To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Auditiv stimulus.

Journal articles on the topic 'Auditiv stimulus'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Auditiv stimulus.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Dyson, Benjamin J., and Philip T. Quinlan. "Stimulus Processing Constraints in Audition." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30, no. 6 (2004): 1117–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.30.6.1117.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Castro, Candela, Veronika Diaz Abrahan, and Nadia Justel. "Modulación del estado de ánimo a través de estímulos musicales activantes." Interdisciplinaria Revista de Psicología y Ciencias Afines 38, no. 1 (2020): 41–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.16888/interd.2021.38.1.3.

Full text
Abstract:
La música provoca respuestas emocionales que se ven reflejadas en los cambios fisiológicos y cognitivos de los oyentes. Es una temática de estudio en común entre distintas disciplinas como la psicología de la música, la musicoterapia, la antropología de la música y la la neuropsicología, entre otras. Entender de qué manera la música puede incidir en el estado de ánimo de las personas permite contribuir al conocimiento sobre el efecto comportamental y a la creación de nuevas técnicas orientadas para regular las emociones mediante la música. Teniendo en cuenta estos antecedentes, se realizó un e
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Schnuerch, Robert, Carina Kreitz, and Kathrin Lange. "Independent effects of temporal expectation and stimulus intensity in audition." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 75, no. 7 (2013): 1520–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0491-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Cysarz, Dirk, Arndt Büssing, Gudrun Bornhöft, Matthias Geisse, Peter F. Matthiessen, and Thomas Ostermann. "Wirkung von Gong-Klängen auf das kardiorespiratorische System." Musik-, Tanz- und Kunsttherapie 18, no. 2 (2007): 66–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1026/0933-6885.18.2.66.

Full text
Abstract:
Zusammenfassung. Hintergrund: Neben dem Monochord kann auch der Gong als passiv-rezeptive Therapieform in der Musiktherapie eingesetzt werden. Die Wirkungen von Gongklängen wird von vielen Patienten übereinstimmend als positiv beschrieben. Neben einer Besserung des psychischen Befindens berichten Patienten häufig auch von einer Beeinflussung des Herzschlags. Fragestellung: Ziel dieser Pilotstudie war daher die Überprüfung eines Einflusses von Gongklängen auf die Rhythmen von Herzschlag und Atmung. Methoden: In Ergänzung zu den Gongklängen wurde fünf Probanden neben “Ruhe“ als Referenz weiterhi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Kubovy, Michael, and Minhong Yu. "Multistability, cross-modal binding and the additivity of conjoined grouping principles." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367, no. 1591 (2012): 954–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0365.

Full text
Abstract:
We present a sceptical view of multimodal multistability—drawing most of our examples from the relation between audition and vision. We begin by summarizing some of the principal ways in which audio-visual binding takes place. We review the evidence that unambiguous stimulation in one modality may affect the perception of a multistable stimulus in another modality. Cross-modal influences of one multistable stimulus on the multistability of another are different: they have occurred only in speech perception. We then argue that the strongest relation between perceptual organization in vision and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Stephens, Greg J., Christopher J. Honey, and Uri Hasson. "A place for time: the spatiotemporal structure of neural dynamics during natural audition." Journal of Neurophysiology 110, no. 9 (2013): 2019–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00268.2013.

Full text
Abstract:
We use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to analyze neural responses to natural auditory stimuli. We characterize the fMRI time series through the shape of the voxel power spectrum and find that the timescales of neural dynamics vary along a spatial gradient, with faster dynamics in early auditory cortex and slower dynamics in higher order brain regions. The timescale gradient is observed through the unsupervised clustering of the power spectra of individual brains, both in the presence and absence of a stimulus, and is enhanced in the stimulus-locked component that is shared across
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Wang, Aijun, Heng Zhou, Yuanyuan Hu, et al. "Endogenous Spatial Attention Modulates the Magnitude of the Colavita Visual Dominance Effect." i-Perception 12, no. 4 (2021): 204166952110271. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/20416695211027186.

Full text
Abstract:
The Colavita effect refers to the phenomenon wherein people tend to not respond to an auditory stimulus when a visual stimulus is simultaneously presented. Although previous studies have shown that endogenous modality attention influences the Colavita effect, whether the Colavita effect is influenced by endogenous spatial attention remains unknown. In the present study, we established endogenous spatial cues to investigate whether the size of the Colavita effect changes under visual or auditory cues. We measured three indexes to investigate the effect of endogenous spatial attention on the siz
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Villanueva, Lia, and Massimiliano Zampini. "Reciprocal Interference Between Audition and Touch in the Perception of Duration." Multisensory Research 31, no. 5 (2018): 351–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002583.

Full text
Abstract:
Audition and touch interact with one another and share a number of similarities; however, little is known about their interplay in the perception of temporal duration. The present study intended to investigate whether the temporal duration of an irrelevant auditory or tactile stimulus could modulate the perceived duration of a target stimulus presented in the other modality (i.e., tactile or auditory) adopting both a between-participants (Experiment 1) and a within-participants (Experiment 2) experimental design. In a two-alternative forced-choice task, participants decided which of two events
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Convento, Silvia, Kira A. Wegner-Clemens, and Jeffrey M. Yau. "Reciprocal Interactions Between Audition and Touch in Flutter Frequency Perception." Multisensory Research 32, no. 1 (2019): 67–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20181334.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In both audition and touch, sensory cues comprising repeating events are perceived either as a continuous signal or as a stream of temporally discrete events (flutter), depending on the events’ repetition rate. At high repetition rates (>100 Hz), auditory and tactile cues interact reciprocally in pitch processing. The frequency of a cue experienced in one modality systematically biases the perceived frequency of a cue experienced in the other modality. Here, we tested whether audition and touch also interact in the processing of low-frequency stimulation. We also tested whether mul
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Schneider, Till R., Andreas K. Engel, and Stefan Debener. "Multisensory Identification of Natural Objects in a Two-Way Crossmodal Priming Paradigm." Experimental Psychology 55, no. 2 (2008): 121–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.55.2.121.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. The question of how vision and audition interact in natural object identification is currently a matter of debate. We developed a large set of auditory and visual stimuli representing natural objects in order to facilitate research in the field of multisensory processing. Normative data was obtained for 270 brief environmental sounds and 320 visual object stimuli. Each stimulus was named, categorized, and rated with regard to familiarity and emotional valence by N = 56 participants (Study 1). This multimodal stimulus set was employed in two subsequent crossmodal priming experiments t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

VEUILLET, E., L. COLLET, and R. DUCLAUX. "STIMULATIONS AUDITIVES CONTROLATÉRALES ET OTO-ÉMISSIONS ACOUSTIQUES PROVOQUÉES : I-CARACTÉRISATION DU STIMULUS." Le Journal de Physique Colloques 51, no. C2 (1990): C2–127—C2–130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/jphyscol:1990230.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Riazi, Mariam, and John A. Ferraro. "Observations on Mastoid versus Ear Canal Recorded Cochlear Microphonic in Newborns and Adults." Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 19, no. 01 (2008): 046–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.19.1.5.

Full text
Abstract:
The cochlear microphonic (CM) may play an important role in the diagnosis of auditory neuropathy (AN) in newborns. However, since the CM tends to mirror the waveform of the acoustic stimulus, conscientious recording methodology must be applied to separate true response from artifact. The difficulty in achieving this separation has limited the clinical usefulness of the CM. In an effort to call attention to the importance of recording protocol when measuring the CM, the present study was designed to optimize CM recordings in humans by investigating the following parameters: (1) secondary minus
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Vaughan, Nancy, Kenneth James, Daniel McDermott, Susan Griest, and Stephen Fausti. "Auditory Brainstem Response Differences in Diabetic and Non-diabetic Veterans." Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 18, no. 10 (2007): 863–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.18.10.5.

Full text
Abstract:
A number of studies have found statistically significant delays in auditory brainstem latencies of patients with diabetes compared with non-diabetic controls. However, the mechanisms ascribed as responsible for the latency differences between diabetic and non-diabetic patients vary among studies, and the latency differences, while significant, are small. In this 5-year prospective study, auditory brainstem response testing was conducted with 416 non-diabetic and 375 diabetic veterans from the Portland Veterans Affairs Medical Center as part of a larger study. Patients with diabetes had signifi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Shaikh, G., R. Ireland, M. McBreen, and R. Ramana. "Audit of a recently introduced stimulus dosing policy in an electroconvulsive therapy clinic." Psychiatric Bulletin 23, no. 9 (1999): 541–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/pb.23.9.541.

Full text
Abstract:
Aims and methodTo audit the clinical practice of seizure threshold estimation and subsequent stimulus dose adjustment in the electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) clinic. Case notes of patients who had ECT over a six-month period were audited. Results were discussed at an audit meeting and guidelines and training modified appropriately prior to the second cycle of the audit.ResultsInitial dose titration was poor in the first period, but improved in the second. The majority of patients were insufficiently stimulated after missed seizures in both periods and stimulus doses were not being reduced follo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Recanzone, Gregg H. "Auditory Influences on Visual Temporal Rate Perception." Journal of Neurophysiology 89, no. 2 (2003): 1078–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00706.2002.

Full text
Abstract:
Visual stimuli are known to influence the perception of auditory stimuli in spatial tasks, giving rise to the ventriloquism effect. These influences can persist in the absence of visual input following a period of exposure to spatially disparate auditory and visual stimuli, a phenomenon termed the ventriloquism aftereffect. It has been speculated that the visual dominance over audition in spatial tasks is due to the superior spatial acuity of vision compared with audition. If that is the case, then the auditory system should dominate visual perception in a manner analogous to the ventriloquism
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Ehrenstein, W. H., J. Lewald, and L. Schlykowa. "Effects of Gaze Direction in Vision and Audition." Perception 26, no. 1_suppl (1997): 137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v970024.

Full text
Abstract:
We asked to what extent the respective gaze direction influences (i) the spatial congruence of perceived direction of auditory and visual cues, and (ii) the discrimination of the direction of target motion. With fixed head position, subjects directed their gaze in various positions and localised auditory targets (band-pass noise, 2 kHz) presented at one of nine positions (straight ahead, or four symmetric positions to the left or right separated by 2.75 deg, respectively). Forced-choice judgements, whether the sound was perceived to the left or right of a visual reference light, show that the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Lizcano-Portilla, Alberto, Luis Mendoza, and Zulmary Nieto-Sánchez. "Procesamiento de señales cerebrales provenientes de estímulos visuales y auditivos utilizando análisis wavelet y redes neuronales artificiales." Revista UIS Ingenierías 19, no. 2 (2020): 119–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.18273/revuin.v19n2-2020013.

Full text
Abstract:
Este artículo presenta el diseño y desarrollo de un prototipo portátil para la adquisición, procesamiento y clasificación de señales EEG con el objetivo de caracterizar estímulos visuales y auditivos, se trabajó con dos pacientes diferentes para la validación de los resultados, se realizó el registro de las señales durante 4 segundos a una frecuencia de 500Hz. Los pacientes fueron expuestos a estímulos visuales y auditivos en diferentes casos, cuya frecuencia de aparición permanecía constante. Para el registro de las señales se diseñó un sistema de adquisición de 4 canales configurables entre
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Cebulla, Mario, Ekkehard Stürzebecher, Claus Elberling, and Jochen Müller. "New Clicklike Stimuli for Hearing Testing." Journal of the American Academy of Audiology 18, no. 09 (2007): 725–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.18.9.2.

Full text
Abstract:
The click stimulus generally used for newborn hearing screening generates a traveling wave along the basilar membrane, which excites each of the frequency bands in the cochlea, one after another. Due to the lack in synchronization of the excitations, the summated response amplitude is low. A repetitive click-like stimulus can be set up in the frequency domain by adding a high number of cosines, the frequency intervals of which comply with the desired stimulus repetition rate. Straight-forward compensation of the cochlear traveling wave delay is possible with a stimulus of this type. As a resul
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Lange, Kathrin, and Brigitte Röder. "Orienting Attention to Points in Time Improves Stimulus Processing Both within and across Modalities." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, no. 5 (2006): 715–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.5.715.

Full text
Abstract:
Spatial attention affects the processing of stimuli of both a task-relevant and a task-irrelevant modality. The present study investigated if similar cross-modal effects exist when attention is oriented to a point in time. Short (600 msec) and long (1200 msec) empty intervals, marked by a tactile onset and an auditory or a tactile offset marker, were presented. In each block, the participants had to attend one interval and one modality. Event-related potentials (ERPs) to auditory and tactile offset markers of attended as compared to unattended intervals were characterized by an enhancement of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Yang, Charlotte L., Noelle R. B. Stiles, Carmel A. Levitan, and Shinsuke Shimojo. "Temporal rate adaptation transfers cross-modally at a subconscious level." Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012): 30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/187847612x646523.

Full text
Abstract:
In an earlier study, we demonstrated that the temporal rate adaptation effect can be transferred from audition to vision and vice versa. However, it was unclear whether this effect was due to a top-down cognitive process, or rather to an earlier calibration process which is stimulus-driven and automatic. We therefore examined the effect of interocular masking of the adapting stimuli on the temporal rate adaptation and its cross-modal transfer from vision to audition (VA). Participants were trained, using feedback, to classify repetitive auditory stimuli presented at a range of frequencies (3.2
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Parker, Scott, and Bruce Schneider. "The stimulus range effect: Evidence for top-down control of sensory intensity in audition." Perception & Psychophysics 56, no. 1 (1994): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03211685.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Bruns, Patrick, and Brigitte Röder. "Tactile Capture of Auditory Localization Is Modulated by Hand Posture." Experimental Psychology 57, no. 4 (2010): 267–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000032.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known that spatial discrepancies between synchronized auditory and visual events can lead to mislocalizations of the auditory stimulus toward the visual stimulus, the so-called ventriloquism effect. Recently, a similar effect of touch on audition has been reported. This study investigated whether this audio-tactile ventriloquism effect depends on hand posture. Participants reported the perceived location of brief auditory stimuli that were presented from left, right, and center locations, either alone or with concurrent tactile stimuli to the fingertips situated at the left and righ
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Wesslein, Ann-Katrin, and Christian Frings. "Identity-Based Crossmodal Negative Priming: Aftereffects of Ignoring in One Sensory Modality on Responding to Another Sensory Modality." Multisensory Research 33, no. 7 (2020): 703–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20201471.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Negative Priming (NP) refers to the phenomenon that responses towards previously ignored stimuli, as compared to new stimuli, are impaired. That is, NP is reflected in the performance on the probe display of a prime–probe sequence. NP is established in vision, audition and touch. In the current study, we presented participants with auditory, visual, and tactile manifestations of the same temporal patterns in order to measure NP across the senses. On each trial, the sensory modality shifted from the prime to the probe. Each prime and probe display consisted of a target and a distractor
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Mäenpää, Pia, Marja Laasonen, Marja-Leena Haapanen, Joonas Pulkkinen, and Veijo Virsu. "Crossmodal Temporal Processing Acuity in Children with Oral Clefts." Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal 45, no. 4 (2008): 393–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1597/07-050.1.

Full text
Abstract:
Objective: We have previously found that, in children with certain oral clefts, the rate of sequential information processing is significantly impaired in vision and tactile somatosensation but not so clearly in audition. Here, we studied crossmodal functions by investigating temporal processing acuity of cleft children with audiovisual, audiotactile, and visuotactile tasks. Participants: Temporal processing acuity was studied in 10-year-old children, 19 with cleft lip with or without cleft palate and 38 with cleft palate or submucous cleft palate. Design: Children estimated whether brief stim
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Mead, Tim P., John N. Drowatzky, and Lisa Hardin-Crosby. "Positive and Negative Stimuli in Relation to Tennis Players' Reaction Time." Perceptual and Motor Skills 90, no. 1 (2000): 236–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2000.90.1.236.

Full text
Abstract:
Research has indicated that negative and positive thoughts may affect sport performance. The purpose of this analogue study was to assess whether negative and positive stimuli influenced tennis performance similar to positive and negative thought. The reaction time (RT) of 40 competitive tennis players was measured during a timed response to a tennis ball rotating in a topspin, sidespin, or backspin direction on the computer screen. Immediately prior to the ball presentation, a phrase (accessory stimulus) was presented visually or aurally. The accessory stimulus provided either positive (e.g.,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Marlatt, Edward. "The Importance of Visual Stimulus as a Component of Audition Stimulus in the Aural/Oral (Re)Habilitation of Children/Clients With Hearing Loss." American Annals of the Deaf 159, no. 4 (2014): 317–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/aad.2014.0027.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Göpfert, Martin C., and Daniel Robert. "The mechanical basis of Drosophila audition." Journal of Experimental Biology 205, no. 9 (2002): 1199–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.205.9.1199.

Full text
Abstract:
SUMMARY In Drosophila melanogaster, antennal hearing organs mediate the detection of conspecific songs. Combining laser Doppler vibrometry, acoustic near-field measurements and anatomical analysis, we have investigated the first steps in Drosophila audition, i.e. the conversion of acoustic energy into mechanical vibrations and the subsequent transmission of vibrations to the auditory receptors in the base of the antenna. Examination of the mechanical responses of the antennal structures established that the distal antennal parts (the funiculus and the arista) together constitute a mechanical e
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Hsieh, P. J., J. T. Colas, and N. Kanwisher. "Spatial pattern of BOLD fMRI activation reveals cross-modal information in auditory cortex." Journal of Neurophysiology 107, no. 12 (2012): 3428–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.01094.2010.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent findings suggest that neural representations in early auditory cortex reflect not only the physical properties of a stimulus, but also high-level, top-down, and even cross-modal information. However, the nature of cross-modal information in auditory cortex remains poorly understood. Here, we used pattern analyses of fMRI data to ask whether early auditory cortex contains information about the visual environment. Our data show that 1) early auditory cortex contained information about a visual stimulus when there was no bottom-up auditory signal, and that 2) no influence of visual stimula
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Broerse, Jack, Peter C. Dodwell, and Walter H. Ehrenstein. "Experiments on the Afterimages of Stimulus Change (Dvořák 1870): A Translation with Commentary." Perception 23, no. 10 (1994): 1135–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p231135.

Full text
Abstract:
In 1870 Dvořák rejected Helmholtz's eye-movement account of motion aftereffects (MAEs) on the grounds that it was inconsistent with previous reports of nonuniform rotation in MAEs induced with Plateau spirals. Subsequent observations with spirals that were modified to induce both expanding and contracting MAEs simultaneously, together with the use of stationary negative afterimages during induction and test, were offered as further counter-examples to the eye-movement hypothesis. Dvořák's conjectures that perception (and misperception) of movement involves a unitary perceptual dimension of sti
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Heiligenberg, W. "Coding and processing of electrosensory information in gymnotiform fish." Journal of Experimental Biology 146, no. 1 (1989): 255–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.146.1.255.

Full text
Abstract:
Studies of the electrosensory system of gymnotiform fish have revealed principles of neuronal coding and processing of information which also characterize more advanced systems, such as vision and audition in higher vertebrates. 1. Animals may have different classes of receptors adapted to code different variables within a given modality, and the separation of their central projections provides the basis for independent initial processing of these variables by higher-order neurones. 2. These separate pathways, however, eventually converge at the level of still higher-order neurones which are a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Fucci, Donald, Daniel Harris, Linda Petrosino, and Elizabeth Randolph-Tyler. "Auditory Psychophysical Scaling Exposure Effects: Magnitude Estimation and Cross-Modal Matching." Perceptual and Motor Skills 66, no. 2 (1988): 643–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1988.66.2.643.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of the present study was to investigate possible effects of exposure upon suprathreshold psychological responses when auditory magnitude estimation and cross-modal matching with audition as the standard are conducted within the same experiment. Four groups of 10 subjects each whose over-all age range was 18 to 23 yr. were employed. During the cross-modal marching task the Groups 1 and 2 subjects adjusted a vibrotactile stimulus presented to the dorsal surface of the tongue and the Groups 3 and 4 subjects adjusted a vibrotactile stimulus presented to the thenar eminence of the right
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Dykstra, Andrew R., and Alexander Gutschalk. "Does the mismatch negativity operate on a consciously accessible memory trace?" Science Advances 1, no. 10 (2015): e1500677. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1500677.

Full text
Abstract:
The extent to which the contents of short-term memory are consciously accessible is a fundamental question of cognitive science. In audition, short-term memory is often studied via the mismatch negativity (MMN), a change-related component of the auditory evoked response that is elicited by violations of otherwise regular stimulus sequences. The prevailing functional view of the MMN is that it operates on preattentive and even preconscious stimulus representations. We directly examined the preconscious notion of the MMN using informational masking and magnetoencephalography. Spectrally isolated
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Thabet, Imen. "Auditor skills, traits and knowledge evidence from Tunisia." International Journal of Accounting and Economics Studies 5, no. 1 (2016): 7. http://dx.doi.org/10.14419/ijaes.v5i1.6995.

Full text
Abstract:
Several researches have sought an understanding of personal and psychological characteristics contributing to auditor expertise in decision making. The motivation for this study is issued from the need to improve understanding of auditor skills and characteristics in the step of the expression of professional judgment.This paper aims to looking for and to understand auditors’ skills, traits and knowledge contributing to business risk assessment ability. A survey questionnaire is used as a stimulus to gather data from practicing auditors listed on the Roll of the Tunisian Chartered Ac-countants
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Näätänen, Risto. "The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13, no. 2 (1990): 201–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00078407.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis article examines the role of attention and automaticity in auditory processing as revealed by event-related potential (ERP) research. An ERP component called the mismatch negativity, generated by the brain's automatic response to changes in repetitive auditory input, reveals that physical features of auditory stimuli are fully processed whether or not they are attended. It also suggests that there exist precise neuronal representations of the physical features of recent auditory stimuli, perhaps the traces underlying acoustic sensory (“echoic”) memory. A mechanism of passive atten
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Ju, Angela, Emily Orchard-Mills, Erik van der Burg, and David Alais. "Rapid Audiovisual Temporal Recalibration Generalises Across Spatial Location." Multisensory Research 32, no. 3 (2019): 215–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20191176.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Recent exposure to asynchronous multisensory signals has been shown to shift perceived timing between the sensory modalities, a phenomenon known as ‘temporal recalibration’. Recently, Van der Burg et al. (2013, J Neurosci, 33, pp. 14633–14637) reported results showing that recalibration to asynchronous audiovisual events can happen extremely rapidly. In an extended series of variously asynchronous trials, simultaneity judgements were analysed based on the modality order in the preceding trial and showed that shifts in the point of subjective synchrony occurred almost instantaneously,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Nerosti, Nerosti. "COREOGRAPHY IN MALAYSIA IN MULTICULTURAL CONCEPT: CASE STUDY PROGRAM MINOR ARTS DANCE UPSI." Komposisi: Jurnal Pendidikan Bahasa, Sastra, dan Seni 15, no. 2 (2014): 197. http://dx.doi.org/10.24036/komposisi.v15i2.7498.

Full text
Abstract:
KOREOGRAFI DI MALAYSIA DALAM KONSEP MULTIKULTURAL: KAJIAN KES PROGRAM MINOR SENI TARI UPSIAbstractThis writing is the result of my research entitled “Analisis Deskriptif Koreografi Pelajar Minor Seni Tari UPSI: Kajian Kes Tiga Produksi Tari 2008 – 2011”. The aim of the research is to answer the question: To what extend the dance work by Minor Dance Art students which was presented in Art Dance Production fulfills the coreography elements, by using descriptive analysis method from primary and secondary data. By using qualitative approach which involved library research, observation, interview a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Leon, Paula Sanz, Ivo Vanzetta, Guillaume S. Masson, and Laurent U. Perrinet. "Motion clouds: model-based stimulus synthesis of natural-like random textures for the study of motion perception." Journal of Neurophysiology 107, no. 11 (2012): 3217–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00737.2011.

Full text
Abstract:
Choosing an appropriate set of stimuli is essential to characterize the response of a sensory system to a particular functional dimension, such as the eye movement following the motion of a visual scene. Here, we describe a framework to generate random texture movies with controlled information content, i.e., Motion Clouds. These stimuli are defined using a generative model that is based on controlled experimental parametrization. We show that Motion Clouds correspond to dense mixing of localized moving gratings with random positions. Their global envelope is similar to natural-like stimulatio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Yousaf, Farida, Martin Lee, and Jose King. "A re-audit of ECT training and practice." Psychiatric Bulletin 23, no. 7 (1999): 419–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/pb.23.7.419.

Full text
Abstract:
Aims and methodsA re-audit of the current practice of electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) was carried out over a period of five months using the new guidelines produced by the Royal College of Psychiatrists. The areas audited included obtaining consent from the patients, information recording, training and supervision of the junior doctors and the practical aspects of ECT administration, including dose titration.ResultsAn improvement in the training and supervision of junior doctors was seen since the last audit but shortcomings were identified in areas of pre- and post-ECT preparation, informatio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Fournier, Guillaume. "Perception des intervalles harmoniques et apprentissage perceptuel." Revue musicale OICRM 4, no. 1 (2017): 17–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.7202/1040298ar.

Full text
Abstract:
Cet article élabore une nouvelle approche pédagogique pour étudier les intervalles harmoniques aux niveaux collégial et universitaire. Sont d’abord passés en revue les principaux mécanismes responsables de la perception des intervalles harmoniques, notamment la perception des hauteurs sonores, la capacité à discriminer les notes jouées simultanément, la perception des battements et l’harmonicité. En s’appuyant sur les mécanismes d’apprentissage perceptuel de Goldstone et Byrge (2015), différentes stratégies sont ensuite discutées pour isoler, stimuler et renforcer la perception des principaux
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

KRAMER, BERND, and IVO KAUNZINGER. "Electrosensory Frequency and Intensity Discrimination in the Wave-Type Electric Fish Eigenmannia." Journal of Experimental Biology 161, no. 1 (1991): 43–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.161.1.43.

Full text
Abstract:
Eigenmannia's frequency and intensity discrimination thresholds were determined for a range of seven frequencies (50–1700Hz), centered on a fish's individual discharge frequency, using a conditioned go/no-go paradigm. The threshold criterion was ‘50% correct choices’ (of the rewarded stimulus, S+, over the unrewarded stimulus, S-); this was validated by testing for statistically significantly shorter response latencies for the S+ compared with the S- stimulus. The stimuli consisted of sine wave bursts presented for up to 20 s, repeated at 2 s−1 (rise and fall times of an individual burst, 50 m
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Hoshino, Osamu. "Neuronal Bases of Perceptual Learning Revealed by a Synaptic Balance Scheme." Neural Computation 16, no. 3 (2004): 563–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976604772744910.

Full text
Abstract:
Our ability to perceive external sensory stimuli improves as we experience the same stimulus repeatedly. This perceptual enhancement, called perceptual learning, has been demonstrated for various sensory systems, such as vision, audition, and somatosensation. I investigated the contribution of lateral excitatory and inhibitory synaptic balance to perceptual learning. I constructed a simple associative neural network model in which sensory features were expressed by the activities of specific cell assemblies. Each neuron is sensitive to a specific sensory feature, and the neurons belonging to t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Schwartz, Jean-Luc, Nicolas Grimault, Jean-Michel Hupé, Brian C. J. Moore, and Daniel Pressnitzer. "Multistability in perception: binding sensory modalities, an overview." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367, no. 1591 (2012): 896–905. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0254.

Full text
Abstract:
This special issue presents research concerning multistable perception in different sensory modalities. Multistability occurs when a single physical stimulus produces alternations between different subjective percepts. Multistability was first described for vision, where it occurs, for example, when different stimuli are presented to the two eyes or for certain ambiguous figures. It has since been described for other sensory modalities, including audition, touch and olfaction. The key features of multistability are: (i) stimuli have more than one plausible perceptual organization; (ii) these o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Higgs, Dennis M., and Lee A. Fuiman. "Associations between Behavioural Ontogeny and Habitat change in Clupeoid Larvae." Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 78, no. 4 (1998): 1281–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0025315400044490.

Full text
Abstract:
The ontogeny of evasive behaviours was examined in Atlantic menhaden (Brevoortia tyrannus, Clupeidae) and bay anchovy (Anchoa mitchilli, Engraulidae), with supplemental data for scaled sardine (Harengula jaguana, Clupeidae). Predicted relationships between ecology and behavioural ontogeny were tested using the different patterns of inshore vs offshore habitat utilization displayed by these species. In two of the species, menhaden and anchovy, the roles of vision, mechanoreception, and audition in evasive behaviours were examined ontogenetically and compared to the morphological development of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Gentsch, Kornelia, Ursula Beermann, Lingdan Wu, Stéphanie Trznadel, and Klaus R. Scherer. "Temporal Unfolding of Micro-valences in Facial Expression Evoked by Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Stimuli." Affective Science 1, no. 4 (2020): 208–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s42761-020-00020-y.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractAppraisal theories suggest that valence appraisal should be differentiated into micro-valences, such as intrinsic pleasantness and goal-/need-related appraisals. In contrast to a macro-valence approach, this dissociation explains, among other things, the emergence of mixed or blended emotions. Here, we extend earlier research that showed that these valence types can be empirically dissociated. We examine the timing and the response patterns of these two micro-valences via measuring facial muscle activity changes (electromyography, EMG) over the brow and the cheek regions. In addition,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Renzi, Chiara, Patrick Bruns, Kirstin-Friederike Heise, et al. "Spatial Remapping in the Audio-tactile Ventriloquism Effect: A TMS Investigation on the Role of the Ventral Intraparietal Area." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, no. 5 (2013): 790–801. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00362.

Full text
Abstract:
Previous studies have suggested that the putative human homologue of the ventral intraparietal area (hVIP) is crucially involved in the remapping of tactile information into external spatial coordinates and in the realignment of tactile and visual maps. It is unclear, however, whether hVIP is critical for the remapping process during audio-tactile cross-modal spatial interactions. The audio-tactile ventriloquism effect, where the perceived location of a sound is shifted toward the location of a synchronous but spatially disparate tactile stimulus, was used to probe spatial interactions in audi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Rau, Pei-Luen Patrick, Jian Zheng, Lijun Wang, Jingyu Zhao, and Dangxiao Wang. "Haptic and Auditory–Haptic Attentional Blink in Spatial and Object-Based Tasks." Multisensory Research 33, no. 3 (2020): 295–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20191483.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Dual-task performance depends on both modalities (e.g., vision, audition, haptics) and task types (spatial or object-based), and the order by which different task types are organized. Previous studies on haptic and especially auditory–haptic attentional blink (AB) are scarce, and the effect of task types and their order have not been fully explored. In this study, 96 participants, divided into four groups of task type combinations, identified auditory or haptic Target 1 (T1) and haptic Target 2 (T2) in rapid series of sounds and forces. We observed a haptic AB (i.e., the accuracy of i
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Bertonati, Giorgia, Maria Bianca Amadeo, Claudio Campus, and Monica Gori. "Auditory speed processing in sighted and blind individuals." PLOS ONE 16, no. 9 (2021): e0257676. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257676.

Full text
Abstract:
Multisensory experience is crucial for developing a coherent perception of the world. In this context, vision and audition are essential tools to scaffold spatial and temporal representations, respectively. Since speed encompasses both space and time, investigating this dimension in blindness allows deepening the relationship between sensory modalities and the two representation domains. In the present study, we hypothesized that visual deprivation influences the use of spatial and temporal cues underlying acoustic speed perception. To this end, ten early blind and ten blindfolded sighted part
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Lockhead, Gregory R. "Psychophysical scaling: Judgments of attributes or objects?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15, no. 3 (1992): 543–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00069934.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractPsychophysical scaling models of the form R = f(I), with R the response and I some intensity of an attribute, all assume that people judge the amounts of an attribute. With simple biases excepted, most also assume that judgments are independent of space, time, and features of the situation other than the one being judged. Many data support these ideas: Magnitude estimations of brightness (R) increase with luminance (I). Nevertheless, I argue that the general model is wrong. The stabilized retinal image literature shows that nothing is seen if light does not change over time. The classi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Schäfer, Johannes, André Beauducel, and Burkhard Brocke. "Zu Posners Theorie der Aufmerksamkeit: rechtshemisphärischer Verarbeitungsvorteil bei Daueraufmerksamkeit." Experimental Psychology 47, no. 1 (2000): 58–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1026//0949-3964.47.1.58.

Full text
Abstract:
Zusammenfassung. Posners Aufmerksamkeitstheorie umfasst die Entdeckung von Zielreizen, (visuelle) Orientierung und Daueraufmerksamkeit. Letztere soll überwiegend rechtslateralisierte Hirnstrukturen nutzen. Whitehead fand unter Daueraufmerksamkeit kürzere Reaktionszeiten bei rechshemisphärischer Präsentation. Posner postuliert, dass ein zusätzlicher auditiver Stimulus über eine automatische Veränderung des Aktivierungsniveaus eine Interaktion mit dieser Hemisphärenasymmetrie bewirkt. Auch diese Dreifachinteraktion konnte von Whitehead gezeigt werden. Ungeklärt bleibt dabei, wie die linke Hemisp
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Freeman, Marc E., Béla Kanyicska, Anna Lerant, and György Nagy. "Prolactin: Structure, Function, and Regulation of Secretion." Physiological Reviews 80, no. 4 (2000): 1523–631. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/physrev.2000.80.4.1523.

Full text
Abstract:
Prolactin is a protein hormone of the anterior pituitary gland that was originally named for its ability to promote lactation in response to the suckling stimulus of hungry young mammals. We now know that prolactin is not as simple as originally described. Indeed, chemically, prolactin appears in a multiplicity of posttranslational forms ranging from size variants to chemical modifications such as phosphorylation or glycosylation. It is not only synthesized in the pituitary gland, as originally described, but also within the central nervous system, the immune system, the uterus and its associa
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!