Academic literature on the topic 'Behavioral ontogeny'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Behavioral ontogeny.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Behavioral ontogeny"
Ujike, Hiroshi, Kazuo Tsuchida, Kazufumi Akiyama, Yutaka Fujiwara, and Shigetoshi Kuroda. "Ontogeny of behavioral sensitization to cocaine." Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 50, no. 4 (April 1995): 613–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0091-3057(94)00352-1.
Full textKroodsma, Donald E. "Behavioral ontogeny research: No pain, no gain?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11, no. 4 (December 1988): 639–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00053838.
Full textHorback, K. M., and T. D. Parsons. "Ontogeny of behavioral traits in commercial sows." Animal 12, no. 11 (2018): 2365–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1751731118000149.
Full textScalzo, F. "The ontogeny of behavioral sensitization to phencyclidine." Neurotoxicology and Teratology 14, no. 1 (February 1992): 7–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0892-0362(92)90023-4.
Full textYu, Yibo, Yun Wang, Liang Zhong, Hongjuan Zhu, and Jiapeng Qu. "Variations in Behavioral and Physiological Traits in Yearling Tibetan Sheep (Ovis aries)." Animals 11, no. 6 (June 4, 2021): 1676. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ani11061676.
Full textRodriguez, M., and D. Afonso. "Ontogeny of T-maze behavioral lateralization in rats." Physiology & Behavior 54, no. 1 (July 1993): 91–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(93)90048-k.
Full textHusband, Alan J., and Maree Gleeson. "Ontogeny of Mucosal Immunity—Environmental and Behavioral Influences." Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 10, no. 3 (September 1996): 188–204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/brbi.1996.0018.
Full textGuenther, A., and F. Trillmich. "Photoperiod influences the behavioral and physiological phenotype during ontogeny." Behavioral Ecology 24, no. 2 (November 1, 2012): 402–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars177.
Full textCoyle, Sallyeana, T. Celeste Napier, and George R. Breese. "Ontogeny of tolerance to haloperidol: Behavioral and biochemical measures." Developmental Brain Research 23, no. 1 (November 1985): 27–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0165-3806(85)90004-5.
Full textCrusio, Wim E., and Andrea Schmitt. "Prenatal effects of parity on behavioral ontogeny in mice." Physiology & Behavior 59, no. 6 (June 1996): 1171–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(95)02251-1.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Behavioral ontogeny"
Finger, Jean Sebastien. "Personality in wild juvenile lemon sharks: Consistency, behavioral syndrome and ontogeny." Doctoral thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.18452/19996.
Full textIn this thesis, I investigated different aspects of personality in juvenile lemon sharks (Negaprion brevirostris). I repeatedly tested individuals in a novel open field test. This experiment showed that juvenile lemon sharks consistently differ in their behavior. In addition, repeated exposures to the novel open field, allowed me to demonstrate the presence of habituation. Habituation was used as an indication that this test can be used to investigate reaction to novelty. Finally, this experiment also revealed that individuals have variable rates of habituation. Second, I tested consistent individual differences in some aspects of their social behavior over a few days up to a four-month period. While retesting individuals, group composition was changed to insure that repeatability was not due to the repetition of the same social environment between tests. Here again, I found that juvenile lemon sharks showed personality differences in their social behavior and this despite group composition changes and a four-month period between tests. Third, I tested the presence of a behavioral syndrome between sociability and reaction to a novel open field while considering potential variation in this syndrome through ontogeny and locations of capture. In addition, I investigated the maintenance of individual differences in different age classes and locations of capture. I found a significant negative correlation between sociability and reaction to novelty in sharks from one of the two nurseries tested but only when they were older than a year. In addition, I found that young of year sharks did not demonstrate long term consistency in their behavior as opposed to older sharks.
Ngo, Chi Thao. "ONTOGENY OF EPISODIC MEMORY: A COMPONENTIAL APPROACH." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2019. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/571412.
Full textPh.D.
Episodic memory binds together the people, objects, and locations that make up the specific events of our lives, and allows the recall of our past in the service of current and future goals. Recent models of memory have posited that the hippocampus instantiates computations critical for episodic memory including mnemonic discrimination, relational binding, and holistic retrieval. Collectively, this set of studies aim to chart the ontogeny of each key components of episodic memory. We found robust improvements in children’s abilities to form complex relational structures and to make fine-grained discrimination for individual items from age 4 to age 6. However, relational memory dependent on context discrimination appears to follow a more protracted development. Furthermore, relational binding and mnemonic discrimination (item and context levels) undergo age-related decrements in senescence. Despite relatively poor relational binding capabilities, children as young as age 4 are able to retrieve multi-element events holistically, such as successfully retrieving of one aspect of an event predicts the retrieval success of other aspects from the same event. Critically, the degree of holistic episodic retrieval increases from age 4 to young adulthood. This multi-process approach provides important theoretical insights into lifespan profile of episodic memory.
Temple University--Theses
Gurguis, Christopher Ignatius. "The Function And Early Ontogeny Of Individual Variation In Conspicuous Begging Behavior In A Passerine Bird." Thesis, The University of Arizona, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/338958.
Full textTamborski, Steven W. "The Ontogeny of the Mouse Oxytocin System and Potential Organizational Effects of Oxytocin on Intermale Aggression." Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1397649029.
Full textField, Kristin L. "Effects of sex ratio on ontogeny of sexual behavior and mating competence in male guppies, poecilia reticulata." The Ohio State University, 2004. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1091578035.
Full textChaney, Morgan Edward. "Learning to Live, or Living to Learn?Age-related differences in foraging behavior and the extended juvenile period of Cebus capucinus." Kent State University / OhioLINK, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=kent1428846210.
Full textBalda, Mara A. "Ontogeny- and Sex-Dependent Contributions of the Neuronal Nitric Oxide Synthase (nNOS) Gene to Rewarding and Psychomotor Stimulating Effects of Cocaine." Scholarly Repository, 2009. http://scholarlyrepository.miami.edu/oa_dissertations/257.
Full textFinger, Jean Sebastien [Verfasser], Marcel [Gutachter] Robischon, Gudrun [Gutachter] Brockmann, Max [Gutachter] Wolf, Pawel [Gutachter] Romanczuk, and Jens [Gutachter] Krause. "Personality in wild juvenile lemon sharks: Consistency, behavioral syndrome and ontogeny / Jean Sebastien Finger ; Gutachter: Marcel Robischon, Gudrun Brockmann, Max Wolf, Pawel Romanczuk, Jens Krause." Berlin : Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 2019. http://d-nb.info/1189145952/34.
Full textSales, Raul Fernandes Dantas de. "Ecologia alimentar e comportamento de forrageamento de Ameivula aff. ocellifera (Squamata: Teiidae) em ?rea de caatinga do nordeste do Brasil." Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, 2013. http://repositorio.ufrn.br:8080/jspui/handle/123456789/17344.
Full textCoordena??o de Aperfei?oamento de Pessoal de N?vel Superior
This study investigated the influence of intrinsic and extrinsic factors on the feeding ecology and foraging behavior of the whiptail lizard Ameivula aff. ocellifera, a new species widely distributed in the Brazilian Caatinga, and that is in process of description. In attendance to the objectives, the Dissertation was structured in two chapters, which correspond to scientific articles, one already published and the other to be submitted for publication. In Chapter 1 were analyzed the general diet composition, the relationship between lizard size and prey size, and the occurrence of sexual and ontogenetic differences in the diet. Chapter 2 contemplates a seasonal analysis of diet composition during two rainy seasons interspersed with a dry season, and the quantitative analysis of foraging behavior during two distinct periods. The diet composition was determined through stomach analysis of lizards (N = 111) collected monthly by active search, between September 2008 and August 2010, in the Esta??o Ecol?gica do Serid? (ESEC Serid?), state of Rio Grande do Norte. Foraging behavior was investigated during a rainy and a dry month of 2012 also in ESEC Serid?, by determining percent of time moving (PTM), number of movements per minute (MPM) and prey capture rate by the lizards (N = 28) during foraging. The main prey category in the diet of Ameivula aff. ocellifera was Insect larvae, followed by Orthoptera, Coleoptera and Araneae. Termites (Isoptera) were important only in numeric terms, having negligible volumetric contribution (<2%) and low frequency of occurrence, an uncommon feature among whiptail lizards. Males and females did not differ neither in diet composition nor in foraging behavior. Adults and juveniles ingested similar prey types, but differed in prey size. Maximum and minimum prey sizes were positively correlated with lizard body size, suggesting that in this population individuals experience an ontogenetic change in diet, eating larger prey items while growing, and at the same time excluding smaller ones. The diet showed significant seasonal differences; during the two rainy seasons (2009 and 2010), the predominant prey in diet were Insect larvae, Coleoptera and Orthoptera, while in the dry season the predominant prey were Insect larvae, Hemiptera, Araneae and Orthoptera. The degree of mobility of consumed prey during the rainy seasons was lower, mainly due to a greater consumption of larvae (highly sedentary prey) during these periods. Population niche breadth was higher in the dry season, confirming the theoretical prediction that when food is scarce, the diets tend to be more generalized. Considering the entire sample, Ameivula aff. ocellifera showed 61,0 ? 15,0% PTM, 2,03 ? 0,30 MPM, and captured 0,13 ? 0,14 per minute. Foraging mode was similar to that found for other whiptail lizards regarding PTM, but MPM was relatively superior. Seasonal differences were verified for PTM, which was significantly higher in the rainy season (66,4 ? 12,1) than in the dry season (51,5 ? 15,6). It is possible that this difference represents a behavioral adjustment in response to seasonal variation in the abundance and types of prey available in the environment in each season
Este estudo investigou a influ?ncia de fatores intr?nsecos e extr?nsecos sobre a ecologia alimentar e o comportamento de forrageamento do lagarto cauda-de-chicote (whiptail) Ameivula aff. ocellifera, uma esp?cie nova com ampla distribui??o na Caatinga, e que est? em fase de descri??o. Em atendimento aos objetivos, a Disserta??o foi estruturada na forma de dois cap?tulos, os quais correspondem a artigos cient?ficos, um j? publicado e o outro a ser submetido ? publica??o. No Cap?tulo 1 s?o analisadas a composi??o geral da dieta, a rela??o entre o tamanho corporal dos lagartos e o tamanho das presas consumidas, e a ocorr?ncia de diferen?as sexuais e ontogen?ticas na dieta. O Cap?tulo 2 contempla a composi??o da dieta em termos sazonais, durante duas esta??es chuvosas intercaladas por uma esta??o seca, e an?lise quantitativa do comportamento de forrageamento durante dois per?odos distintos. A composi??o da dieta foi identificada atrav?s da an?lise do conte?do estomacal de lagartos (N = 111) coletados mensalmente por busca ativa entre setembro de 2008 e agosto de 2010, na Esta??o Ecol?gica do Serid? (ESEC Serid?), estado do Rio Grande do Norte. O comportamento de forrageamento foi investigado durante um m?s chuvoso e um m?s seco do ano de 2012 tamb?m na ESEC Serid?, avaliando-se a porcentagem do tempo gasta em movimento (PTM), o n?mero de movimentos por minuto (MPM) e taxa de captura de presas pelos lagartos (N = 28) durante o forrageamento. A principal categoria de presa na dieta de Ameivula aff. ocellifera foi Larvas de insetos, seguido por Orthoptera, Coleoptera e Araneae. T?rmitas (Isoptera) foram importantes somente em n?mero, com contribui??o volum?trica desprez?vel (<2%) e baixa frequ?ncia de ocorr?ncia, um tra?o incomum entre os lagartos whiptails. Machos e f?meas n?o diferiram nem na composi??o da dieta nem no comportamento de forrageamento. Adultos e juvenis se alimentaram de categorias de presa similares, mas diferiram no tamanho das presas. Os tamanhos m?ximo e m?nimo das presas foram positivamente correlacionados com o tamanho dos lagartos, sugerindo que na popula??o estudada os indiv?duos sofrem uma mudan?a ontogen?tica na dieta, consumindo itens alimentares maiores ? medida que crescem, e ao mesmo tempo excluindo presas menores. A dieta apresentou diferen?as sazonais significativas; durante as duas esta??es chuvosas (2009 e 2010), as presas predominantes na dieta foram Larvas de inseto, Coleoptera e Orthoptera, enquanto na esta??o seca as presas predominantes foram Larvas de inseto, Hemiptera, Araneae e Orthoptera. O grau de mobilidade das presas consumidas durante as esta??es chuvosas foi menor, principalmente devido ao maior consumo de larvas (presas altamente sedent?rias) durante esses per?odos. A largura de nicho da popula??o foi maior na x esta??o seca, confirmando a predi??o te?rica de que quando o alimento ? escasso, as dietas tendem a ser mais generalizadas. Considerando a amostra total, Ameivula aff. ocellifera apresentou 61,0 ? 15,0% PTM, 2,03 ? 0,30 MPM, e capturou 0,13 ? 0,14 presas por minuto. O modo de forrageamento foi similar ao encontrado para outros lagartos whiptails quanto a PTM, mas MPM foi relativamente superior. Diferen?as sazonais foram verificadas quanto a PTM, que foi significativamente maior na esta??o chuvosa (66,4 ? 12,1) que na esta??o seca (51,5 ? 15,6). ? poss?vel que essa diferen?a represente um ajuste comportamental em resposta ? varia??o sazonal na abund?ncia e tipos de presas dispon?veis no ambiente nas diferentes esta??es
Goliath, Jesse Roberto. "A 3D Morphological Analysis of the Ontogenetic Patterning of Human Subchondral Bone Microarchitecture in the Proximal Tibia." The Ohio State University, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1494273830449469.
Full textBooks on the topic "Behavioral ontogeny"
G, Else James, and Lee Phyllis C, eds. Primate ontogeny, cognition, and social behaviour. Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]: Cambridge University Press, 1986.
Find full textSchassburger, Ronald M. Vocal communication in the timber wolf, Canis lupus, Linnaeus: Structure, motivation, and ontogeny. Berlin: Paul Parey Scientific Publishers, 1993.
Find full textMiller, Warren B. The ontogeny of human bonding systems: Evolutionary origins, neural bases, and psychological manifestations. Boston: Kluwer Academic, 2001.
Find full textFrom fetus to child: An observational and psychoanalytic study. London: Tavistock/Routledge, 1992.
Find full textRobinson, Scott R. A comparative study of prenatal behavior ontogeny in altricial and precocial murid rodents. 1989.
Find full textHan, Shihui. A culture–behavior–brain-loop model of human development. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198743194.003.0008.
Full textHodin, Jason, Matthew C. Ferner, Andreas Heyland, and Brian Gaylord, eds. I Feel That! Fluid Dynamics and Sensory Aspects of Larval Settlement across Scales. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198786962.003.0013.
Full textSilk, Joan B., and Bailey R. House. The Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Prosocial Behavior. Oxford University Press, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199738182.013.0020.
Full textBook chapters on the topic "Behavioral ontogeny"
Miller, Warren B., and Joseph Lee Rodgers. "Behavioral Domains: Three Perspectives." In The Ontogeny of Human Bonding Systems, 5–13. Boston, MA: Springer US, 2001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-1551-7_2.
Full textShouse, Margaret N., James Langer, Paul Farber, Michael Bier, Orly Alcalde, and Ronald Szymusiak. "Ontogeny of Temporal Lobe Epilepsy in Amygdala-Kindled Kittens." In Advances in Behavioral Biology, 45–63. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-5375-5_5.
Full textSeethy, Ashikh, Subhradip Karmakar, and Karthikeyan Pethusamy. "Ontogeny." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 1–4. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_526-1.
Full textAlberts, J. R. "Postnatal Development of Olfactory-Guided Behavior in Rodents." In Ontogeny of Olfaction, 227–35. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-71576-1_17.
Full textDzal, Yvonne A., Allyson K. Menzies, Quinn M. R. Webber, and Craig K. R. Willis. "Torpor and Tinbergen: Integrating Physiological and Behavioral Traits with Ontogeny, Phylogenetic History, Survival and Fitness to Understand Heterothermy in Bats." In 50 Years of Bat Research, 223–38. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-54727-1_14.
Full textSpear, Norman E., and James S. Miller. "Ontogeny of Spontaneous Alternation Behavior." In Spontaneous Alternation Behavior, 131–44. New York, NY: Springer New York, 1989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-8879-1_7.
Full textJohnson, Michele A., Ellee G. Cook, and Bonnie K. Kircher. "Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Display Behavior." In Behavior of Lizards, 259–87. Boca Raton, Florida : CRC Press, [2019]: CRC Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1201/9781498782739-9.
Full textHall, W. G. "The Ontogeny of Ingestive Behavior." In Neurobiology of Food and Fluid Intake, 77–123. Boston, MA: Springer US, 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4613-0577-4_5.
Full textKoziol, Leonard F., Deborah Ely Budding, and Dana Chidekel. "The Ontogeny of Functional Brain Networks." In ADHD as a Model of Brain-Behavior Relationships, 29. New York, NY: Springer New York, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8382-3_11.
Full textMartínez, Salvador, Eduardo Puelles, and Diego Echevarria. "Ontogeny of the Vertebrate Nervous System." In Neurosciences - From Molecule to Behavior: a university textbook, 47–61. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-10769-6_3.
Full text