesised that highly identified group members experience an intensified feeling of humiliation after witnessing an ingroup member being humiliated when compared to low identifiers (Hypothesis 1), that the role of visual exposure as situational determinant of humiliating events, the appraisals, and the emotional patterns elicited, differ between personally and vicariously humiliating events (Hypotheses 2a, 2b, 3 and 4), and lastly, that vicarious humiliation regulated through emotional blends leads to behavioural intentions that influence future intergroup relations (Hypotheses 5a to 5c). Evidence for our hypotheses was exploratively and experimentally provided in six studies. Results implied that vicarious humiliation is a common experience, that visual exposure as situational determinant is more important for personally than vicariously humiliating events, and that humiliation is indeed a blended emotion (Study 1, N = 1048). Moreover, results showed that highly identified group member feel relatively stronger humiliated (Study 2, N = 175), that the appraisal and emotional patterns are related to identity processes (i.e., personal and vicarious humiliation) (Study 3, N = 74; Study 4, N = 359; Study 5 = 376), and that the feeling of humiliation and accompanying emotions regulate the relationship between vicariously humiliating events and the intentional responses such as avoidance, non-normative approach, dehumanisation and social exclusion (Study 6, N = 998). Overall, our results imply that vicarious humiliation as an emotional experience has the potential to provoke intergroup conflict. SUMMARY The present research studied a phenomenon that we are all familiar with – being humiliated. Unfortunately, this is an experience that is rather common as we might not only experience to be humiliated personally but also to be humiliated on behalf of others. It is this vicarious experience of humiliation that the present research aimed at studying. We firstly explored people’s experiences with and understandings of humiliation through a cross-sectional survey (Study 1). Results indicated that vicarious humiliation is indeed a rather common experience, that personally and vicariously humiliating events differ in terms of the situational determinants that characterise these events, and that the feeling of humiliation is experienced as a blended emotion. We furthermore tested experimentally the effects of ingroup identification, identity processes and the presence of an audience on the appraisal processes of and the emotional and motivational responses to vicarious humiliation. We found that people who highly identified with the group they share with the humiliated person, experienced stronger feelings of humiliation (Study 2), and that being personally humiliated and being vicariously humiliated resulted in different appraisal patterns, which consequently elicited the different emotional blends of humiliation with self-focused and other-focused emotions, respectively (Studies 3 to 5). We were, however, unable to provide evidence that the presence of an audience aggravated the appraisal processes and the feeling of humiliation (which we attributed to methodological limitations of our studies). That the emotional blends of humiliation regulate the behavioural intentions, that people engage in as a result of being vicariously humiliated, was demonstrated in our last study (Study 6). More specifically, we found that humiliation accompanied by self-focused emotions was related to intentions to avoid, to non-normatively approach, and/or to socially exclude the humiliator(s) through dehumanising them. It is this latter finding that provides evidence for both the role of the social context that might determine the appropriateness of certain behaviours (e.g., social norms) and for the proposed cycle of humiliation in that humiliated persons are often believed to retaliate by humiliating the humiliator(s) in return, which has the potential to provoke intergroup conflicts.
Psychology
D. Phil. (Psychology)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
7

Vorster, Anja. „I feel for you. therefore, I respond on your behalf. Social psychological processes leading to and consequences of vicarious humiliation“. Thesis, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/10500/27714.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Vicarious humiliation as a devaluing intergroup event is a rather common experience, which has the potential to adversely influence present and future intergroup relations. Based on an extensive literature review and previous research, we hypothesised that highly identified group members experience an intensified feeling of humiliation after witnessing an ingroup member being humiliated when compared to low identifiers (Hypothesis 1), that the role of visual exposure as situational determinant of humiliating events, the appraisals, and the emotional patterns elicited, differ between personally and vicariously humiliating events (Hypotheses 2a, 2b, 3 and 4), and lastly, that vicarious humiliation regulated through emotional blends leads to behavioural intentions that influence future intergroup relations (Hypotheses 5a to 5c). Evidence for our hypotheses was exploratively and experimentally provided in six studies. Results implied that vicarious humiliation is a common experience, that visual exposure as situational determinant is more important for personally than vicariously humiliating events, and that humiliation is indeed a blended emotion (Study 1, N = 1048). Moreover, results showed that highly identified group member feel relatively stronger humiliated (Study 2, N = 175), that the appraisal and emotional patterns are related to identity processes (i.e., personal and vicarious humiliation) (Study 3, N = 74; Study 4, N = 359; Study 5 = 376), and that the feeling of humiliation and accompanying emotions regulate the relationship between vicariously humiliating events and the intentional responses such as avoidance, non-normative approach, dehumanisation and social exclusion (Study 6, N = 998). Overall, our results imply that vicarious humiliation as an emotional experience has the potential to provoke intergroup conflict. SUMMARY The present research studied a phenomenon that we are all familiar with – being humiliated. Unfortunately, this is an experience that is rather common as we might not only experience to be humiliated personally but also to be humiliated on behalf of others. It is this vicarious experience of humiliation that the present research aimed at studying. We firstly explored people’s experiences with and understandings of humiliation through a cross-sectional survey (Study 1). Results indicated that vicarious humiliation is indeed a rather common experience, that personally and vicariously humiliating events differ in terms of the situational determinants that characterise these events, and that the feeling of humiliation is experienced as a blended emotion. We furthermore tested experimentally the effects of ingroup identification, identity processes and the presence of an audience on the appraisal processes of and the emotional and motivational responses to vicarious humiliation. We found that people who highly identified with the group they share with the humiliated person, experienced stronger feelings of humiliation (Study 2), and that being personally humiliated and being vicariously humiliated resulted in different appraisal patterns, which consequently elicited the different emotional blends of humiliation with self-focused and other-focused emotions, respectively (Studies 3 to 5). We were, however, unable to provide evidence that the presence of an audience aggravated the appraisal processes and the feeling of humiliation (which we attributed to methodological limitations of our studies). That the emotional blends of humiliation regulate the behavioural intentions, that people engage in as a result of being vicariously humiliated, was demonstrated in our last study (Study 6). More specifically, we found that humiliation accompanied by self-focused emotions was related to intentions to avoid, to non-normatively approach, and/or to socially exclude the humiliator(s) through dehumanising them. It is this latter finding that provides evidence for both the role of the social context that might determine the appropriateness of certain behaviours (e.g., social norms) and for the proposed cycle of humiliation in that humiliated persons are often believed to retaliate by humiliating the humiliator(s) in return, which has the potential to provoke intergroup conflicts.
Psychology
D. Phil. (Psychology)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen

Bücher zum Thema "Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)"

1

Avoiding emotions, living emotions. Hove, East Sussex: Routledge, 2011.

Den vollen Inhalt der Quelle finden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

1954-, Barber Jacques P., Hrsg. Echoes of the trauma: Relational themes and emotions in children of Holocaust survivors. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2008.

Den vollen Inhalt der Quelle finden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Schwartz, Nina. Dead fathers: The logic of transference in modern narrative. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1994.

Den vollen Inhalt der Quelle finden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
4

Le défi des relations: Comment résoudre nos transferts affectifs. [Montréal]: Éditions de l'Homme, 2004.

Den vollen Inhalt der Quelle finden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
5

Mitrani, Judith L. Psychoanalytic Technique and Theory: Taking the Transference. Taylor & Francis Group, 2014.

Den vollen Inhalt der Quelle finden
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen

Buchteile zum Thema "Emotions. Transference (Psychology) Avoidance (Psychology)"

1

Ghaemi, S. Nassir. „Psychology of Psychopharmacology“. In Clinical Psychopharmacology, herausgegeben von S. Nassir Ghaemi, 437–39. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780199995486.003.0042.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
The practice of giving medications for psychiatric conditions has inevitable psychological aspects. A central one is transference and countertransference, the unconscious and conscious emotions that arise between patient and clinician. Secondary gain can be another factor, with conscious non-medical goals on the part of the patient—as in other psychiatric settings, patients either come to treatment, or avoid it, because of reasons or factors which may have nothing to do with the actual treatment itself. Pill-seeking patients who come for clinical care sometimes aggressively seek medications. The social role of the psychiatric clinician is relevant as well, as patients in the Western world in particular go to mental health professionals for concerns that sometimes are non-medical in nature. In the past, people with personal problems unrelated to diseases often went to priests or other religious guides. Currently, in the industrialized West, fewer people follow those traditions and many instead seek the help of mental health professionals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
2

Nezu, Christine Maguth, Christopher R. Martell und Arthur M. Nezu. „Psychotherapeutic Interventions“. In Specialty Competencies in Cognitive and Behavioral Psychology, 87–116. Oxford University Press, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/9780195382327.003.0006.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
Chapter 6 assumes the challenge of describing cognitive and behavioural interventions and clinical strategies that are required for competent practice without reducing the specialty to a menu of techniques applied to specific problems. It describes a range of interventions that represent the application of learning theories to reduce symptoms and increase skills, with the overall goal of improving quality of life. In addition to the techniques that have their hallmark in basic learning theory, it also presents interventions that have their origins in information processing theory and reflect how the specialty expanded in the 1970s to add a strong social-cognitive focus. Lastly, it discusses the influence of the past few decades to include the importance of affect regulation, and targeted avoidance of negative thoughts and emotions in what has been referred to as the ‘third wave’ of cognitive behavioural intervention.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
3

Craig, A. D. (Bud). „Feelings and Emotions On Both Sides of the Brain“. In How Do You Feel? Princeton University Press, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.23943/princeton/9780691156767.003.0008.

Der volle Inhalt der Quelle
Annotation:
This chapter addresses the different roles of the left and right anterior insular cortex (AIC). It begins by noting evidence that the left AIC and the right AIC were activated asymmetrically in many of the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies mentioned in the preceding chapters. The chapter then details two recent reviews that document a consistent pattern of asymmetric activation of the amygdala and insular cortex, as well as the gender differences that had obscured this pattern. The accumulated evidence from years of work in psychology shows that electroencephalographic activation in the left frontal hemisphere correlates with positive affect and approach motivation, and that activation in the right frontal hemisphere correlates with negative affect, cortisol release, sympathetic arousal, and avoidance motivation. The chapter also describes evidence that supports the opponent inhibition model and the nature of emotional imbalance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO und andere Zitierweisen
Wir bieten Rabatte auf alle Premium-Pläne für Autoren, deren Werke in thematische Literatursammlungen aufgenommen wurden. Kontaktieren Sie uns, um einen einzigartigen Promo-Code zu erhalten!

Zur Bibliographie