To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Behavioral repertoire.

Journal articles on the topic 'Behavioral repertoire'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Behavioral repertoire.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Aggleton, John P. "Revealing the rat's behavioral repertoire." Nature Neuroscience 8, no. 7 (2005): 843. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn0705-843.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lana, Robert E. "SOCIAL HISTORY AND THE BEHAVIORAL REPERTOIRE." Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 62, no. 2 (1994): 315–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1994.62-315.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Graziano, Michael S. A., and Tyson N. Aflalo. "Mapping Behavioral Repertoire onto the Cortex." Neuron 56, no. 2 (2007): 239–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.013.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Luttrell, Sarah A. M., Megan E. Gallagher, and Bernard Lohr. "Multiple estimation methods suggest similar repertoire sizes for Gulf Coast and eastern marsh wrens with no correlation between repertoire size and migratory distance." Behaviour 153, no. 3 (2016): 287–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003342.

Full text
Abstract:
There are many ways for signalling systems to be complex, one of which is a large signal repertoire. However, it is frequently challenging to estimate larger repertoires accurately. We present the first description of repertoire size for a subspecies of marsh wren on the Gulf Coast of North America,Cistothorus palustris thryophilus, using four repertoire estimation techniques (simple enumeration, Wildenthal curve-fitting, Davidson & Wilkinson curve-fitting, and the Coupon Collector model). We demonstrate that the repertoire estimation methods in question perform with varying degrees of accuracy under natural conditions, and propose that rather than establishing an absolute repertoire size for a given individual it may instead be necessary to use multiple techniques to establish a repertoire range. We find thatC. p. thryophilushas a song repertoire size falling well within the range of repertoires produced by other marsh wren subspecies in eastern North America, although it may be on the larger end of this range. We find no evidence supporting a correlation between migratory distance and increased repertoire size in this species, as might be predicted under a sexual selection hypothesis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Ghirlanda, Stefano, Magnus Enquist, and Johan Lind. "Coevolution of intelligence, behavioral repertoire, and lifespan." Theoretical Population Biology 91 (February 2014): 44–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tpb.2013.09.005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Cunha, Ana Margarida, Joana Pereira-Mendes, Armando Almeida, Marco Rafael Guimarães, and Hugo Leite-Almeida. "Chronic pain impact on rodents’ behavioral repertoire." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 119 (December 2020): 101–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Cully, A., and J. B. Mouret. "Evolving a Behavioral Repertoire for a Walking Robot." Evolutionary Computation 24, no. 1 (2016): 59–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/evco_a_00143.

Full text
Abstract:
Numerous algorithms have been proposed to allow legged robots to learn to walk. However, most of these algorithms are devised to learn walking in a straight line, which is not sufficient to accomplish any real-world mission. Here we introduce the Transferability-based Behavioral Repertoire Evolution algorithm (TBR-Evolution), a novel evolutionary algorithm that simultaneously discovers several hundreds of simple walking controllers, one for each possible direction. By taking advantage of solutions that are usually discarded by evolutionary processes, TBR-Evolution is substantially faster than independently evolving each controller. Our technique relies on two methods: (1) novelty search with local competition, which searches for both high-performing and diverse solutions, and (2) the transferability approach, which combines simulations and real tests to evolve controllers for a physical robot. We evaluate this new technique on a hexapod robot. Results show that with only a few dozen short experiments performed on the robot, the algorithm learns a repertoire of controllers that allows the robot to reach every point in its reachable space. Overall, TBR-Evolution introduced a new kind of learning algorithm that simultaneously optimizes all the achievable behaviors of a robot.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Graziano, Michael. "THE ORGANIZATION OF BEHAVIORAL REPERTOIRE IN MOTOR CORTEX." Annual Review of Neuroscience 29, no. 1 (2006): 105–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112924.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Peshek, Kathleen R., and Daniel T. Blumstein. "Can rarefaction be used to estimate song repertoire size in birds?" Current Zoology 57, no. 3 (2011): 300–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/57.3.300.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Song repertoire size is the number of distinct syllables, phrases, or song types produced by an individual or population. Repertoire size estimation is particularly difficult for species that produce highly variable songs and those that produce many song types. Estimating repertoire size is important for ecological and evolutionary studies of speciation, studies of sexual selection, as well as studies of how species may adapt their songs to various acoustic environments. There are several methods to estimate repertoire size, however prior studies discovered that all but a full numerical count of song types might have substantial inaccuracies associated with them. We evaluated a somewhat novel approach to estimate repertoire size—rarefaction; a technique ecologists use to measure species diversity on individual and population levels. Using the syllables within American robins’ Turdus migratorius repertoire, we compared the most commonly used techniques of estimating repertoires to the results of a rarefaction analysis. American robins have elaborate and unique songs with few syllables shared between individuals, and there is no evidence that robins mimic their neighbors. Thus, they are an ideal system in which to compare techniques. We found that the rarefaction technique results resembled that of the numerical count, and were better than two alternative methods (behavioral accumulation curves, and capture-recapture) to estimate syllable repertoire size. Future estimates of repertoire size, particularly in vocally complex species, may benefit from using rarefaction techniques when numerical counts are unable to be performed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bostwick, Kimberly S. "Display Behaviors, Mechanical Sounds, and Evolutionary Relationships of The Club-Winged Manakin (Machaeropterus Deliciosus)." Auk 117, no. 2 (2000): 465–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/auk/117.2.465.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Male Club-winged Manakins (Machaeropterus deliciosus) are known to produce a unique mechanical sound with their extremely modified secondary feathers, yet the species remains poorly known behaviorally. I observed lekking males in Reserva Maquipucuna, Pichincha Province, Ecuador, to better understand the role of mechanical sounds, the behavioral repertoire, and other details of the natural history of this species. The behavioral repertoire of M. deliciosus is much more diverse than previously documented; it includes four mechanical phrases, two vocal sounds, and seven display behaviors. Mechanical sounds constitute the most prominent elements of the species' displays, replacing vocal sounds for territorial advertisement. I examine possible homology of the observed behaviors and propose a new phylogenetic hypothesis, that Machaeropterus forms a monophyletic clade with the genus Pipra, based on the existence of a complex shared courtship display.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Chen, Chuan-Yu, Catherine M. Dormitzer, Ulises Gutiérrez, Kenneth Vittetoe, Gonzalo B. González, and James C. Anthony. "The adolescent behavioral repertoire as a context for drug exposure: behavioral autarcesis at play." Addiction 99, no. 7 (2004): 897–906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00774.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Akyörek, Aladin, and Alex F. Kalverboer. "Aspects of the Behavioral Repertoire of an Autistic/Epileptic Child." Perceptual and Motor Skills 62, no. 3 (1986): 843–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.62.3.843.

Full text
Abstract:
The behavior of an autistic/epileptic child was investigated through indoor, outdoor, and laboratory contexts to acquire a catalog of behavior for the autistic condition, which to date was only partially available, and to provide the parameters of the units composing it. The catalog obtained is composed of 111 units of behavior whose descriptions and quantitative characteristics are also given. The study has also dealt with the subject's global behavioral output and lexical aspects of her repertoire. The results appear to admit the conclusion that the child's behavior was operating under a tendency to return to a stereotypic output state from other, short-lasting states of behavior. This observation needs a subsequent confirmatory analysis of behavioral output states involved. Moreover, the search for the orderly type/token relationship, proposed in 1977 by Fagen and Goldman to hold for behavior, has led to equivocal results.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Forkosh, Oren, Stoyo Karamihalev, Simone Roeh, et al. "Identity domains capture individual differences from across the behavioral repertoire." Nature Neuroscience 22, no. 12 (2019): 2023–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0516-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

JOHANSON, CHRIS-ELLYN, FARIFTEH F. DUFFY, and JAMES C. ANTHONY. "Associations between drug use and behavioral repertoire in urban youths1." Addiction 91, no. 11 (1996): 1731–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1996.tb02277.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

JOHANSON, CHRIS-ELLYN, FARIFTEH F. DUFFY, and JAMES C. ANTHONY. "Associations between drug use and behavioral repertoire in urban youths1." Addiction 91, no. 4 (1996): 523–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1996.tb02310.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

JOHANSON, CHRIS-ELLYN, FARIFTEH F. DUFFY, and JAMES C. ANTHONY. "Associations between drug use and behavioral repertoire in urban youths1." Addiction 91, no. 4 (1996): 523–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1360-0443.1996.9111173114.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Johanson, Chris-Ellyn, Farifteh F. Duffy, and James C. Anthony. "Associations between drug use and behavioral repertoire in urban youths." Addiction 91, no. 11 (1996): 1731–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09652149639286.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Lawrence, Katherine A., Peter Lenk, and Robert E. Quinn. "Behavioral complexity in leadership: The psychometric properties of a new instrument to measure behavioral repertoire." Leadership Quarterly 20, no. 2 (2009): 87–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.01.014.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Marler, Peter, JÖRG BÖHNER, and Marthaleah Chaiken. "Repertoire Turnover and the Timing of Song Acquisition in European Starlings." Behaviour 128, no. 1-2 (1994): 25–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853994x00037.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractIn order to establish unambiguously whether a reputed open-ended learner, the European starling, could memorize new songs in adulthood, we exposed hand-reared male starlings to a series of either live conspecific tutors or tape recordings of conspecific song over a period of 18 months, and isolated them from any other exposure to conspecific song. The song motifs produced by the birds were compared with those of their tutors or training tapes to infer the time of motif memorization. We found that the starlings continued to memorize and produce new song motifs, both within and between breeding seasons, up to the age of at least 18 months. Not all the new motifs in the repertoires had been recently memorized; in some cases there was an apparent lag of a year and a half between memorization and production. There were no significant differences between live-tutored and tape-tutored groups in the timing of learning or in the rate of repertoire turnover (55-92% new motifs in each repertoire sample), although, as we have reported previously, live-tutored birds imitated a greater number of motifs and developed larger repertoires.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Hedwig, Daniela, Kurt Hammerschmidt, Roger Mundry, Martha M. Robbins, and Christophe Boesch. "Acoustic structure and variation in mountain and western gorilla close calls: a syntactic approach." Behaviour 151, no. 8 (2014): 1091–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003175.

Full text
Abstract:
Our understanding of the functioning of a species’ vocal repertoire can be greatly improved by investigating acoustic variation and using objective classification schemes based on acoustic structure. Here we used a syntactic approach to investigate the acoustic structure of the gorilla close distance vocalizations (‘close calls’), which remain as yet little understood. We examined 2130 calls of 10 mountain gorillas (Gorilla beringei beringei) from Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda, and 5 western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) from Bai Hokou, Central African Republic. We segmented calls into units using distinct acoustic features and employed model-based cluster analyses to define the repertoire of unit types. We then examined how unit types were combined into calls. Lastly, we compared unit type use between age–sex classes and the two study groups. We found that the gorilla close calls consist of 5 intergraded acoustic unit types which were flexibly but yet non-randomly concatenated into 159 combinations. Our results are in line with previous quantitative acoustic analyses demonstrating a high degree of acoustic variation in a variety of animal vocal repertoires, particularly close distance vocalizations. Our findings add on to (1) the recent argument that the common practice of describing vocal repertoires as either discrete or graded may be of little value as such distinctions may be driven by human perception and non-quantitative descriptions of vocal repertoires, and (2) recent studies indicating that flexibility in close range social calls can come about through combinatorial systems, which previously have been studied primarily in long distance vocalizations. Furthermore, our study highlights differences in the vocal repertoire of western and mountain gorillas, as expected given differences in environment and social behaviour. Our results offer opportunities for further in-depth studies investigating the function of the gorilla close calls, which will contribute to a more comprehensive understanding of ape vocal communication in general.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Vieira, Alexsandro Santana, Wedson Desidério Fernandes, and William Fernando Antonialli-Junior. "Behavioral Differentiation and Ovarian Development of Unmated Gynes, Queens, and Workers ofEctatomma vizottoiAlmeida 1987 (Formicidae, Ectatomminae)." Psyche: A Journal of Entomology 2012 (2012): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2012/349896.

Full text
Abstract:
Behavioral differentiation and ovarian development of unmated gynes, queens, and workers ofEctatomma vizottoiwere investigated in laboratory conditions. Forty-one behavioral acts were identified and quantified for workers, 19 for queens and 24 for unmated gynes, for an overall species repertoire of 42 different behavioral acts. Ovipositing reproductive eggs was an exclusive task of the queen, whereas workers showed 15 caste-specific behaviors. The most important (frequent) behaviors for the queens were brood care, immobility, and reproduction, and for workers were immobility, grooming/interaction, brood care, and foraging. Unmated gynes (not winged) primarily showed immobility, brood care, grooming/interaction, and foraging. Analysis of ovarian development showed that unmated gynes had little-developed ovarioles, in contrast to queens. Queens and unmated gynes showed a clear behavioral differentiation, in which queens played the role of reproducers and unmated gynes performed activities belonging to the worker repertoire. Despite the presence of several breeding queens in the colony, functional monogyny was the rule.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Marques, João C., Simone Lackner, Rita Félix, and Michael B. Orger. "Structure of the Zebrafish Locomotor Repertoire Revealed with Unsupervised Behavioral Clustering." Current Biology 28, no. 2 (2018): 181–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.12.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Poirier-Poulin, Samuel, and Julie A. Teichroeb. "The vocal repertoire of an African colobine, Colobus angolensis ruwenzorii: a multi-level society compared to congeners in stable groups." Behaviour 157, no. 7 (2020): 597–628. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10015.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Vocal behaviour offers a window into understanding the social life and evolution of animals. Colobine monkeys show great interspecific and interpopulation variation in their social organization and behaviour. Recent research has shown that Rwenzori Angolan colobus (Colobus angolensis ruwenzorii) differ substantially from other black-and-white colobus in forming a multi-level society. No previous research has been conducted on the communication of C. a. ruwenzorii, but the social complexity hypothesis for communication suggests that more complex societies should evolve more complex communication repertoires. Our objective was to catalogue the vocal repertoire of C. a. ruwenzorii at Nabugabo, Uganda, and to compare it with the data available on congeners regarding intergroup tolerance, vocal repertoire size, and acoustic and behavioural features of vocal communication. Vocalizations were subject to spectrographic and behavioural analysis, and a descriptive analysis of each vocalization type was made. The influence of a few environmental and social factors on calling rates was also examined. We describe five vocalizations (i.e., the snort, roar, squeak, scream and pok) and one non-vocal signal (i.e., the tongue click) in this subspecies and their contexts. Distinct alarm calls are made for dogs, and these are given more often near the edge of the forest where humans frequent. We did not find that C. a. ruwenzorii showed a greater vocal repertoire than C. guereza or C. polykomos, which do not live in multi-level societies. Further, preliminary data do not indicate greater calling rates in larger core units of C. a. ruwenzorii compared to smaller units. These findings support the view that these primates’ vocalizations tend to be relatively conserved despite large differences in social organization.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Sorjonen, Jorma. "Temporal and Spatial Differences in Traditions and Repertoires in the Song of the Thrush Nightingale (Luscinia Luscinia)." Behaviour 102, no. 3-4 (1987): 196–211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853986x00126.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractDifferences in the song repertoires of males and in the song-pools of the thrush nightingale populations were studied in 1972, 1983 and 1984 in southern Finland. Changes in male repertoires and in the song-pool of one population were monitored in 1972 and 1980-1985. The thrush nightingales in a local population had repertoires that were more similar to each other than to those of the males in other local populations. The similarity of the repertoires decreased with increasing distance, but there were no clear-cut dialect boundaries between local populations. The song repertoire of a male was more similar to that of the adjacent males than to his own repertoire of the previous year. The similarity of the repertoires of adjacent singers increased during the singing period. This similarity was partly due to the same song-types being used with about equal frequency, but obviously the males were also able to learn new songs from their adjacent singers. After dispersal to a breeding area thrush nightingales learn at least some new song-types, even at the age of two to four years. Some old breeders were able to copy new song-types from the immigrants (mostly young males) or the playback tape. The newly copied song-types were loud and simple in structure, whereas the song-types soon to be abandoned were weak in amplitude. In one population, studied from 1980 to 1985, the repertoires of the males tended to become more similar in successive years. This tendency, however, did not occur in 1984 when the proportion of immigrants in the population was unusually high. The major changes in local song traditions were due to "cultural diffusion" by males originating from areas with other traditions; this diffusion greatly enriched the local song-pool, especially in years when the rate of immigration was high.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Rosado de Miranda Justo, João Manuel. "Neonatal behavior as biologically motivated behavior." International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology. Revista INFAD de Psicología. 1, no. 1 (2016): 25. http://dx.doi.org/10.17060/ijodaep.2015.n1.v1.123.

Full text
Abstract:
Objectives: To discuss the possibility that neonatal behavior can be understood as biologically motivated behavior. Hypothesis: Human newborns present a behavioral repertoire which corresponds to the phylogenetic evolution and adaptation of the human species to environmental circumstances. Method: The behavioral repertoire of the human newborn is discussed at the light of the neurobehavioral evaluation of babies at the first moments of extra-uterine life, namely the NBAS (Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale) by T. Berry Brazelton. This description is articulated with several aspects of human experience: a) adequacy for survival after birth; b) sequence from prenatal behavior to postnatal behavior, c) similarity between prenatal sleep/awake states and postnatal sleep/awake states and d) postnatal behavioral competence as a result of prenatal training (the performance of prenatal behavior as an exercise towards the enhancement of postnatal behavioral performance). Conclusion: The articulation above described will be used to enlighten future research in the fields of neonatal behavior and prenatal behavior. It will also be used to deepen possibilities of psychological interventions based at presentations offered to mothers, fathers or families when assessing newborns’ behavior (Brazelton demonstrations).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Kuo, Nai-Cheng, Loretta Aniezue, Ashley Herzberg, Stephanie Cruz, and Olivia Rodriguez. "Academic and Behavioral Interventions for Students with Emotional and Behavioral Disorders (EBD): A Systematic Review." Journal of Studies in Education 8, no. 2 (2018): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/jse.v8i2.12802.

Full text
Abstract:
Teachers who work with students with emotional and behavioral disorders (EBD) play a vital role in public education, but they often experience a wide range of challenges when working with this group of students. To improve the quality of instruction for students with EBD, teachers not only need to equip themselves with a repertoire of up-to-date evidence-based practices, but also know the limitations of these practices. Through a systematic review of empirical studies published in peer-reviewed journals between 2006 and early 2018, this study reviews and discusses practical teaching and research implications on serving and supporting students with EBD in public education.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Cao, Dandan, Hong Zhou, Wei Wei, et al. "Vocal repertoire of adult captive red pandas (Ailurus fulgens)." Animal Biology 66, no. 2 (2016): 145–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15707563-00002493.

Full text
Abstract:
Vocal signals are a common communication tool used to recognize different individuals, advertise fertile phases or discriminate amongst potential mates. Therefore, a thorough understanding of vocal repertoires forms the basis for investigating the role of acoustic signaling in the sexual and social behavior of any animal. Red pandas (Ailurus fulgens) are classified as a vulnerable species and have declined by as much as 40% over the past 50 years in China. Adult red pandas are known to call frequently during mating and aggressive encounters; however, no quantitative description of their vocalizations has been attempted. Here, the vocal repertoire of captive red pandas was investigated. Acoustical and statistical analyses indicated seven vocalization types during the breeding season: “growl”, “bark”, “squeal”, “bleat”, “hoot”, “grunt” and “twitter”; the spectrogram for each vocalization type was extracted. The type of vocalizations produced varied with behavioral state and implies different functional contexts. Future studies are needed to uncover the functions of red panda vocalizations in individual recognition, sexual selection and social interaction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Rosa, Gabriel L. M., Jesse M. Ellis, Elisa Bonaccorso, and Luiz dos Anjos. "Friend or foe? Social system influences the allocation of signals across functional categories in the repertoires of the New World jays." Behaviour 153, no. 4 (2016): 467–524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-00003360.

Full text
Abstract:
Many factors could influence repertoire size and function. Here, we provide a review of the descriptions of vocal repertoires of New World jays and a unifying terminology for function of their vocalizations. We summarized 19 different functional types of vocal behaviour shared by at least two species, for review purposes. Comparing the 14 studied species, we also classified vocalizations into four major classes defined a priori: ‘Social Integration’, ‘Conflict Resolution’, ‘Predator-related’, and ‘Breeding’. Cooperative breeding, as well as group size, are related not only to repertoire size, but to how species allocate their vocalizations into functional classes. We discuss the tendencies of Conflict Resolution and Social Integration and the possibility of the existence and implications of a trade-off between those functional classes. We identified gaps in the current knowledge on vocalizations of the studied species, and emphasize the importance of empiric evidence of vocalization function in the New World jays.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Drucker, E. G. "Function of pectoral fins in rainbow trout: behavioral repertoire and hydrodynamic forces." Journal of Experimental Biology 206, no. 5 (2003): 813–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.00139.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

CHANGIZI, MARK A. "Relationship between Number of Muscles, Behavioral Repertoire Size, and Encephalization in Mammals." Journal of Theoretical Biology 220, no. 2 (2003): 157–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2003.3125.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Hangya, Balázs, Hyun-Jae Pi, Duda Kvitsiani, Sachin P. Ranade, and Adam Kepecs. "From circuit motifs to computations: mapping the behavioral repertoire of cortical interneurons." Current Opinion in Neurobiology 26 (June 2014): 117–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2014.01.007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Champagne, Danielle L., Carlijn C. M. Hoefnagels, Ronald E. de Kloet, and Michael K. Richardson. "Translating rodent behavioral repertoire to zebrafish (Danio rerio): Relevance for stress research." Behavioural Brain Research 214, no. 2 (2010): 332–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.06.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Sobroza, Tainara Venturini, Laia Segarra Cerqueda, Pedro Ivo Simões, and Marcelo Gordo. "Vocal Repertoire and Its Behavioral Contexts in the Pied Tamarin, Saguinus bicolor." International Journal of Primatology 38, no. 4 (2017): 642–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10764-017-9971-z.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Ferron, J., and J. P. Ouellet. "Physical and behavioral postnatal development of woodchucks (Marmota monax)." Canadian Journal of Zoology 69, no. 4 (1991): 1040–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/z91-149.

Full text
Abstract:
The physical and behavioral postnatal development of woodchucks (Marmota monax) was analyzed from birth to 42 days of age, the age at which weaning begins. Postnatal growth was evaluated by means of daily measurements of body weight, body length, tail length, and length of hind foot. Data on length of ear pinnae, vibrissae, and fur, and on the timing of appearance of some events of physical development are also presented. We considered postnatal development of locomotion, alertness, exploratory behavior, feeding, and comfort and social behavior. Our results indicate no sexual dimorphism in body size in young woodchucks, despite its occurrence in adults. Intraspecific comparisons reveal that woodchucks from different populations show marked variation in growth rate. The high level of intraspecific variation in developmental rates of sciurid rodents provides a warning to those performing interspecific comparisons. The data also suggest that the ratio of growth rate to adult body weight is not related to environmental severity. Behavioral development in M. monax is similar to that of ground squirrels but faster than that of tree squirrels and flying squirrels. There is also no direct relationship between size and timing of behavioral development in sciurid rodents. The behavioral repertoire of young woodchucks is simpler than that of young Spermophilus lateralis, another asocial species. It is possible that specific differences in rates of development of social interactions led to such differentiation in the repertoire of social behavior.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Verheyen, Rudolf Frans, Marcel Eens, and Rianne Pinxten. "Male Song as a Cue for Mate Choice in the European Starling." Behaviour 116, no. 3-4 (1991): 210–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853991x00049.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis study had two aims. First, we looked at individual differences in song characteristics between males of the European starling, and we related song behaviour to factors such as male age, pairing date, polygyny and male breeding success. Second, we experimentally tested whether song has an effect on female mating decisions. 1. During the breeding season, male starlings sing a very long and complex song consisting of a rapid succession of large number of different song types. We observed marked differences between males in average song bout length (a song bout was defined as a period of at least five seconds of song containing no pauses larger than 1.5 seconds) and in song repertoire size. Average song bout lengths ranged from 16 to 35 seconds. The individual repertoire size ranged from 23 to 67 song types. Repertoire size and average song bout length were positively correlated. 2. Both in the field and in captivity, yearling males sang shorter average song bout lengths and had smaller repertoire sizes than older males. 3. Males singing longer average song bouts and having larger repertoire sizes attracted females for pairing before their rivals with shorter average song bouts and smaller repertoire sizes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Opaev, A. S., Meisi Liu, and Zujie Kang. "Behavioral Ecology of Elliot’s Laughingthrush (Trochalopteron (Garrulax) elliotii, Timaliidae, Aves): 2. Vocal Repertoire." Biology Bulletin 44, no. 9 (2017): 1100–1110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1134/s1062359017090084.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Lee, James Siho, Pei-Yin Shih, Oren N. Schaedel, Porfirio Quintero-Cadena, Alicia K. Rogers, and Paul W. Sternberg. "FMRFamide-like peptides expand the behavioral repertoire of a densely connected nervous system." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, no. 50 (2017): E10726—E10735. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1710374114.

Full text
Abstract:
Animals, including humans, can adapt to environmental stress through phenotypic plasticity. The free-living nematode Caenorhabditis elegans can adapt to harsh environments by undergoing a whole-animal change, involving exiting reproductive development and entering the stress-resistant dauer larval stage. The dauer is a dispersal stage with dauer-specific behaviors for finding and stowing onto carrier animals, but how dauers acquire these behaviors, despite having a physically limited nervous system of 302 neurons, is poorly understood. We compared dauer and reproductive development using whole-animal RNA sequencing at fine time points and at sufficient depth to measure transcriptional changes within single cells. We detected 8,042 genes differentially expressed during dauer and reproductive development and observed striking up-regulation of neuropeptide genes during dauer entry. We knocked down neuropeptide processing using sbt-1 mutants and demonstrate that neuropeptide signaling promotes the decision to enter dauer rather than reproductive development. We also demonstrate that during dauer neuropeptides modulate the dauer-specific nictation behavior (carrier animal-hitchhiking) and are necessary for switching from repulsion to CO2 (a carrier animal cue) in nondauers to CO2 attraction in dauers. We tested individual neuropeptides using CRISPR knockouts and existing strains and demonstrate that the combined effects of flp-10 and flp-17 mimic the effects of sbt-1 on nictation and CO2 attraction. Through meta-analysis, we discovered similar up-regulation of neuropeptides in the dauer-like infective juveniles of diverse parasitic nematodes, suggesting the antiparasitic target potential of SBT-1. Our findings reveal that, under stress, increased neuropeptide signaling in C. elegans enhances their decision-making accuracy and expands their behavioral repertoire.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Johnson, A. K., L. J. Sadler, K. J. Stalder, and W. Powers. "Influence of corn co-products on the behavioral repertoire of growing-finishing swine." Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A - Animal Science 58, no. 4 (2008): 209–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09064700802578202.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Stryker, Judith A., Jim L. Atkinson, Robert D. Brown, et al. "Behavioral repertoire assessment of Bengal tigers (Panthera tigris) with focus on thermoregulatory behavior." International Journal of Biometeorology 63, no. 10 (2019): 1369–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00484-019-01753-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Stahlbaum, Cathi C., and Katherine A. Houpt. "The role of the Flehmen response in the behavioral repertoire of the stallion." Physiology & Behavior 45, no. 6 (1989): 1207–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(89)90111-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Silva, Aline N., Rhewter Nunes, Dieferson C. Estrela, Guilherme Malafaia, and André L. S. Castro. "Behavioral repertoire of the poorly known Red-legged Seriema, Cariama cristata (Cariamiformes: Cariamidae)." Revista Brasileira de Ornitologia 24, no. 2 (2016): 73–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf03544333.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Peleg, Gili, Gadi Katzir, Ofer Peleg, et al. "Facial expressions in various emotional states in congenitally blind and sighted subjects." Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution 55, no. 1 (2009): 11–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1560/ijee.55.1.11.

Full text
Abstract:
Most of the studies comparing facial expressions of born blind and sighted persons that have been carried out up until now have involved children as subjects. Various studies claimed that the quantity and quality of facial expressions of congenitally blind persons deteriorate with increasing age. Here, we compared facial expressions of born blind and sighted individuals using adults predominantly. Facial expressions were documented in an individual interview inducing such emotions as think-concentrate, sadness, anger, disgust, joy, and surprise.Common characteristics found amongst studied individuals were: similar repertoires*of movements over the entire interview, high-frequency and high-repertoire proportion of facial movements in concentration, sadness, and anger relative to those in disgust, joy, and surprise, similar distributions of a cumulative repertoire proportion of facial movements, and also common behavioral profiles of frequencies of facial movements in the emotional states discussed. Similar displays of eyebrow movements were found as well in concentration, sadness, and anger.Our study indicates that most tested characteristics of facial movements are common to born blind and sighted subjects, except for different cumulative mean frequencies in different emotional states, which is possibly related to the lack of visual feedback in born blind persons.Our study substantiates the hypothesis that facial expressions are innate and have important cues in the evolution of social communication.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Frey, Bruno S., and Werner W. Pommerehne. "L'Art Pour L'Art? Behavioral Effects of Performing Arts Organizations." Empirical Studies of the Arts 5, no. 1 (1987): 59–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/bwxe-5x8r-qkc8-4w7u.

Full text
Abstract:
The institutional forms of cooperative, profit-oriented (private) and government subsidized non-profit performing arts companies affect the behavior of their management systematically. Thus, the managers of profit-oriented theaters, operas, and orchestras tend to let plays run longer, to have a smaller number of new productions and a more narrow repertoire than do public companies. The number of rehearsals will, ceteris paribus, be smaller, and more plays suiting the preferences of the general public will be offered. These propositions are derived using the economic model of behavior looking also on the incentive effects of different types of government subsidies.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Fernandes, Danilo Camargo, and Daniel Cunha Passos. "The voices of an alleged mute: sound emissions in a Tropidurus lizard." Behaviour 158, no. 8-9 (2021): 819–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1568539x-bja10092.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Although the major Squamata lineages are primarily oriented by chemical or visual sensory systems, many lizards are able to use acoustic information and several species produce sounds. However, while gekkotans are renowned by their complex vocal repertoires, sounds of other lizards are much less known. Herein we characterize the sounds emitted by individuals of Tropidurus catalanensis (Tropiduridae) from southeastern Brazil in response to threat stimuli. Our results revealed that the acoustic display was consistently emitted by adult individuals. The typical sound emission consisted of a single click, very short in duration, and without frequency modulation. This is the first report of sound emission by Tropidurus lizards, expanding the knowledge on the behavioural repertoire of the genus, and contributing to understanding the extension of sound emission in lizards.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Dukas, Reuven, and Andrew Scott. "Fruit fly courtship: The female perspective." Current Zoology 61, no. 6 (2015): 1008–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/61.6.1008.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Research on fruit fly courtship has mostly focused on males’ behavioral repertoire whereas females have been assumed to respond by either rejecting or accepting males. In many fruit fly species including Drosophila melanogaster, however, mating typically follows an extended period of courtship, which provides ample opportunities for females to inform males about their likelihood of mating. Our experiments indeed revealed that sexually immature females in both D. melanogasterand D. simulans showed responses to conspecific males that were distinct from those of sexually mature females. Furthermore, females’ responses to conspecific males were different from their responses to heterospecific males. Our data indicate that females’ behavioral repertoire early in courtship can inform males about their probability of mating if they persist in courting. We hypothesize (i) that males can rely on behavioral feedback from females for optimally allocating their courtship efforts towards distinct female classes, (ii) that males may learn to modulate their courtship behavior based on specific feedback from females, and (iii) that females may learn to alter their behavior towards distinct types of males in order to elicit the desired male response. Overall, we suggest that, although little explored, female behavior determines the dynamics of courtship and mating and can thus influence sexual selection and incipient speciation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Basnet, Ram, Daniela Zizioli, Somrat Taweedet, Dario Finazzi, and Maurizio Memo. "Zebrafish Larvae as a Behavioral Model in Neuropharmacology." Biomedicines 7, no. 1 (2019): 23. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines7010023.

Full text
Abstract:
Zebrafish larvae show a clear and distinct pattern of swimming in response to light and dark conditions, following the development of a swim bladder at 4 days post fertilization. This swimming behavior is increasingly employed in the screening of neuroactive drugs. The recent emergence of high-throughput techniques for the automatic tracking of zebrafish larvae has further allowed an objective and efficient way of finding subtle behavioral changes that could go unnoticed during manual observations. This review highlights the use of zebrafish larvae as a high-throughput behavioral model for the screening of neuroactive compounds. We describe, in brief, the behavior repertoire of zebrafish larvae. Then, we focus on the utilization of light-dark locomotion test in identifying and screening of neuroactive compounds.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Hadders-Algra, Mijna. "Variation and Variability: Key Words in Human Motor Development." Physical Therapy 90, no. 12 (2010): 1823–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.2522/ptj.20100006.

Full text
Abstract:
This article reviews developmental processes in the human brain and basic principles underlying typical and atypical motor development. The Neuronal Group Selection Theory is used as theoretical frame of reference. Evidence is accumulating that abundance in cerebral connectivity is the neural basis of human behavioral variability (ie, the ability to select, from a large repertoire of behavioral solutions, the one most appropriate for a specific situation). Indeed, typical human motor development is characterized by variation and the development of adaptive variability. Atypical motor development is characterized by a limited variation (a limited repertoire of motor strategies) and a limited ability to vary motor behavior according to the specifics of the situation (ie, limited variability). Limitations in variation are related to structural anomalies in which disturbances of cortical connectivity may play a prominent role, whereas limitations in variability are present in virtually all children with atypical motor development. The possible applications of variation and variability in diagnostics in children with or at risk for a developmental motor disorder are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Marcuk, Vladislav, Cromwell Purchase, Donovan de Boer, Marcellus Bürkle, and Katrin Scholtyssek. "Qualitative description of the submission and agonistic behavior of the Spix’s Macaw (Cyanopsitta spixii, Spix 1824), with special reference to the displacement displays." Journal of Ethology 38, no. 3 (2020): 253–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10164-020-00650-6.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe Spix’s Macaw (Cyanopsitta spixii) represents one of the four avian taxa, in which its global population is entirely captively managed. The species was declared “extinct in the wild” after several attempts failed to rediscover any remaining individuals in the wild since 2000. As an integral part of the ongoing ex situ conservation efforts, a long-term ethological study was conducted at the ACTP facility to investigate the behavioral repertoire of the largest subpopulation of this species in captivity. In this paper we provide an illustrated comprehensive ethogram with detailed description of the submission, displacement and agonistic behavior. The agonistic behavior is categorized in two subcategories, where qualitative aspects for distinct behavior elements for the intimidatory and conflict behavior are given. In addition, displacement displays are described in detail for the first time for a species of the genera. In total, 35 distinct behavior elements of the agonistic, displacement and submission behavioral repertoire are covered. Digital video images related to the article are available at http://www.momo-p.com/index-e.html, movieid:momo200417sm05a, momo200416sm01a, momo200417sm06a, momo200416sm06a, momo200416sm04a, momo200417sm03a, momo200417sm02a, momo200416sm05a, momo200417sm04a, momo200416sm02a, momo200416sm03a, and momo200417sm01a.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

McIntosh, Anthony R., and Viktor K. Jirsa. "The hidden repertoire of brain dynamics and dysfunction." Network Neuroscience 3, no. 4 (2019): 994–1008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00107.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this paper is to describe a framework for the understanding of rules that govern how neural system dynamics are coordinated to produce behavior. The framework, structured flows on manifolds (SFM), posits that neural processes are flows depicting system interactions that occur on relatively low-dimension manifolds, which constrain possible functional configurations. Although this is a general framework, we focus on the application to brain disorders. We first explain the Epileptor, a phenomenological computational model showing fast and slow dynamics, but also a hidden repertoire whose expression is similar to refractory status epilepticus . We suggest that epilepsy represents an innate brain state whose potential may be realized only under certain circumstances. Conversely, deficits from damage or disease processes, such as stroke or dementia, may reflect both the disease process per se and the adaptation of the brain. SFM uniquely captures both scenarios. Finally, we link neuromodulation effects and switches in functional network configurations to fast and slow dynamics that coordinate the expression of SFM in the context of cognition. The tools to measure and model SFM already exist, giving researchers access to the dynamics of neural processes that support the concomitant dynamics of the cognitive and behavioral processes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Stewart, Alexander J., Todd L. Parsons, and Joshua B. Plotkin. "Evolutionary consequences of behavioral diversity." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 45 (2016): E7003—E7009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1608990113.

Full text
Abstract:
Iterated games provide a framework to describe social interactions among groups of individuals. This body of work has focused primarily on individuals who face a simple binary choice, such as “cooperate” or “defect.” Real individuals, however, can exhibit behavioral diversity, varying their input to a social interaction both qualitatively and quantitatively. Here we explore how access to a greater diversity of behavioral choices impacts the evolution of social dynamics in populations. We show that, in public goods games, some simple strategies that choose between only two possible actions can resist invasion by all multichoice invaders, even while engaging in relatively little punishment. More generally, access to a larger repertoire of behavioral choices results in a more ”rugged” fitness landscape, with populations able to stabilize cooperation at multiple levels of investment. As a result, increased behavioral choice facilitates cooperation when returns on investments are low, but it hinders cooperation when returns on investments are high. Finally, we analyze iterated rock–paper–scissors games, the nontransitive payoff structure of which means that unilateral control is difficult to achieve. Despite this, we find that a large proportion of multichoice strategies can invade and resist invasion by single-choice strategies—so that even well-mixed populations will tend to evolve and maintain behavioral diversity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography