Academic literature on the topic 'Autism, fiction'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Autism, fiction.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Autism, fiction"
Anju Sosan George. "Shifting Autism Popular Fiction: Representing Asperger’s Syndrome in Select Works of Mark Haddon, Jodi Picoult and Steig Larsson." Creative Saplings 2, no. 09 (December 26, 2023): 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.56062/gtrs.2023.2.09.460.
Full textDavidson, Meghan M., and Susan Ellis Weismer. "A preliminary investigation of parent-reported fiction versus non-fiction book preferences of school-age children with autism spectrum disorder." Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 3 (January 2018): 239694151880610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2396941518806109.
Full textBates, Gordon. "Autism in fiction and autobiography." Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 16, no. 1 (January 2010): 47–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/apt.bp.108.005660.
Full textGaliatsatos, Polymnia, Adrian Gologan, and Esther Lamoureux. "Autistic Enterocolitis: Fact or Fiction?" Canadian Journal of Gastroenterology 23, no. 2 (2009): 95–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2009/394317.
Full textArmstrong, Rebecca M., Jessica Paynter, and Marleen F. Westerveld. "Fiction or non-fiction: Parent-reported book preferences of their preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder." Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 4 (January 2019): 239694151989673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2396941519896736.
Full textIsakova, S. V., and V. N. Feofanov. "The Theme of Autism Spectrum Disorders in Fiction." Autism and Developmental Disorders 13, no. 4 (2015): 43–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.17759/autdd.2015130406.
Full textTanguay, Peter E. "The Science and Fiction of Autism." Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69, no. 9 (September 30, 2008): 1503–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.4088/jcp.v69n0920b.
Full textMcClure, Iain. "The Science and Fiction of Autism." BMJ 333, no. 7560 (July 20, 2006): 205.1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.333.7560.205.
Full textBerney, Tom. "The Science and Fiction of Autism." Child and Adolescent Mental Health 12, no. 2 (May 2007): 102–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-3588.2007.00450_11.x.
Full textKurowicka, Anna. "“Aliens” Speaking Out: Science Fiction by Autistic Authors." Przegląd Kulturoznawczy, no. 3 (45) (2020): 261–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.4467/20843860pk.20.026.12586.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Autism, fiction"
Leung, Ching-man, and 梁靜雯. "Autism: narrative and representation in postmodern fiction." Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2012. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B48334686.
Full textpublished_or_final_version
Literary and Cultural Studies
Master
Master of Arts
Ruby, Cameron H. "Roots and Leaves: A Story for High-Functioning Autistic Youth." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2013. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/cmc_theses/791.
Full textGarbutt, Ian. "Asperger's syndrome and fiction : autistic worlds and those who build them." Thesis, University of Stirling, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/1893/26133.
Full textSveningsson, Sofia. "Karaktärer, känslor och konversationer är ingenting för mig : En kvalitativ studie om hur fyra lärare behandlar skönlitterära texter för elever inom autismspektrumstillstånd." Thesis, Jönköping University, Högskolan för lärande och kommunikation, 2021. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:hj:diva-53901.
Full textStudents with functional variation have cognitive impairments that can make it difficult to understand a textual content in a fiction genre. Students with functional variation happens to be good at decoding texts, but to be able to explain the content, understanding between the lines or connect the text to their own perspective is something that is complicated for a student with functional variation. The purpose of the study is thus to increase knowledge about how some teachers work with fictional texts for students in autism spectrum disorders. The questions address how teachers work with different strategies and methods around fictional texts, how teachers adapt fictional texts both in teaching and in connection with reading lessons and finally what obstacles and opportunities reading lessons in the genre of fictional texts can contribute to a student within autism spectrum disorders. To fulfil the aim and answer the research questions, the qualitative research method through semi-structured interviews has been applied through semi-structured interviews. In order to be able to analyze the empirical data, a behavioristic perspective has been applied. The behavioristic perspective emphasizes individualized teaching, good teaching materials and strategies of repetition as a way of working. The result remained that the teachers used a lot of visual materials, book talks and reading fixes as strategies. The teachers also supported the student with autism spectrum disorder by using fiction texts of the student's interest with one-on-one teaching. All teachers also emphasize the importance of cooperating with guardians, as students within autism spectrum disorder have to receive a good support and with the same tasks both at home and at school. Finally, the teachers reported that students within autism spectrum disorder have different individual needs, thus it is important to have a more in-depth knowledge of autism spectrum disorder for improvement work in school activities.
Grant, Bernard. "All Hours." University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1617105424447492.
Full textTatelman, Anna. "Seismic Communication." ScholarWorks@UNO, 2019. https://scholarworks.uno.edu/td/2707.
Full textTrice, Natalie Collins. "Reading Autistic Experience." Digital Archive @ GSU, 2008. http://digitalarchive.gsu.edu/english_diss/27.
Full textDornelas, Aline Bisotti. "Interação fictiva como estratégia comunicativa de crianças ecolálicas com transtorno do espectro autista." Universidade Federal de Juiz de Fora (UFJF), 2018. https://repositorio.ufjf.br/jspui/handle/ufjf/6652.
Full textApproved for entry into archive by Adriana Oliveira (adriana.oliveira@ufjf.edu.br) on 2018-04-11T11:16:11Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 alinebisottidornelas.pdf: 28770579 bytes, checksum: a3c470619bd879d5ded475ef852a70b9 (MD5)
Made available in DSpace on 2018-04-11T11:16:12Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 alinebisottidornelas.pdf: 28770579 bytes, checksum: a3c470619bd879d5ded475ef852a70b9 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2018-03-09
CAPES - Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior
Indivíduos com Transtorno do Espectro Autista apresentam desempenho deficiente em atividades que demandam atenção conjunta, imitação e leitura de intenções, o que afeta principalmente seu comportamento social e comunicação. A ecolalia, repetição das palavras exatas de um discurso anterior (KANNER, 1943), está relacionada a tal condição cognitiva (CARPENTER; TOMASELLO, 2000). A fala ecolálica de indivíduos com autismo possui, em sua maioria, função comunicativa (SCHULER, 1979; PRIZANT; RYDELL, 1984; PRIZANT; DUCHAN, 1987; RYDELL; MIRENDA, 1991; FERNANDES, 2003; DOBBINSON; PERKINS; BOUCHER, 2003; STERPONI; SHANKEY, 2014). Entendemos as produções ecolálicas funcionais como tipos de interação fictiva, que consiste na utilização de reportações discursivas, ou do padrão de interação, como estratégias comunicativas. A interação fictiva implica o uso de um domínio cognitivo baseado em interações face a face aplicado a outras funções no processo comunicativo, o frame de conversa, diferentes da meramente reportativa. Essas construções têm sido identificadas em vários gêneros discursivos, em diferentes línguas e em casos de afasia de Broca (PASCUAL, 2006; 2014; PASCUAL; SANDLER, 2016). O presente trabalho tem como objetivos: mapear as ocorrências de interação fictiva na fala ecolálica, sua tipologia e desenvolvimento nos casos de crianças com Transtorno do Espectro Autista; comparar os resultados com dois grupos controle, o primeiro correspondendo idade cognitiva com as crianças com TEA e o segundo, idades cronológicas. Para isso, foram realizados dois estudos: um estudo empírico, com a gravação audiovisual interações semiespontâneas entre cinco crianças autistas (quatro meninos com TEA severo e uma menina com TEA moderado) e suas terapeutas em sessões semanais durante um mês, totalizando quatro sessões para cada criança. Com base nos achados do primeiro estudo, foram elaboradas duas tarefas para elicitação de dados, também aplicadas nas mesmas crianças com TEA pelas terapeutas. As crianças dos grupos controle realizaram interações semelhantes no primeiro estudo e as mesmas atividades no segundo estudo, com suas mães. Observou-se que as crianças com autismo utilizaram não só ecolalias com repetição ipsis litteris do discurso anterior, mas também ecolalias mitigadas, em que houve acréscimo de palavras no enunciado ecolálico para que este se adequasse ao contexto; também houve produção de paráfrases e até mesmo algumas ocorrências mais criativas utilizando padrão pergunta-resposta. Essas construções eram utilizadas para acesso ao léxico, sanar dificuldade de estruturação gramatical e referenciar personagens ou pessoas. As crianças do grupo controle 1 (de dois a quatro anos de idade) também utilizaram construções de interação fictiva com repetição de discurso anterior quando se deparavam com dificuldades gramaticais e discursivas. As crianças do grupo controle 2 (de seis a doze anos), utilizaram as construções de interação fictiva de modo mais criativo, com poucas ocorrências de repetição, e como opção discursiva, em vez de estratégia adaptativa. Os resultados sugerem que as construções de interação fictiva foram mais eficazes e mais frequentes que outras estratégias no grupo de crianças com TEA. A utilização do frame de conversa parece ser, então, de grande importância para que esse grupo seja capaz de se engajar no discurso e buscar estratégias comunicativas com seus interlocutores.
Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorder have poor performance in activities that demand joint attention, imitation and intention reading, which mainly affects their social behavior and communication. Echolalia, the repetition of the exact words from a prior discourse (KANNER, 1943), is related to such cognitive condition (CARPENTER; TOMASELLO, 2000). The echolalic speech of individuals with autism has, in general, a communicative function (SCHULER, 1979; PRIZANT; RYDELL, 1984; PRIZANT; DUCHAN, 1987; RYDELL; MIRENDA, 1991; FERNANDES, 2003; DOBBINSON; PERKINS; BOUCHER, 2003; STERPONI; SHANKEY, 2014). We understand functional echolalic productions as fictive interaction constructions, which consists in the use of discursive reportations, or the pattern of interaction, as communicative strategies. The fictive interaction implies the use of a cognitive domain based on face-to-face interactions applied to other functions in the communicative process, the conversation frame, differently from the merely reportive one. These constructions have been identified in several discursive genres, in different languages and in cases of Broca's aphasia (PASCUAL, 2006; 2014; PASCUAL; SANDLER, 2016). The present work has as objectives: mapping the occurrences of fictive interaction in the echolalic speech, its typology and development in the cases of children with Autisic Spectrum Disorder; compare the results with two control groups, the first corresponding cognitive ages with children with ASD and the second, chronological ages. Two studies were carried out: an empirical study, with audiovisual recording of semi-spontaneous interactions between five autistic children (four boys with severe ASD and one girl with moderate ASD) and their therapists in weekly sessions during one month, totaling four sessions for each child. Based on the findings of the first study, two data elicitation tasks were also developed, applied in the same children with ASD by the therapists. The children in the control groups performed similar interactions in the first study and the same activities in the second study with their mothers. It was observed that children with autism used not only ipsis literal repetitions of the previous discourse, but also mitigated echolalias, in which there was an addition of words in the echolaliclic statement so that it adapted to the context; there was also production of paraphrases and even some more creative occurrences using question-answer pattern. These constructions were used to access the lexicon, to solve difficult grammatical structuring and to refer to characters or people. Children in the control group 1 (two to four years old) also used fictional interaction constructions with repetition of previous discourse when faced with grammatical and discursive difficulties. Children in the control group 2 (six to twelve years old) used fictive interaction constructions in a more creative way, with few occurrences of repetition, and as a discursive option rather than an adaptive strategy. The results suggest that fictive interaction constructions were more effective and more frequent than other strategies in the group of children with ASD. The use of the conversation frame seems to be of great importance for this group to be able to engage in discourse and come up with communicative strategies with its interlocutors.
Prado, Daniel Nicory do. "No mundo dos autos: uma teoria da narrativa judicial." Faculdade de Direito, 2018. http://repositorio.ufba.br/ri/handle/ri/25031.
Full textApproved for entry into archive by Ana Valéria de Jesus Moura (anavaleria_131@hotmail.com) on 2018-01-16T19:02:03Z (GMT) No. of bitstreams: 1 DANIEL NICORY DO PRADO.pdf: 1026729 bytes, checksum: 76688c9b2086ace57e533faf769e73cc (MD5)
Made available in DSpace on 2018-01-16T19:02:03Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 DANIEL NICORY DO PRADO.pdf: 1026729 bytes, checksum: 76688c9b2086ace57e533faf769e73cc (MD5)
O presente trabalho teve o objetivo de discutir a constituição do universo e a definição dos critérios de verdade da narrativa judicial. Adotando como marcos teórico filosofia aristotélica e as decorrentes reflexões sobre tempo e narrativa de Paul Ricouer e sobre ação comunicativa de Jürgen Habermas, iniciou-se uma revisão das teorias da ação, de base teleológica, da filosofia, as suas consequências para a construção da teoria do fato na ciência jurídica, e a passagem da ação à narração por meio da teoria dos atos de fala. Chegando à questão da narrativa, analisaram-se, a partir de Paul Ricouer, suas principais modalidades, a narrativa histórica e a ficção, sendo que a primeira apresenta uma pretensão de correspondência, verificada pelas provas fornecidas pelo historiador, e a segunda uma pretensão de credibilidade, verificada pela coerência narrativa. À primeira vista, a narrativa judicial seria enquadrada como forma de narrativa histórica, em face da evidente relação entre verdade-correspondência e justiça. No entanto, existem vários indicadores no sistema e na prática jurídica de que nem sempre se alcança a correspondência, mesmo quando não se trata de um problema de insuficiência cognitiva, mas de proibição jurídica de acesso aos elementos disponíveis. Isto é justificado, em parte, porque o processo judicial teria outros valores a preservar além da verdade, mas poderia também ser dito o contrário, que, nesses casos, de renúncia à correspondência, outro tipo de pretensão de validade (como a correção normativa ou o consenso) prevalece sobre a busca da correspondência. Portanto, pode-se concluir que a narrativa judicial é ficcional, mas, como a correspondência não pode ser completamente abandonada, trata-se de ficção baseada em fatos reais. A estrutura da narrativa judicial é binária, dividida em história do processo, narrada no modo mimético alto, dentro da qual se revela gradualmente a história do conflito, narrada nos modos mimético baixo ou irônico, em que o desfecho da primeira é simultaneamente o da segunda. Quanto ao narrador, a pluralidade de pontos de vista narrativos é a principal característica, e decorre do próprio sistema (princípio do contraditório) destinando-se a gerar uma incerteza provisória quanto à verdade, a ser superada com a decisão definitiva. O reconhecimento de que cada processo judicial é um pequeno universo ficcional tem por consequência a adoção da coerência, e não da correspondência, como critério de verdPoade. Por ser ficção baseada em fatos reais, a correspondência não é completamente abandonada, já que a coerência pode ser enganosa. A coerência externa pode gerar equívocos quando remete a uma narrativa familiar, no âmbito da tradição, mas que não reflete corretamente o caso em discussão; a coerência interna pode gerar equívocos quando há falso consenso entre as partes, ou não oposição deliberada de uma delas; quando uma narrativa é coerente por um critério e incoerente por outro, a correção do equívoco deve partir da verificação desta divergência até a decisão por um dos critérios; se uma narrativa é duplamente coerente, mas falsa, ou duplamente incoerente, mas verdadeira, os recursos narrativos disponíveis são insuficientes para a correção do problema.
This work intended to discuss how the universe of judicial narrative is constructed and which are its criteria of truth. Adopting Aristotle’s philosophy, and its further developments of the relations between time and narrative, by Paul Ricouer, and of communicative action, by Jürgen Habermas, as the main theoretical framework, it starts reviewing the teleological concept of action, its many philosophical theories, their consequences to the construction of fact as a legal concept, and then its further passage to the narrative theory using the theory of speech acts as a theoretical transition. Regarding narrative theory, Paul Ricouer’s work was used to distinguish the main narrative forms (historical and fictional), according to whom the first has a correspondence claim, verified by the evidence provided by the historian, while the second has a credibility claim, verified by its narrative coherence. At first, it seems evident that judicial narrative should be a form of historical narrative, because of the undeniable relationship between truth (as correspondence) and justice. Despite that, there are many indications, in legal practice, that correspondence is not always reached, not only because of a cognitive defect, but also because of a legal prohibition to access and evaluate the available data. It is justified, in part, by the thought that the Judicial System has other goals, besides truth-finding, but that can be phrased in a different way: when it is a case of abandonment of correspondence, other types of validity-claims (like normative correction or consensus) prevail over its search. So, it can be said that judicial narrative is fictional, but, since correspondence can’t be completely abandoned, it is fiction based on true facts. Judicial narrative’s structure is binary, divided in the trial story, told in the high mimetic mode, in which the conflict story, told in the low mimetic or the ironic mode, is gradually revealed, and the ending of the first is also the ending of the second. Regarding the narrators, the plurality of points of view is the most important aspect, which is a consequence of the system itself and destined to generate a provisional uncertainty about the truth, that the final decision will overcome. Recognizing each case file as a fictional universe means that coherence, and not correspondence, has to be adopted as the criterion of truth. On the other hand, since it is a true facts based fiction, correspondence can’t be completely abandoned, because coherence can be misleading. External coherence can be misleading when it evokes a familiar narrative, ingrained in the tradition, but that doesn’t reflect correctly the particular case. Internal coherence can be misleading when there is a false consensus between the parties, or a false confession by one of them; when a narrative is coherent by one measure and incoherent by another, the correction can come from a verification of this divergence and a decision for one of the criteria, but if a narrative is doubly coherent, but false, or doubly incoherent, but true, the narrative resources are insufficient to correct the problem.
Raine, Danuta Electra. "Getting here." Thesis, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/1959.13/1310490.
Full textIn January, 2009, as part of my research for this award, I discovered my mother had been born in a Nazi concentration camp for the extermination of Slavic infants. The following Palm Sunday, I was the first descendant of a Polish infant survivor to have visited the site of the Frauen Entbindungslager, Birth and Abortion Camp, in Waltrop, Recklinghausen, North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany. I shared communion with a predominantly octogenarian congregation that been young men and women in 1943, some of them the residents of this German Catholic town when it enforced the fates of the pregnant Slav workers. Nearly seventy years after my mother’s escape, I became the custodian of a story I should never have been born to tell. Although more a piece of literary fiction than an autobiographical novel, >>The Glass Mountain<< engages with family stories to explore the depth, transference and healing of trauma across four generations as it weaves between the contemporary Australian lives of Kaz and her autistic 17 year old son, Jason, and the experiences of Zuitka and her infant daughter, Julka, in Germany during the last years of WWII. In 2011, Christophe Laue from the Herford Archive, Herford, North Rhine-Westphalia emailed Nazi documents relating to my mother, as well as an historical book and a museum program in which she is named. Scholars have asked, “What happened to Danuta Anita?” The exegesis, >>The Legacy of Danuta Anita<<, responds to this while exploring practice led research in creative projects involving intergenerational trauma and migration. It engages with the researcher as subject, authorial authenticity and performativity, the science and literature of trauma and intergenerational (transgenerational) trauma, the unreliability of memory in researching trauma narratives, the origins and ongoing influences of eugenics, infanticide and genocide, and the complexities of representing trauma and autism in literature.
Books on the topic "Autism, fiction"
Carlson, Richard W. My brother has autism =: Mi hermano tiene autismo. [Charleston, SC: CreateSpace], 2016.
Find full textill, Wroth Dean, ed. Chef Philip has autism. Hollidaysburg, Pa: Jason & Nordic Publishers, Inc., 2014.
Find full textill, Harrington David 1964, ed. Since we're friends: An autism picture book. New York: Skyhorse Pub., 2012.
Find full textill, Rivard Rebecca, ed. Ben has autism, Ben is awesome. Hollidaysburg, Penn: Jason and Nordic Publishers, 2011.
Find full textLears, Laurie. Ian's walk: A story about autism. Morton Grove, IL: Albert Whitman, 1998.
Find full textill, Li Ellen, ed. A girl like Tilly: Growing up with autism. London: Jessica Kingsley Limited, 2017.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Autism, fiction"
Reid, Robin Anne. "“I Came for the ‘Pew-Pew Space Battles’; I Stayed for the Autism”." In The Routledge Companion to Gender and Science Fiction, 95–101. London: Routledge, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003082934-15.
Full textBottema-Beutel, Kristen, Laura Sterponi, and Rebecca Louick. "Animating Characters and Experiencing Selves: A Look at Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder Constructing Fictional Storyboards with Typically Developing Peers." In A Practical Guide to Social Interaction Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 325–49. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-59236-1_13.
Full text"Characteristics of Autism." In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 26–48. Harvard University Press, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvk12rs8.5.
Full text"What Causes Autism?" In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 75–108. Harvard University Press, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvk12rs8.7.
Full text"4. What Causes Autism?" In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 75–108. Harvard University Press, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.4159/9780674043299-006.
Full text"2. Characteristics of Autism." In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 26–48. Harvard University Press, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.4159/9780674043299-004.
Full text"Critical Evaluation of Issues in Autism." In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 6–25. Harvard University Press, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvk12rs8.4.
Full text"Are There Core Deficits in Autism?" In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 109–31. Harvard University Press, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvk12rs8.8.
Full textWhitehead, Anne. "Empathy and Mind." In Medicine and Empathy in Contemporary British Fiction, 25–58. Edinburgh University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/edinburgh/9780748686186.003.0002.
Full text"5. Are There Core Deficits in Autism?" In The Science and Fiction of Autism, 109–31. Harvard University Press, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.4159/9780674043299-007.
Full text