Academic literature on the topic 'Behavioural biases'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Behavioural biases.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Majmudar, Nirav, Prashant Joshi, and Krishna Kant Dave. "Behavioural Biases in IPO Market." RESEARCH REVIEW International Journal of Multidisciplinary 03, no. 09 (2018): 427–33. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1442464.

Full text
Abstract:
Traditional finance models make many unrealistic assumptions about investors rationality, perfect competition, information asymmetry etc. These assumptions are not valid in real world market. Extant researches show that investors are not rational in their decisions and are biased in their investment behaviour. Researchers have identified several Behavioural Biases. There is lack of research available about Behavioural Bias for IPO Markets. The study identifies eleven Behaviour Biases and attempts to examine the them in IPO Market by conducting a primary study in the state of Gujarat. The study proves that the investors show Behaviour Bias in their investment decisions in IPO Market. Overall Investors exhibit Loss Aversion Bias, Stories to Facts Bias, Recency Bias, & Overconfidence Bias. While they are not biased for Confirmation, Self-Serving, Planning Fallacy, Choice Paralysis, Herding, Rule of Thumb and Disposition Effect. The investors are different when evaluated for association between location and Behavioural Biases. Surat is among the most biased Investors having nine biases. The Investors of Rajkot have five biases and for three they are unbiased. While Investors of Vadodara are just one bias and being unbiased to seven Behavioural Bias.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kumar, Rahul, and Nagendra Kumar Jha. "BEHAVIOURAL BIASES INFLUENCING INVESTMENT DECISIONS OF INDIVIDUAL INVESTORS." International Journal of Research in Commerce and Management Studies 06, no. 03 (2024): 68–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.38193/ijrcms.2024.6308.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this present study is to ratify the scale of behavioural biases such as overconfidence, herding effect, loss aversion and investment decisions relating to individual investors who invest in Indian stock market. The present study assesses the influence of aforesaid behavioural biases on investment decisions made by individual investors. The study is based on a questionnaire investigation by including the individual investors of Indian stock market residing in Bihar (India) using a convenient sampling technique. 500 hundred questionnaires have been distributed among the individual investors and 428 responses has been taken into consideration for the present study. The collected data were analysed through multiple regression using SPSS. The present study found that the scale used to measure behavioural biases and investment decisions of individual investors were valid and overconfidence biases have a significant influence on investment decisions of individual investors. In contrary, herding effect and loss aversion does not significantly influence the investment decision of individual investor. The present study found that the individual investors are behaviourally biased while making investment decisions. The present study backs to the academia of behavioural aspects and individual investors who invest in Indian stock market. The present study supports in understanding the concept of behavioural biases of individual investors investing in Indian stock market.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Akin, Isik. "The Most Common Behavioural Biases among Young Adults in Bristol, UK and Istanbul." Financial Markets, Institutions and Risks 6, no. 1 (2022): 27–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.21272/fmir.6(1).27-39.2022.

Full text
Abstract:
According to traditional finance theories, individuals behave rationally and take financial decisions under this rationality. Contrary to traditional finance theories, behavioural finance states that individuals do not always act rationally because they are affected by emotions and feelings. Thus, behavioural finance can be defined as systematic errors that keep individuals away from rationality. The biases might cause unhelpful or even hurtful decisions. Therefore, a high level of behavioural biases might negatively affect the financial well-being of individuals. It is vital to investigate young adults’ financial behaviours as the future of the economies are influenced by their decisions. In this research, behavioural biases among young adults in Bristol, UK and Istanbul, Turkey, was examined to prevent young adults from making irrational financial decisions by identifying the most common behavioural biases. Thus, economies might be robust than today. According to result of this research, young adults have different behavioural biases depending on their culture. The most common biases among young adults in Bristol are over-optimism, anchoring, categorisation, conservatism, and the illusion of control while they are framing, cognitive dissonance, the illusion of knowledge and cue competition among young adults in Istanbul. These common behavioural biases that young adults in Bristol and Istanbul have to lead to many irrational financial decisions. It is not possible to reduce these behavioural biases by direct intervention, and for this, individuals need to be educated. Families may educate young adults about behavioural biases. After that rest of the education about behavioural biases may be given in the schools. Lastly, individuals should be informed about their behavioural biases and possible effects of these biases on their financial well-being.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Amit, Kumar Singh, Kumar Mohapatra Amiya, Kumar Mohit, and Saxena Ankur. "INFLUENCE OF THE SELECTED BEHAVIOURAL BIASES ON INTERMEDIARIES IN INITIAL PUBLIC OFFERINGS MARKET: A SURVEY." Indian Journal of Economics and Business 20, no. 2 (2021): 427–49. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5506406.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract: This study investigates the role of the selected behavioural biases on intermediaries’ decision making in the Initial Public Offerings (IPO) market. Based on the review of literature, four behavioural biases viz. ‘overconfidence’, ‘availability’, ‘representativeness’ and ‘anchoring’ are included in this study. A self-administered structured questionnaire is designed to measure the behavioural biases. Further, as a part of data analysis, non-parametric tests are used to examine the role of demographic factors on behavioural biases. Besides, behavioural biases are also ranked according to their prevalence among intermediaries. It has been found that intermediaries in the IPO market are prone to behavioural biases. In addition, demographic factors of intermediaries seem to influence behavioural biases. The ‘availability’ and ‘representativeness’ are found to be prominent behavioural biases that influence the intermediaries’ decision-making in the Indian IPO market. The findings can help intermediaries themselves, regulators and issuer firm to augment their understanding of the role of behavioural biases in decision making. A guided ‘nudge’ would help intermediaries to make their decision more rational. Besides, the findings would help policymakers in designing policies, training manuals, and course curricula to minimize intuition-based decision making.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Lisauskiene, Nomeda, and Valdone Darskuviene. "Linking the Robo-advisors Phenomenon and Behavioural Biases in Investment Management: An Interdisciplinary Literature Review and Research Agenda." Organizations and Markets in Emerging Economies 12, no. 2 (2021): 459–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/omee.2021.12.65.

Full text
Abstract:

 
 
 Technological advancements bring continuous changes into the investment industry. The paper aims to provide insights on future research agenda based on a review of the current stance of research on the links between the Robo-advisors phenomenon and behavioural biases of individual investors. A qualitative investigation method has been applied for literature review on Robo-advisors and their impact on behavioural biases.
 The key findings indicate that Robo-advisors can help users to make better informed and less biased decisions. However, Robo-advisors activate the investors’ automatic system processes. The resulting passive investment approach could lead to alienation of the investors from the stock market, decreasing their understanding of the investment process that could widen a gap between different clusters of investors.
 The paper makes several contributions to the literature. First, it provides arguments on why a dual process theoretical framework in the relationship between financial advisory and investment behavioural biases is applicable. Second, it studies the Robo-advisor phenomenon and proposes a comprehensive definition of Robo-advisors. Third, the literature review suggests drivers of the Robo-advisors effect on the changes of behavioural biases as a future research direction.
 
 
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Hasan, Zahid, Daicy Vaz, Vidya S. Athota, Sop Sop Maturin Désiré, and Vijay Pereira. "Can Artificial Intelligence (AI) Manage Behavioural Biases Among Financial Planners?" Journal of Global Information Management 31, no. 2 (2023): 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/jgim.321728.

Full text
Abstract:
The main novelty of this paper is proposing artificial intelligence (AI) to manage behavioural biases in the financial decision-making process. An empirical study by Kahneman and Tversky identifies the evidence of behavioural biases in the investment decision-making process: a reversal of an established tenet in traditional finance. Financial planners are vulnerable to behavioural biases and are therefore unable to provide optimal investment solutions for their clients. Identifying the limitations of current practice, this research attempts to address how AI can help financial planners in subduing their behavioural biases and proposes the adoption of AI in financial planning services to circumvent behavioural biases. In recent years, AI has attained significant efficacy and has proven to be efficacious through supervised and unsupervised learning. Applying these AI techniques in mitigating behavioural biases, this study confirms that the backpropagation within the neural network and deep reinforcement learning can help overcome confirmation and hindsight biases.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

V, Ramalakshmi, Swathi B.V, and V. Renu Priya. "Behavioural Biases of Investors and its Impact." Journal of Advanced Research in Dynamical and Control Systems 11, no. 11-SPECIAL ISSUE (2019): 704–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.5373/jardcs/v11sp11/20193088.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Srinivasan, Kuppusamy, and Parthasarathy Karthikeyan. "Investigating self-efficacy and behavioural bias on investment decisions." E+M Ekonomie a Management 26, no. 4 (2023): 119–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.15240/tul/001/2023-4-008.

Full text
Abstract:
The determinants of irrational decisions on the stock market are found in numerous empirical studies. However, self-efficacy and behavioural biases have a sturdy influence on stock market investment decisions. Behavioural biases are formed with heuristics, prospect theory and herding effect concerning stock market investments. Self-efficacy is independent of behavioural biases but is closely connected with controlling behavioural intentions in decision-making. The research was conducted to find the influence of self-efficacy and behavioural biases in the decision of stock market investment. The study was conducted with 250 individual investors and applied the SEM technique. Findings indicated that heuristics had a positive relationship with behavioural biases, but behavioural biases reported a negative relationship with the herding effect and prospect theory. Heuristics were mostly developed on the intrinsic strength of individual investors; therefore, investors believe heuristics will be a better decision-making tool than prospect theory or the herding effect. Prospect theory is shaped and influenced by regret aversion, loss aversion, self-control and mental accounting. Financial literacy, risk tolerance, and peer support profoundly develop the self-efficacy of investors to make profitable investment decisions. Self-efficacy is formed by risk tolerance, financial literacy and peer support in the stock market investment decision and identified the evidence of individual investors not making rational decisions and facing one or more behavioural biases and self-efficacy factors. The study finds the combined effect of behavioural biases and self-efficacy in stock market investment decisions, which have significant implications among individual investors, particularly in emerging markets.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hidajat, Taofik. "BEHAVIOURAL BIASES IN BITCOIN TRADING." Fokus Ekonomi : Jurnal Ilmiah Ekonomi 14, no. 2 (2019): 337–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.34152/fe.14.2.337-354.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper aims to propose some behavioural biases of trading in Bitcoin. It is review literature in the areas of behavioural finance that address issues related to Bitcoin to underpin the conceptual model. A conceptual model for understanding the behavioural bias that affects investing in cryptocurrency is proposed. The biases are herding, optimism, overconfidence, confirmation bias, loss aversion, and gamblers’ fallacy. This paper ought to fill the research gap on cryptocurrency from the behavioral perspective. This paper implies that prices and Bitcoin transactions are more determined by psychological factors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Das, Poonam, Amit Kumar Das, and Sourav Acharjee. "A Bibliometric Analysis of Research in Behavioural Finance: Special Emphasis on Selected Behavioural Biases in Investment Decision Making." Colombo Business Journal 15, no. 2 (2024): 1–27. https://doi.org/10.4038/cbj.v15i2.174.

Full text
Abstract:
Behavioural finance combines psychology and economics to understand the anomalies of financial markets by examining human behaviour. There has been a significant global increase in research on behavioural biases and investment decisions. This paper aims to conduct a bibliometric analysis of behavioural biases and investment decisions using literature from the Scopus database for the period from 1991 to 2022 using the VOSviewer software. The research findings confirm a substantial growth in research on behavioural biases and investment decisions between 2008 and 2022. The United States is the most influential country based on citation count, and Odean (1998) stands out as the most prominent author in this research field. Co-citation analysis identifies key clusters in "efficient market, behavioural biases, and trading frequency," while bibliographic coupling highlights "Information processing and investors' behavioural biases." Co-occurrence analysis reveals gaps in research on financial literacy, personality traits, and optimism bias, suggesting future research opportunities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Yao, Songyao. "Behavioural biases and asset pricing." Thesis, University of Leeds, 2008. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.503246.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Foschi, Matteo. "Behavioural biases and contract theory." Thesis, University of Leicester, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/2381/37956.

Full text
Abstract:
This thesis studies how concepts of behavioural biases and bounded rationality affect classical results in contract theory and industrial organization. Chapter 2 studies the concept of naïveté (Strotz, 1956) in a principal agent model. Agents are assumed to be unaware of their true type, and formbiased (naïve) beliefs about it. The latter depend on the actual type of the agent. Results show how the information about agents’ true nature, that can be elicited from their beliefs, plays a crucial role in the principal’s optimal contracting strategy. In particular, the principal faces a trade-off between exploiting the agent with the most naïve beliefs and designing efficient contracts for the most widespread type of agent, according to her posteriors. Chapter 3 and Chapter 4 analyse models where agents suffer from temptation and self-control problems (à la Gul and Pesendorfer, 2001). Chapter 3 presents a new justification for loyalty schemes in the retailing industry. In the literature, loyalty schemes have been mostly studied as competition devices (Caminal and Claici, 2007) or as ways to increase consumers’ lifetime value (Caminal, 2012). This work focuses on how a seller can use loyalty schemes to acquire information about consumers’ preferences and gain the ability to perform individual pricing. Finally, Chapter 4 presents a two-period mechanism design problem with no commitment. It shows how the presence of consumers that suffer from self-control problems can explain the existence of entry bonuses paid by the seller to the consumer, regardless of whether the latter makes the purchase or not.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kolev, Gueorgui I. "Behavioural Biases and Chief Executive Officers Compensation." Doctoral thesis, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/7408.

Full text
Abstract:
Esta tesis consiste de tres ensayos. En el primero, documentamos la correlación imaginaria entre las decisiones de compensación de los ejecutivos (CEO) al demostrar que el hándicap de los ejecutivos que juegan al golf no está correlacionado con su desempeño en la empresa mientras que sí lo está con su compensación. Los golfistas ganan más que los que no juegan al golf, y las pagas se incrementan con la habilidad en este juego. En el segundo ensayo explicamos la reciente espiral de las compensaciones de los ejecutivos basados en el sesgo de atribución fundamental. El análisis de las series temporales agregadas y de datos de sección cruzada correspondiente a la burbuja del mercado accionario en los noventa sugiere que los accionistas exageran al atribuir las subidas y bajadas de los precios de las acciones corporativas a las aptitudes de liderazgo del ejecutivo mientras que subestiman el rol de las fluctuaciones del mercado accionario que se encuentran fuera del control de estos. En el tercer ensayo demostramos que un gran número de Ofertas Públicas Iniciales predice sistemáticamente, tanto dentro como fuera de la muestra, el subsiguiente bajo rendimientos agregado y ponderado, y la diferencia de rendimientos entre las pequeñas y grandes firmas.<br>This thesis consists of three essays. In the first, we document illusory correlation in CEO compensation decisions by demonstrating that golf handicaps of CEOs are uncorrelated with corporate performance, but related to CEO compensation. Golfers earn more than non-golfers and pay increases with golfing ability. In the second essay we propose a fundamental attribution bias-based explanation of the recent explosive growth in CEO pay. Analysis of aggregate time series data and cross sectional data from the late 1990s stock market bubble period suggests that shareholders overattribute prominent increases and decreases in the prices of corporate stocks to the leadership and skill of the CEOs and underestimate the role of stock market fluctuations that are beyond CEO control. In the third essay we show that increases in the number of Initial Public Offerings reliably predicts in-sample and out-of-sample decreases in subsequent equally weighted aggregate stock returns and the return differential between small and big firms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Grant, Joel. "Local futures traders and behavioural biases evidence from Australia /." Access electronically, 2007. http://www.library.uow.edu.au/adt-NWU/public/adt-NWU20080922.154750/index.html.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Large, Imogen. "Investigating neural structures and behavioural biases in perceptual decision-making." Thesis, University of Oxford, 2015. https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:1a107371-424b-4dfd-96e4-d851d801bec8.

Full text
Abstract:
Why do we behave as we do? The general mechanisms underlying visual perception and decision-making within a social context have been under scientific scrutiny for over half a century (Sherif, 1945; Asch, 1951; Berns et al, 2005). In spite of this, neither a definitive mechanism for how perceptual biases arise, nor a robust neural basis have emerged. In the first part of my thesis, I use a combination of visual behavioural testing and computational modelling to investigate the development of perceptual biases under social advice in children, and explore their potential mechanisms. In the second part, I use structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging in adults to examine the organisation of a cortical region that has been implicated in such visual perceptual decisions. First, I use a two-alternative forced choice visual task to probe the extent to which typically-developing children (between 6 and 14 years) conform to social advice, when making judgments about ambiguous and unambiguous structure-from-motion stimuli. Perceptual bias reported by typically-developing children with a single advisor was largely absent in the youngest children, but increases with age. When reaction time data were analysed with a drift diffusion model, the results suggested that the conforming bias of the older children was likely the product of a perceptual, not simply a decision, bias. The typically-developing children were then compared with age and IQ-matched children with autism. The autistic group showed a small conforming bias in decisions for the youngest children, while the effect of social advice decreased with age. Drift diffusion modelling showed no evidence of a perceptual bias, but rather a potential bias at later stages of processing, in the formation of the judgment. Second, I investigate how well visual area hMT+ can be mapped and characterised in individual human subjects. Visual cortical area hMT+ in humans is homologous to a macaque monkey region associated with perceptual decisions about the stimuli used in our visual psychophysics experiment with children (Krug 2004; Krug, 2013). By comparing two types of structural MRI scan sensitive to myelination (MP2RAGE at 7T and T1w/T2w at 3T) with functional localisers for hMT+, I found a consistent association of MRI contrasts indicating dense myelin with the functional localisers for group averaged datasets. However, regional patterns of myelination, and their association with functional hMT+, varied considerably between individual subjects. I went on to examine the internal functional organisation of area hMT+ with a high-resolution 7T fMRI scan. These scans provided evidence that in humans as in the monkey, cortical clusters respond selectively to specific stimulus aspects, such as specific directions of motion, or 3D depth. These behavioural experiments in conjunction with computational modelling suggest that advice given by others can bias perceptual decisions about visual stimuli. High field MRI of a cortical region implicated in these perceptual processes, visual area hMT+, indicate a columnar architecture that can be revealed using current technology. Combining the two approaches should allow us directly to probe the neural basis of perceptual changes under social influence in humans.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Tselika, Maria. "Behavioural Biases in Financial Markets : the Elusives Case of Hearding." Thesis, Nantes, 2020. http://www.theses.fr/2020NANT3015.

Full text
Abstract:
Suite à la mise en évidence de certains aspects cognitifs dans les prise de décision économique, plusieurs comportements collectifs en économie et en finance ont été identifiés. Le mimétisme sur les marchés boursiers est l'une de ces manifestations, défini comme la tendance des agents économiques à suivre leurs pairs, en ignorant parfois une information dont ils disposent. En examinant la littérature, cette thèse identifie une incohérence entre la conception théorique de mimétisme et son identification empirique. Les modèles empiriques sont divisés en deux catégories : ceux s'intéressant aux investisseurs (flux) et ceux examinant les rendements. Les modèles empiriques existants pour le mimétisme sur les rendements ne considèrent pas le mimétisme au niveau micro et certains facteurs importants, comme la variabilité des prix, qui affectent la robustesse de la méthodologie d'identification du mimétisme au niveau macro, basée sur les mesures de dispersion en coupe des rendements. En utilisant une combinaison de données relatives aux transactions boursières et aux rendements des actions boursières, cette thèse examine dans un premier temps le manque de cohérence des résultats empiriques sur le mimétisme au niveau micro par rapport à celui portant sur le consensus des rendements. Il est montré quele miméstime au niveau micro, sa direction (vente ou achat) et la composition du marché (types d'investisseurs) affectent le mécanisme de transmission du mimétisme vers le consensus sur les rendements. En outre, un examen méthodologique des mesures de dispersion en coupe des rendements des actions révèle la faiblesse des résultats empiriques existants en raison de l'existence d'une relation entre la volatilité réalisée et ces mêmes mesures de dispersion<br>The introduction of cognitive limitations and psychological barriers to human decision making lies behind the identification of collective behavior in economics and finance. Herding, is such a manifestation of mimetic behavior and is defined as the tendency of financial agents to follow their peers, even disregarding their personal information set. Through examination of the existing literature, this thesis identifies a discontinuity between the theoretical conception of herding and its empirical identification. Empirical models of herding are divided into herding in the level of investors and herding to the consensus of returns. Existing empirical models of herding to the consensus of returns, neither consider herding in micro-level nor account for important factors, such as price variability, that affect both the methodology developed to identify macro level herding, based on the measures of cross– sectional dispersion of returns, and the decision making process of financial agents. Using a combination of trading and stock related data, this thesis, initially examines the disagreement of empirical results of herding in the micro level compared to that of consensus of returns. Evidence that micro-level herding, the composition of the market in terms of investors and the direction of herding affect the mechanism of transmission of herding to the consensus of returns arise. Moreover, further methodological examination of the measures of dispersion of assets, reveals a relation between realized volatility and macro-level herding. This thesis encounters evidence that the existing methodologies for the identification of herding, do not consider the mechanism of transmission of herding from flows to returns and the methodological and conceptual importance of price variability
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Wang, Tongya. "Behavioural biases and evolutionary dynamics in an agent-based financial market." Thesis, University of Leeds, 2014. http://etheses.whiterose.ac.uk/7911/.

Full text
Abstract:
This research is devoted to the study of financial market dynamics in a framework which combines agent-based modelling and concepts from behavioural finance. The thesis explores, in an agent-based financial market model, the interlinkage between investor heterogeneity, bounded rationality, behavioural biases and the aggregate market dynamics. We develop a dynamic equilibrium model of a financial market in the presence of heterogeneous, boundedly rational investors. The model combines a performance-driven strategy-switching mechanism of an adaptive belief system (Brock and Hommes, 1998) and an evolutionary finance model (Evstigneev, Hens and Schenk-Hopp´e, 2011). A key feature of this new model is that it contains a combination of passive and active learning dynamics. Passive learning refers to the market force by which wealth accumulates on investment strategies which have done relatively well. Active learning refers to the switching behaviour by which investors actively move their wealth into strategies which have performed well in the recent or distant past. This thesis extends the literature by examining the joint effect of passive and active learning in relation to the evolutionary dynamics of financial markets. By drawing in concepts from behavioural finance, we focus on the micro-level modelling of various heuristics and behavioural biases which may affect investors’ active learning and financial forecasting, such as overconfidence, recency bias, sentiment, etc. We quantify the macro-level market impact of these behavioural elements and study the evolutionary prospects of market dynamics. We show that the interaction between passive and active learning is crucial to understanding the market selection of dominant strategy or the survival of different strategies. Investors’ bounded rationality and behavioural biases in active learning and financial forecasting play an important role in shaping the market dynamics. Our findings point to the causes of the persistence of market inefficiencies and a variety of stylised facts of financial market. The added value of drawing together agent-based modelling and behavioural finance on the study of financial markets dynamics is demonstrated.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kamwendo, Zara Thokozani. "Heuristics and biases to behavioural economics : a sociology of a psychology of error." Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/25831.

Full text
Abstract:
This thesis is a sociological history of the making of behavioural economics. Behavioural economics is a discipline in which economists draw on psychological knowledge and approaches to understand economic behaviour. The narrative begins with the lives and work of psychologists Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky in the newly established state of Israel. It then moves from the making of the so called Heuristics and Biases Programme in the 1970’s to the privately funded Behavioural Economics Program in the USA in the 1980’s. Using a blend of analysis of archival documents, published material, and interviews I seek to understand the formation of the discipline of behavioural economics by applying the notion of a psychology)of)error as an analytical tool. The small number of historians who have studied behavioural economics have all identified a concern with human error as a crucial element of its intellectual makeup. I take this observation further by arguing that both Kahneman and Tversky’s Heuristics and Biases Programme and behavioural economics are psychologies) of) error because the object to be explained in both fields was restricted to behavioural deviations from a normative core. In the case of Heuristics and Biases that normative core consisted of a blend of statistical and logical norms imported from traditional decision theory about what constituted rational decision making. In the case of behavioural economics the normative core was made up of assumptions about rational economic behaviour developed by neo-classical economists. Understanding behavioural economics as a psychology of error allows me to shed light on the complicated relationship between behavioural economics and neo-classical economics. Specifically it helps explain how behavioural economists sought to strike a careful balance between critiquing the descriptive claims of neo-classical economists and reinforcing their normative ambitions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Pearce, Graeme. "Biases and discrimination : an economic analysis using lab and field experiments." Thesis, University of Exeter, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10871/23749.

Full text
Abstract:
This thesis uses laboratory and field experiments to examine the underlying motivations that drive biased and discriminatory behaviour. Its focus is on the differential treatment of others that stems from individuals’ preferences for particular social and ethnic groups. The unifying theme of this thesis is the exploration of how such discriminatory tastes can manifest themselves within individuals’ social and other–regarding preferences, determining the extent to which they care about the welfare of others. The prevalence and implications of these types of preferences are considered in both market and non–market settings.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Muller, Stacey Leigh. "The impact of internal behavioural decision-making biases on South African collective investment scheme performance." Thesis, Rhodes University, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10962/d1020308.

Full text
Abstract:
Market efficiency, based on people acting rationally, has been the dominating finance theory for most of the 20th and 21st Century’s. This classical finance theory is based on assumptions that people are rational, they absorb all available information and maximise utility. This view is outdated; it has been shown that people are in fact irrational and that this could be the cause of anomalies in the market. Behavioural finance takes into account people, and their natural biases. Behavioural finance has integrated classical financial theories and psychological theories to illustrate the way in which irrational people can impact market efficiency. This research looks at the way collective investment scheme manager decision-making can impact market efficiency. Specifically the behavioural biases: overconfidence, over optimism, loss aversion and frame dependence and whether or not collective investment scheme performance is affected by these. This research was carried out using a questionnaire distributed directly to CIS managers and risk-adjusted returns were used in order to allow for comparative results. The results from the questionnaire show evidence that actively managing South African CIS managers do indeed suffer from overconfidence and loss aversion and they do not appear to suffer from frame dependence or over optimism in this research context. There was also evidence showing that managers who suffer from these biases also demonstrated lower investment returns. “The investor’s chief problem, and even his worst enemy, is likely to be himself.” - Benjamin Graham
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Paull, Gillian. Dynamic labour market behaviour in the British household panel survey: The effects of recall bias and panel attrition. Institute of Economics and Statistics, University of Oxford, 1997.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Venezia, Itzhak. Behavioural Finance: Where Do Investors' Biases Come From? World Scientific Publishing Co Pte Ltd, 2016.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases: An Analysis of Bias in Corporate Finance and Financial Planning. Palgrave Macmillan, 2017.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases: An Analysis of Bias in Corporate Finance and Financial Planning. Palgrave Macmillan, 2019.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

The choice factory: How 25 behavioural biases influence the products we decide to buy. 2018.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Clark, Gordon L. Behaviour in Context. Edited by Gordon L. Clark, Maryann P. Feldman, Meric S. Gertler, and Dariusz Wójcik. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198755609.013.10.

Full text
Abstract:
The behavioural revolution has profoundly affected how we conceptualize behaviour. The rational agent of standard microeconomic theory has been found wanting and, in its place, new formulations have been presented which take seriously human traits like myopia and loss aversion. Here it is argued that the behavioural revolution offers a way of understanding common problems in economic geography, such as co-location, clusters of innovation, the diffusion of innovation, and home bias. It is noted that earlier versions of behaviouralism stressed bounded rationality but underestimated the far-reaching consequences of the behavioural revolution. To explain the significance of these developments for understanding the intersection between cognition and context, we look closely at behaviour in time and space. The implications of behaviouralism for institutions are briefly considered, emphasizing the role that collective action in or through institutions can play in ameliorating the adverse effects of behavioural biases and anomalies.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Stokes, Mark, and John Duncan. Dynamic Brain States for Preparatory Attention and Working Memory. Edited by Anna C. (Kia) Nobre and Sabine Kastner. Oxford University Press, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.013.032.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter considers how dynamic brain states continuously fine-tune processing to accommodate changes in behavioural context and task goals. First, the authors review the extant literature suggesting that content-specific patterns of preparatory activity bias competitive processing in visual cortex to favour behaviourally relevant input. Next, they consider how higher-level brain areas might provide a top-down attentional signal for modulating baseline visual activity. Extensive evidence suggests that working memory representations in prefrontal cortex are especially important for generating and maintaining biases in preparatory visual activity via modulatory feedback. Although it is often proposed that such working memory representations are maintained via persistent prefrontal activity, the authors review more recent evidence that rapid short-term synaptic plasticity provides a common substrate for maintaining the content of past experience and the rules for guiding future goal-directed processing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Mevorach, Irit. The Debiasing Role of the Cross-Border Insolvency System. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198782896.003.0002.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter explores what the reasons for deviating from modified universalism in practice may be. To do so, it draws on behavioural international law and economics. The chapter argues that certain decision-making biases may play a role in cross-border insolvency and can explain both negative inclinations and instances of lack of cooperation, as well as the relative success of modified universalism. The key argument here is that instead of yielding to territorial inclinations, cross-border insolvency law has a debiasing role to play. It should attempt to align choices with optimal solutions, overcoming biases, and should also close gaps in the cross-border insolvency system in line with modified universalism.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Mevorach, Irit. The Future of Cross-Border Insolvency. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198782896.001.0001.

Full text
Abstract:
This book interrogates the current cross-border insolvency regime and sets out a pattern to improve its future. In recent decades, and especially since the global financial crisis, a number of important initiatives have focused on developing effective solutions for managing the insolvency of multinational enterprises and financial institutions. This book takes stock of the varying success of previous policy, and identifies the gaps and biases that could be bridged by employing a range of strategies. The book first sets out the theoretical debates regarding cross-border insolvency and surveys the strengths and weaknesses of the prevailing method, ‘modified universalism’, synthesizing divergences into a rubric for both commercial entities and financial institutions. Adhering to these norms more robustly, the book argues, would enhance global welfare and produce the best outcomes for businesses and institutions. Drawing upon sources from international law as well as behavioural and economic theory, the book considers how to translate modified universalism into binding international law, how to choose the right instrument for cross-border insolvency, the impact instrument design has on decisions and choices, and the means to encourage compliance. In particular, the book proposes measures that could potentially overcome, or at least take into account, behavioural biases in decision-making in order to create a system that works for businesses, and offers a blueprint for the future of cross-border insolvency.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Book chapters on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Avi-Yonah, Reuven S., and Kaijie Wu. "Behavioral biases and political actors." In Behavioural Public Finance. Routledge, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781351107372-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Coleman, Les. "Behavioural Biases in Investor Decisions." In Applied Investment Theory. Springer International Publishing, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-43976-1_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Sarkar, Debashis. "The biases in problem-solving." In Behavioural Science for Quality and Continuous Improvement. Routledge, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003250517-13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "Other Biases in the Behavioural Finance Literature." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "The Rise and Rise of Behavioural Finance." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "The Rise and Fall of Neoclassical Finance." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "Overconfidence and Self-Serving Bias." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "Loss Aversion Bias, the Disposition Effect and Representativeness Bias." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "Recent Developments." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Moosa, Imad A., and Vikash Ramiah. "Epilogue." In The Financial Consequences of Behavioural Biases. Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-69389-7_7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Kalchenko, Valeriya. "INFLUENCE OF MODERN MASS MEDIA ON MORAL AND SOCIAL COGNITION: HOW KREMLIN'S PROPAGANDA BECAME ONE OF THE MAIN CAUSES OF THE RUSSIA-UKRAINE WAR, 2014-2024." In 11th SWS International Scientific Conferences on SOCIAL SCIENCES - ISCSS 2024. SGEM WORLD SCIENCE, 2024. https://doi.org/10.35603/sws.iscss.2024/s07/41.

Full text
Abstract:
Propaganda was historically a neutral descriptive term; it was used by playwrights in ancient Greece to express the act of promoting political and religious beliefs. In the 19th century, with the birth of the press, cinema and radio, propaganda transformed into a tool destined to shape people�s beliefs and behaviours. It is generally biased, misleading, or even false to promote a specific agenda or point of view. Especially in Europe, in the first half of the 20th century, propaganda was used by European dictatorial regimes: Nazism, Fascism and Communism for popular consensus building and political repression; and it�s still widely used nowadays for the same proposition by current autocratic regimes around the world. The main purpose of this article is to demonstrate how Kremlin propaganda is used thanks to modern mass communication methods and technologies to influence the social and moral cognition of Russian society to start and continue the 10 year-long war against Ukraine, all now in progress. In this study we demonstrate how the Russian propaganda also as one of main causes of war itself. Because to start a war it is necessary to have first the consent of one�s own people who accept, support and justify the invasion of another country and here the main role is played by propaganda of Putin�s political regime. Well-thought-out Kremlin�s propaganda has constantly incited different fears in individuals through using persuasive neurolinguistic techniques: in information texts, in social networks, and especially on TV; by doing so over the past 20 years has turned at least 80% of Russian society into people who exist in a parallel reality and seem to have no critical thinking. The phenomenon of propaganda and its social and moral damage is analysed using quantitative, qualitative, analytical and descriptive research methods. Furthermore, thanks to this research, we are trying to understand if and how it will be possible to effectively counter the Kremlin�s propaganda machine. The paper�s results suggest communicative and social tools that could change public opinion in Russia and spread the truth about the war. Unfortunately, this would only be possible after the end of the war and the transition of the Kremlin dictatorial regime to a democratic one. Future research may sometimes attempt to identify the most effective means and strategies to achieve this goal, hopefully to be able to implement these soon.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kumar, Satish, and Nisha Goyal. "EXPLORING BEHAVIOURAL BIASES AMONG INDIAN INVESTORS: A QUALITATIVE INQUIRY." In 45th International Academic Conference, London. International Institute of Social and Economic Sciences, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.20472/iac.2019.045.024.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Pilli, Stephen. "Exploring Conversational Agents as an Effective Tool for Measuring Cognitive Biases in Decision-Making." In 2023 10th International Conference on Behavioural and Social Computing (BESC). IEEE, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/besc59560.2023.10386809.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

"Behavioural Biases on Residential House Purchase Decisions: A Multi-Criteria Decision-Making Approach." In 20th Annual European Real Estate Society Conference: ERES Conference 2013. ÖKK-Editions, Vienna, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.15396/eres2013_220.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Candraningrat, Ica Rika, and A. Sakir. "Behavioural Biases of Overconfidence and Disposition Effect and their Impact on Investment Decision in the Indonesian Capital Market." In Proceedings of the 2019 International Conference on Organizational Innovation (ICOI 2019). Atlantis Press, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.2991/icoi-19.2019.20.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Ryan, Paul, and Clare Branigan. "Behavioural Biases in the Acquisition of Multiple Properties by Owner Occupier Investors during the Irish Residential Real Estate Bubble." In 22nd Annual European Real Estate Society Conference. European Real Estate Society, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.15396/eres2015_187.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Ryan, Stephen, and Matthew Richardson. "Behavioural biases among real estate investment decision makers: Has anyone seen my neo-cortex? I'm sure I left it here somewhere." In 22nd Annual European Real Estate Society Conference. European Real Estate Society, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.15396/eres2015_105.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Sturgis, Raphael, Valentin Emiya, Basile Couetoux, and Pierre Garreau. "Vessel Behaviour Classification from AIS Without Geographical Biases." In 2022 IEEE 25th International Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (ITSC). IEEE, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/itsc55140.2022.9921946.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Stephens, Christopher, Dagmara Wrzecionkowska, Estefanía Espitia-Bautista, Roland Díaz-Loving, and Gabriela Contreras. "The Conductome – A New Paradigm for Understanding Human Behaviour." In International Association of Cross Cultural Psychology Congress. International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.4087/lgnw9526.

Full text
Abstract:
As almost every major problem that humankind faces is a consequence of human behaviour, predicting behaviour and behaviour change is fundamental. Given the multitude of factors that affect our decision making, a transdisciplinary understanding of behaviour is impossible without the integration of data that crosses disciplinary boundaries. The concept of Conduct-“ome” is an analog of those holistic –“omic”-approaches found in the biological sciences which take a “totality of factors” approach, and provides a framework for studying human behaviour in a multifactorial, multidisciplinary context, accounting for a wealth of potential causes of behaviour, from the genetic and epigenetic to psychological, neurological, social, physiological, clinical, socio-economic, socio-demographic, socio-political and ethical factors. Conductome, as opposed to behaviour-ome, is used, as it directly addresses the “whys” (causes) of the considered behaviour. We argue that behaviour can only be understood probabilistically, through a process of statistical inference that constructs P(A|X), the probability for a conduct A conditioned on the large set of factors, X, that predict it. This inference process can be based on an “external” ensemble of objective, countable events, using a frequentist interpretation of probabilities, or on an “internal” ensemble, implicit in our mental models and based on a Bayesian interpretation. Including both these approaches allows one to compare objective, observable reality with the subjective perception of reality constructed within a mental model, allowing for the identification of discrepancies between the two in the form of cognitive biases. A key component for constructing the Conductome is the obtention of data that transcends disciplines that can be used to link a range of relevant behaviours as internal and external effects to their causes. A second component is the use of advanced modelling tools, such as machine learning, for the analysis of such multi-scale data and the construction of explicit prediction models for a given conduct. In this article, the feasibility of the Conductome approach is illustrated by considering obesity-related behaviours; as obesity has become one of the key social problems that affects a growing segment of the population worldwide. In summary: The objective is to understand, interpret and provide an interdisciplinary, computational, and data-based framework for generating prediction models for addressing problems that originate in human behaviour.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Azad, Ehsan M., James J. Bell, Roberto Quaglia, Jorge J. Moreno Rubio, and Paul J. Tasker. "Gate Bias Incorporation into Cardiff Behavioural Modelling Formulation." In 2020 IEEE/MTT-S International Microwave Symposium (IMS). IEEE, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/ims30576.2020.9223817.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Behavioural biases"

1

Dasgupta, Anuttama, and Smitha N. Capacity Development Forum 2024 Proceedings. Indian Institute for Human Settlements, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.24943/cdf10.2024.

Full text
Abstract:
The Capacity Development Forum (CDF) is an initiative of the Indian Institute for Human Settlements (IIHS) that aims to bring together diverse stakeholders involved in capacity development in India into a ‘community of practice’ to consolidate learnings from across the country and around the world into a strong and value-added network to consolidate learnings across the country and from around the world. The longer-term objective of the forum is to collaborate on Capacity Development practices and create a repository of knowledge on Capacity Development. The third convening of the IIHS Capacity Development Forum was held on 27-28 June 2024 in a hybrid format – at IIHS, Bengaluru City Campus and online, on the theme: ‘Capacity Development and Behavioural Insights – Implications for Institutional Design and Public Policy’. The CDF 2024 focussed on how the understanding of human behaviour and motivations is key to achieving larger objectives not only at an individual level, but also within institutions and societies. The panels brought together stakeholders from a range of sectors to discuss how nudges and incentives can be used to influence human behaviour at various scales, and how this in turn informs the design, formulation and implementation of policies and programmes to make them more effective. Day 1 of the convening comprised panels that brought together capacity development practitioners from the public and private sectors, to discuss various aspects of policy capacity and enhancing it through incorporating behavioural insights, international perspectives on the role of Capacity Development agencies in developing behavioural science capacity of public officials, and the role of leadership in organisational behaviour change. This was followed by a learning hub on Day 2 focussing on cases from practice incorporating behavioural insights from different sectors, and a game-based learning workshop incorporating behavioural insights and illustrating various biases in human behaviour.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Michaud, Pierre-Carl, and Pascal St-Amour. Longevity, Health and Housing Risks Management in Retirement. CIRANO, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.54932/rnkf5751.

Full text
Abstract:
Annuities, long-term care insurance and reverse mortgages remain unpopular to manage longevity, medical and housing price risks after retirement. We analyze low demand using a life-cycle model structurally estimated with a unique stated-preference survey experiment of Canadian households. Low risk aversion, substitution between housing and consumption and low marginal utility when in poor health explain most of the reduced demand. Bequests motives are found to be a luxury good and play a limited role. The remaining disinterest is explained by information frictions and behavioural status-quo biases. We find evidence of strong spousal co-insurance motives motivating LTCI and of responsiveness to bundling with a near doubling of demand for annuities when reverse mortgages can be used to annuitize, instead of consuming home equity
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Ó Ceallaigh, Diarmaid, Lucie Martin, Shane Timmons, Deirdre Robertson, and Pete Lunn. The response of low-income households to the cost-of-living crisis in Ireland. ESRI, 2024. https://doi.org/10.26504/rs206.

Full text
Abstract:
The rise in inflation in recent years triggered a cost-of-living crisis, in which households’ ability to afford basic necessities declined. The cost-of-living crisis disproportionately affected lower income households, with the proportion in material deprivation (i.e. unable to afford basic necessities) rising by a fifth. This study examines how these households responded to this economic pressure and the challenges they faced in accessing government benefits and available support services during the cost-of-living crisis. In the absence of detailed longitudinal data, the study employed a cross-sectional survey with techniques from behavioural science to improve recall and limit biases in responses. A nationally representative sample of 1,615 financial decision-makers from low- (i.e. below ~85 per cent of the median) income households undertook the study between May and June 2024. Most were recruited from the online panels of market research companies and completed the study online, but a subset was recruited in-person in low-income areas. Participants reported on changes they made in their day-to-day spending, borrowing and saving due to the cost-of-living crisis, described as the period following the Russian invasion of Ukraine in 2022. They also reported on their use of government benefits and support services during the cost-of-living crisis, with a focus on administrative burdens (i.e. the time, effort, and frustration involved and their potential role as barriers to using supports).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

James, Richard J. E., Hyungseo Kim, Lucy Hitcham, and Richard J. Tunney. Causal inference methods in gambling research. Greo Evidence Insights, 2023. https://doi.org/10.33684/2024.004.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this project was to review and utilise methods from other disciplines in the social sciences in order to be able make stronger causal claims using crosssectional gambling data such as gambling prevalence studies. We focused on the question of whether there is a causal relationship between specific gambling products and individual gambling harms, specifically problem gambling. There has been an existing literature that has looked at this issue, but fails to control for selection biases on engagement with specific gambling behaviours. We reviewed and used three approaches: propensity score matching, coarsened exact matching, and sample selection modelling to account for this limitation. Then, we applied them to examine the relationship between 8 types of gambling activity (online betting, offline betting, pools, scratchcards, lotteries, pools, offline bingo, and slot machines/FOBTs) on problem gambling in 9 British gambling prevalence surveys: the Health Survey for England in 2012, 2015, 2016 and 2016; the Scottish Health Survey in 2012, 2015, and 2016, and the British Gambling Prevalence Study in 2007 and 2010. The results showed most gambling activities were, unsurprisingly, associated with an increased risk of addictive behaviour. However, there was a clear gradient of risk. For some gambling activities (i.e., online gambling, slots), the effect sizes were substantially higher than others (i.e., lotteries). The modelling also highlighted covariates that exert strong effects on both the IV and DV of interest at the same time, such as age. The findings of this project thus highlight the importance of controlling for selection mechanisms that influence both engagement and outcomes of interest. There are some activities (e.g., bingo, pools) where these substantially affect relationships between engagement and harms. Nonetheless, there is clear evidence of associations between certain types of gambling product and indicators of harm when these are controlled for.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Blanchard, Alexander, and Laura Bruun. Bias in Military Artificial Intelligence. Stockholm International Peace Research Institute, 2024. https://doi.org/10.55163/cjft9557.

Full text
Abstract:
To support states involved in the policy debate on military artificial intelligence (AI), this background paper provides a deeper examination of the issue of bias in military AI. Three insights arise. First, policymakers could usefully develop an account of bias in military AI that captures shared concern around unfairness. If so, ‘bias in military AI’ might be taken to refer to the systemically skewed performance of a military AI system that leads to unjustifiably different behaviours—which may perpetuate or exacerbate harmful or discriminatory outcomes—depending on such social characteristics as race, gender and class. Second, among the many sources of bias in military AI, three broad categories are prominent: bias in society; bias in data processing and algorithm development; and bias in use. Third, bias in military AI can have various humanitarian consequences depending on context and use. These range from misidentifying people and objects in targeting decisions to generating flawed assessments of humanitarian needs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Paull, Gillian. Dynamic Labour Market Behaviour in the British Household Panel Survey: The Effects of Recall Bias and Panel Attrition. The IFS, 1997. http://dx.doi.org/10.1920/re.ifs.2024.0864.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Schulz, Jan, Daniel Mayerhoffer, and Anna Gebhard. A Network-Based Explanation of Perceived Inequality. Otto-Friedrich-Universität, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.20378/irb-49393.

Full text
Abstract:
Across income groups and countries, the public perception of economic inequality and many other macroeconomic variables such as inflation or unemployment rates is spectacularly wrong. These misperceptions have far-reaching consequences, as it is perceived inequality, not actual inequality informing redistributive preferences. The prevalence of this phenomenon is independent of social class and welfare regime, which suggests the existence of a common mechanism behind public perceptions. We propose a network-based explanation of perceived inequality building on recent advances in random geometric graph theory. The literature has identified several stylised facts on how individual perceptions respond to actual inequality and how these biases vary systematically along the income distribution. Our generating mechanism can replicate all of them simultaneously. It also produces social networks that exhibit salient features of real-world networks; namely, they cannot be statistically distinguished from small-world networks, testifying to the robustness of our approach. Our results, therefore, suggest that homophilic segregation is a promising candidate to explain inequality perceptions with strong implications for theories of consumption behaviour.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Maffioli, Alessandro, and Bronwyn H. Hall. Evaluating the Impact of Technology Development Funds in Emerging Economies: Evidence from Latin-America. Inter-American Development Bank, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.18235/0011145.

Full text
Abstract:
This working paper surveys impact evaluations of government Technology Development Funds (TDFs) in Argentina, Brazil, Chile and Panama. All the evaluations were done at the recipient (firm) level using data from innovation surveys, industrial surveys, and administrative records of the granting units, together with quasi-experimental econometric techniques to minimize the effects of any selection bias. The surveyed evaluations considered four levels of potential impact: R&amp;D input additionality, behavioural additionality, increases in innovative output, and improvements in performance. The evidence suggests that TDFs do not crowd out private investment and that they positively affect R&amp;D intensity. In addition, participation in TDFs induces a more proactive attitude of beneficiary firms towards innovation activities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lunn, Pete, Marek Bohacek, Jason Somerville, Áine Ní Choisdealbha, and Féidhlim McGowan. PRICE Lab: An Investigation of Consumers’ Capabilities with Complex Products. ESRI, 2016. https://doi.org/10.26504/bkmnext306.

Full text
Abstract:
Executive Summary This report describes a series of experiments carried out by PRICE Lab, a research programme at the Economic and Social Research Institute (ESRI) jointly funded by the Central Bank of Ireland, the Commission for Energy Regulation, the Competition and Consumer Protection Commission and the Commission for Communications Regulation. The experiments were conducted with samples of Irish consumers aged 18-70 years and were designed to answer the following general research question: At what point do products become too complex for consumers to choose accurately between the good ones and the bad ones? BACKGROUND AND METHODS PRICE Lab represents a departure from traditional methods employed for economic research in Ireland. It belongs to the rapidly expanding area of ‘behavioural economics’, which is the application of psychological insights to economic analysis. In recent years, behavioural economics has developed novel methods and generated many new findings, especially in relation to the choices made by consumers. These scientific advances have implications both for economics and for policy. They suggest that consumers often do not make decisions in the way that economists have traditionally assumed. The findings show that consumers have limited capacity for attending to and processing information and that they are prone to systematic biases, all of which may lead to disadvantageous choices. In short, consumers may make costly mistakes. Research has indeed documented that in several key consumer markets, including financial services, utilities and telecommunications, many consumers struggle to choose the best products for themselves. It is often argued that these markets involve ‘complex’ products. The obvious question that arises is whether consumer policy can be used to help them to make better choices when faced with complex products. Policies are more likely to be successful where they are informed by an accurate understanding of how real consumers make decisions between products. To provide evidence for consumer policy, PRICE Lab has developed a method for measuring the accuracy with which consumers make choices, using techniques adapted from the scientific study of human perception. The method allows researchers to measure how reliably consumers can distinguish a good deal from a bad one. A good deal is defined here as one where the product is more valuable than the price paid. In other words, it offers good value for money or, in the jargon of economics, offers the consumer a ‘surplus’. Conversely, a bad deal offers poor value for money, providing no (or a negative) surplus. PRICE Lab’s main experimental method, which we call the ‘Surplus Identification’ (S-ID) task, allows researchers to measure how accurately consumers can spot a surplus and whether they are prone to systematic biases. Most importantly, the S-ID task can be used to study how the accuracy of consumers’ decisions changes as the type of product changes. For the experiments we report here, samples of consumers arrived at the ESRI one at a time and spent approximately one hour doing the S-ID task with different kinds of products, which were displayed on a computer screen. They had to learn to judge the value of one or more products against prices and were then tested for accuracy. As well as people’s intrinsic motivation to do well when their performance on a task like this is tested, we provided an incentive: one in every ten consumers who attended PRICE Lab won a prize, based on their performance. Across a series of these experiments, we were able to test how the accuracy of consumers’ decisions was affected by the number and nature of the product’s characteristics, or ‘attributes’, which they had to take into account in order to distinguish good deals from bad ones. In other words, we were able to study what exactly makes for a ‘complex’ product, in the sense that consumers find it difficult to choose good deals. FINDINGS Overall, across all ten experiments described in this report, we found that consumers’ judgements of the value of products against prices were surprisingly inaccurate. Even when the product was simple, meaning that it consisted of just one clearly perceptible attribute (e.g. the product was worth more when it was larger), consumers required a surplus of around 16-26 per cent of the total price range in order to be able to judge accurately that a deal was a good one rather than a bad one. Put another way, when most people have to map a characteristic of a product onto a range of prices, they are able to distinguish at best between five and seven levels of value (e.g. five levels might be thought of as equivalent to ‘very bad’, ‘bad’, ‘average’, ‘good’, ‘very good’). Furthermore, we found that judgements of products against prices were not only imprecise, but systematically biased. Consumers generally overestimated what products at the top end of the range were worth and underestimated what products at the bottom end of the range were worth, typically by as much as 10-15 per cent and sometimes more. We then systematically increased the complexity of the products, first by adding more attributes, so that the consumers had to take into account, two, three, then four different characteristics of the product simultaneously. One product might be good on attribute A, not so good on attribute B and available at just above the xii | PRICE Lab: An Investigation of Consumers’ Capabilities with Complex Products average price; another might be very good on A, middling on B, but relatively expensive. Each time the consumer’s task was to judge whether the deal was good or bad. We would then add complexity by introducing attribute C, then attribute D, and so on. Thus, consumers had to negotiate multiple trade-offs. Performance deteriorated quite rapidly once multiple attributes were in play. Even the best performers could not integrate all of the product information efficiently – they became substantially more likely to make mistakes. Once people had to consider four product characteristics simultaneously, all of which contributed equally to the monetary value of the product, a surplus of more than half the price range was required for them to identify a good deal reliably. This was a fundamental finding of the present experiments: once consumers had to take into account more than two or three different factors simultaneously their ability to distinguish good and bad deals became strikingly imprecise. This finding therefore offered a clear answer to our primary research question: a product might be considered ‘complex’ once consumers must take into account more than two or three factors simultaneously in order to judge whether a deal is good or bad. Most of the experiments conducted after we obtained these strong initial findings were designed to test whether consumers could improve on this level of performance, perhaps for certain types of products or with sufficient practice, or whether the performance limits uncovered were likely to apply across many different types of product. An examination of individual differences revealed that some people were significantly better than others at judging good deals from bad ones. However the differences were not large in comparison to the overall effects recorded; everyone tested struggled once there were more than two or three product attributes to contend with. People with high levels of numeracy and educational attainment performed slightly better than those without, but the improvement was small. We also found that both the high level of imprecision and systematic bias were not reduced substantially by giving people substantial practice and opportunities to learn – any improvements were slow and incremental. A series of experiments was also designed to test whether consumers’ capability was different depending on the type of product attribute. In our initial experiments the characteristics of the products were all visual (e.g., size, fineness of texture, etc.). We then performed similar experiments where the relevant product information was supplied as numbers (e.g., percentages, amounts) or in categories (e.g., Type A, Rating D, Brand X), to see whether performance might improve. This question is important, as most financial and contractual information is supplied to consumers in a numeric or categorical form. The results showed clearly that the type of product information did not matter for the level of imprecision and bias in consumers’ decisions – the results were essentially the same whether the product attributes were visual, numeric or categorical. What continued to drive performance was how many characteristics the consumer had to judge simultaneously. Thus, our findings were not the result of people failing to perceive or take in information accurately. Rather, the limiting factor in consumers’ capability was how many different factors they had to weigh against each other at the same time. In most of our experiments the characteristics of the product and its monetary value were related by a one-to-one mapping; each extra unit of an attribute added the same amount of monetary value. In other words, the relationships were all linear. Because other findings in behavioural economics suggest that consumers might struggle more with non-linear relationships, we designed experiments to test them. For example, the monetary value of a product might increase more when the amount of one attribute moves from very low to low, than when it moves from high to very high. We found that this made no difference to either the imprecision or bias in consumers’ decisions provided that the relationship was monotonic (i.e. the direction of the relationship was consistent, so that more or less of the attribute always meant more or less monetary value respectively). When the relationship involved a turning point (i.e. more of the attribute meant higher monetary value but only up to a certain point, after which more of the attribute meant less value) consumers’ judgements were more imprecise still. Finally, we tested whether familiarity with the type of product improved performance. In most of the experiments we intentionally used products that were new to the experimental participants. This was done to ensure experimental control and so that we could monitor learning. In the final experiment reported here, we used two familiar products (Dublin houses and residential broadband packages) and tested whether consumers could distinguish good deals from bad deals any better among these familiar products than they could among products that they had never seen before, but which had the same number and type of attributes and price range. We found that consumers’ performance was the same for these familiar products as for unfamiliar ones. Again, what primarily determined the amount of imprecision and bias in consumers’ judgments was the number of attributes that they had to balance against each other, regardless of whether these were familiar or novel. POLICY IMPLICATIONS There is a menu of consumer polices designed to assist consumers in negotiating complex products. A review, including international examples, is given in the main body of the report. The primary aim is often to simplify the consumer’s task. Potential policies, versions of which already exist in various forms and which cover a spectrum of interventionist strength, might include: the provision and endorsement of independent, transparent price comparison websites and other choice engines (e.g. mobile applications, decision software); the provision of high quality independent consumer advice; ‘mandated simplification’, whereby regulations stipulate that providers must present product information in a simplified and standardised format specifically determined by regulation; and more strident interventions such as devising and enforcing prescriptive rules and regulations in relation to permissible product descriptions, product features or price structures. The present findings have implications for such policies. However, while the experimental findings have implications for policy, it needs to be borne in mind that the evidence supplied here is only one factor in determining whether any given intervention in markets is likely to be beneficial. The findings imply that consumers are likely to struggle to choose well in markets with products consisting of multiple important attributes that must all be factored in when making a choice. Interventions that reduce this kind of complexity for consumers may therefore be beneficial, but nothing in the present research addresses the potential costs of such interventions, or how providers are likely to respond to them. The findings are also general in nature and are intended to give insights into consumer choices across markets. There are likely to be additional factors specific to certain markets that need to be considered in any analysis of the costs and benefits of a potential policy change. Most importantly, the policy implications discussed here are not specific to Ireland or to any particular product market. Furthermore, they should not be read as criticisms of existing regulatory regimes, which already go to some lengths in assisting consumers to deal with complex products. Ireland currently has extensive regulations designed to protect consumers, both in general and in specific markets, descriptions of which can be found in Section 9.1 of the main report. Nevertheless, the experiments described here do offer relevant guidance for future policy designs. For instance, they imply that while policies that make it easier for consumers to switch providers may be necessary to encourage active consumers, they may not be sufficient, especially in markets where products are complex. In order for consumers to benefit, policies that help them to identify better deals reliably may also be required, given the scale of inaccuracy in consumers’ decisions that we record in this report when products have multiple important attributes. Where policies are designed to assist consumer decisions, the present findings imply quite severe limits in relation to the volume of information consumers can simultaneously take into account. Good impartial Executive Summary | xv consumer advice may limit the volume of information and focus on ensuring that the most important product attributes are recognised by consumers. The findings also have implications for the role of competition. While consumers may obtain substantial potential benefits from competition, their capabilities when faced with more complex products are likely to reduce such benefits. Pressure from competition requires sufficient numbers of consumers to spot and exploit better value offerings. Given our results, providers with larger market shares may face incentives to increase the complexity of products in an effort to dampen competitive pressure and generate more market power. Where marketing or pricing practices result in prices or attributes with multiple components, our findings imply that consumer choices are likely to become less accurate. Policymakers must of course be careful in determining whether such practices amount to legitimate innovations with potential consumer benefit. Yet there is a genuine danger that spurious complexity can be generated that confuses consumers and protects market power. The results described here provide backing for the promotion and/or provision by policymakers of high-quality independent choice engines, including but not limited to price comparison sites, especially in circumstances where the number of relevant product attributes is high. A longer discussion of the potential benefits and caveats associated with such policies is contained in the main body of the report. Mandated simplification policies are gaining in popularity internationally. Examples include limiting the number of tariffs a single energy company can offer or standardising health insurance products, both of which are designed to simplify the comparisons between prices and/or product attributes. The present research has some implications for what might make a good mandate. Consumer decisions are likely to be improved where a mandate brings to the consumer’s attention the most important product attributes at the point of decision. The present results offer guidance with respect to how many key attributes consumers are able simultaneously to trade off, with implications for the design of standardised disclosures. While bearing in mind the potential for imposing costs, the results also suggest benefits to compulsory ‘meta-attributes’ (such as APRs, energy ratings, total costs, etc.), which may help consumers to integrate otherwise separate sources of information. FUTURE RESEARCH The experiments described here were designed to produce findings that generalise across multiple product markets. However, in addition to the results outlined in this report, the work has resulted in new experimental methods that can be applied to more specific consumer policy issues. This is possible because the methods generate experimental measures of the accuracy of consumers’ decision-making. As such, they can be adapted to assess the quality of consumers’ decisions in relation to specific products, pricing and marketing practices. Work is underway in PRICE Lab that applies these methods to issues in specific markets, including those for personal loans, energy and mobile phones.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Schultz, Timothy, Michael Zhou, Jodi Gray, et al. Patient characteristics and interventions associated with complaints and medico-legal claims. The Sax Institute, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.57022/lioq6047.

Full text
Abstract:
There is anecdotal evidence that the rate of complaints and claims against doctors is rising, yet little is known about (Q1) which types of patients are more likely to make a complaint or claim, nor about (Q2) what interventions are effective in reducing rates of complaints and claims and increasing patient satisfaction. This Evidence Check aimed to answer those two questions. The evidence base for both of the questions was of low quality, with only five studies having a comparison group. Twenty-five studies addressed Question 1. The only patient characteristic to have a consistent effect on rates of complaints and claims was having a mental, behavioural or developmental disorder. Other patient characteristics, including those related to their therapeutic context, had inconsistent or weak relationships with rates of complaints and claims. Twenty studies addressed Question 2 (including one which addressed both questions). There were consistently reduced rates of complaints and claims following implementation of risk management programs and also implementation of communication and resolution programs. Peer feedback programs consistently improved doctors’ response to complaints and subsequent performance. However, the results found here should be interpreted with caution, as the risk of bias inherent in the study designs makes them more likely to erroneously demonstrate an effect.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!