Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Cerebrale Asymmetrie'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Cerebrale Asymmetrie.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Debener, Stefan. "Individuelle Unterschiede in der frontalen EEG-Alphaasymmetrie: Emotionalität und intraindividuelle Veränderungen." Doctoral thesis, Saechsische Landesbibliothek- Staats- und Universitaetsbibliothek Dresden, 2001. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:swb:14-1000888669859-38345.
Full textPetzold, Antje. "Lateralized Head Turning Bias in Humans – Cues to the Development of Human Cerebral Asymmetries." Bachelor's thesis, Saechsische Landesbibliothek- Staats- und Universitaetsbibliothek Dresden, 2009. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:14-qucosa-24971.
Full textHausmann, Markus. "Steroidmodulation funktioneller cerebraler Asymmetrien." [S.l.] : [s.n.], 2000. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?idn=960030581.
Full textNicholls, Michael E. R. "Cerebral asymmetries for temporal resolution." Thesis, University of Cambridge, 1993. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.309007.
Full textElias, Lorin J. "Cerebral asymmetries in processing language and time." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1999. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk3/ftp04/nq38236.pdf.
Full textKingery, Lisle R. "The Psychological Correlates of Asymmetric Cerebral Activation." Fogler Library, University of Maine, 2003. http://www.library.umaine.edu/theses/pdf/KingeryLR2003.pdf.
Full textBroks, Paul. "Hemisphere asymmetries in schizophrenia." Thesis, University of Oxford, 1987. http://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:d724a3b5-fce5-4ae0-9db7-a583b625754a.
Full textClark, Gina M. "Cerebral asymmetry in adolescent onset psychosis." Thesis, University of Oxford, 2006. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.442813.
Full textDann, P. L. "Cerebral asymmetry and individual differences in reading." Thesis, University of Plymouth, 1988. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.383783.
Full textLaMendola, Nicholas Paul. "Cerebral asymmetries for radial-maze foraging in the rat." Diss., The University of Arizona, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/290146.
Full textJohnstone, Leah T. "Hemispheric asymmetries : behavioural, kinematic, and electrophysiological predictors of cerebral organisation." Thesis, Bangor University, 2016. https://research.bangor.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/hemispheric-asymmetries-behavioural-kinematic-and-electrophysiological-predictors-of-cerebral-organisation(1fcdf175-059d-4647-8577-8044e365e14c).html.
Full textKilian, Annette. "Funktionelle cerebrale Asymmetrien visueller Prozesse und numerische Fähigkeiten beim Grossen Tümmler (Tursiops truncatus)." [S.l.] : [s.n.], 2004. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?idn=972544941.
Full textNajt, Pablo. "Functional cerebral asymmetries of emotional processes in the healthy and bipolar brain." Thesis, Durham University, 2013. http://etheses.dur.ac.uk/6949/.
Full textJernberg, Grönlund Annika. "Posturala asymmetrier och smärta hos barn med cerebral pares : En populationsbaserad registerstudie." Thesis, Umeå universitet, Avdelningen för fysioterapi, 2020. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-167463.
Full textDemakis, George J. "Functional cerebral asymmetry : a test of the selective activational model /." Thesis, This resource online, 1992. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-12232009-020403/.
Full textChen, Jing, and 陈静. "Distinction between nonconscious and conscious vision : evidence from hemispheric asymmetry effects." Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10722/206426.
Full textpublished_or_final_version
Psychology
Master
Master of Philosophy
Wilcox, Gary A. (Gary Alden). "Handedness, Perceptual and Short Term Memory Asymmetries, and Personality." Thesis, North Texas State University, 1985. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc331119/.
Full textMoffat, Scott Douglas. "Relations between testosterone, neuroanatomical structure and functional cerebral asymmetry in humans." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1998. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk2/tape15/PQDD_0003/NQ31089.pdf.
Full textAlves, Nelson Torro. ""Percepção de expressões faciais da emoção e lateralização cerebral"." Universidade de São Paulo, 2004. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-14092005-140244/.
Full textThe role that each brain hemisphere plays in the processing of emotional information has been frequently discussed in the scientific literature. The aim of this study was to investigate the pattern of hemispheric dominance for the perception of the facial expressions of happiness, sadness, anger and fear. In two experiments the divided-visualfield technique was used with the taquitoscopic presentation of stimuli on a computer screen for 150 ms. The stimuli were composed with pictures of faces of four people (2M,2F) taken from the series Pictures of Facial Affect. Twenty one right-handed observers (9M,12F) took part in the experiment I. In each trial two pictures of faces were presented on the computer screen, one of them placed on the left side and the other one on the right side of the fixation point, in four different conditions: 1) face with emotion on the left and neutral face on the right, 2) neutral face on the left and face with emotion on the right, 3)face with emotion on the right and on the left, 4) neutral face on the right and on the left. In each trial the observers determined the side on which the face seemed to show greater emotional intensity. Seventeen right-handed observers (8M, 9F) took part in the experiment II. In each stimulus presentation, a picture of a face was presented either on the right or left side of the fixation point, placed on the center of the screen and, on the opposite side, a gray rectangle was presented. The following stimuli conditions were elaborated: 1) face with emotion on the left and gray rectangle on the right, 2) gray rectangle on the left and face with emotion on the right, 3) neutral face on the left and gray rectangle on the right, 4) gray rectangle on the left and neutral face on the right. In each trial the observers determined if the face presented had emotion or not. Time reactions and judgement errors were submitted to ANOVAs for repeated measures. In the first experiment, emotion was generally detected more quickly in faces presented on the left 10 visual field (p<0,01). The expressions of sadness and anger were also perceived more quickly when presented on the left visual field (p<0,05). In both experiments, expressions of happiness and fear were perceived more quickly and more accurately than expressions of sadness and anger (p<0,001). The expression of sadness was detected more easily in feminine faces and the expression of anger in masculine faces (p<0,05). In general, however, the emotion was detected more easily in feminine faces. In both experiments there were differences in the perception between the faces of the four individuals that represented the facial expressions. The right hemisphere showed superior to the left hemisphere in the perception of facial expressions, especially for the perception of expressions of sadness and anger. The perceptive advantage of the right hemisphere is more evident for the expressions that are detected with more difficulty. The perception of facial expressions can be affected by the gender of the face and the singularities of the individual facial expression.
Crews, William David Jr. "Cerebral asymmetry in facial affect perception of women : neuropsychological effects of depression /." This resource online, 1992. http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/theses/available/etd-09052009-040440/.
Full textCrews, William David. "Cerebral asymmetry in facial affect perception of women: neuropsychological effects of depression." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/44557.
Full textKornisch, Myriam. "Estimates of functional cerebral hemispheric differences in monolingual and bilingual people who stutter." Thesis, University of Canterbury. Communication Disorders, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10092/10739.
Full textEverhart, Daniel Erik Jr. "Cerebral Regulation of Cardiovascular Functioning and Fluency among Anxious and Nonanxious Men." Diss., Virginia Tech, 1997. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/30497.
Full textPh. D.
Santos, Monica Cena de Sousa. "Hemisferectomia na vida adulta como modelo para o estudo das assimetrias cerebrais no controle da reatividade emocional e atividade locomotora em camundongos." Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, 2013. http://www.bdtd.uerj.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=9326.
Full textEvidence exists indicating that cerebral lateralization is a fundamental feature of all vertebrates. In humans, it has been suggested that the right hemisphere is involved in processing negative emotional information and the left hemisphere is involved in control of motor function. In rodents, evidence for hemispheric lateralization is sparse. In this regard, here, we used unilateral hemispherectomy to study the relative importance of each hemisphere in controlling emotion and spontaneous motor activity in mice. Adult male mice were submitted to right hemispherectomy (RH), left hemispherectomy (LH) or sham surgery (SHAM). To help the interpretation of results an addition sample of mice was submitted to unilateral suction of left frontoparietal area (LFP), right frontoparietal area (RFP) or sham surgery (CONT). Fifteen days after surgery, both the emotional reactivity and the spontaneous locomotor activity were assessed in the open field test 10 min (divided into 1 min intervals). The open field arena consisted of a polypropylene box in which the floor was divided into 16 same-sized rectangles. The total number of rectangles crossed was used as the measure of spontaneous locomotor activity. Considering that mice avoid open areas, the ambulation in the center and time spent in the central rectangles were used to assess emotional reactivity. Regarding locomotor activity, the two surgical techniques reveal asymmetries in opposite direction. The locomotor activity of LH, which increased along the test period, was higher than both RH and SHAM. In contrast, the locomotor activity of RFP which decreased along the test period was higher than both LFP and CONT. Regarding emotional reactivity, the LH group spent less time in the central area than both RH and SHAM groups. No differences were observed between LFP, RFP and CONT groups. Our results suggest that the two hemispheres contribute asymmetrically to control of emotional reactivity and to control of motor activity in mice. Similarly to what is observed in humans, the right hemisphere of mice was more involved in processing negative emotional information. Regarding locomotor hyperactivity, the differences observed between hemispherectomized and frontoparietal-lesioned mice suggest that multiple lateralized circuits (or systems) may underlie spontaneous locomotor activity.
Flindall, Jason. "Manual asymmetries in the kinematics of reach-to-grasp actions." Thesis, Lethbridge, Alta. : University of Lethbridge, Dept. of Kinesiology and Physical Education, c2012, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/10133/3359.
Full textxiv, 121 leaves : ill. ; 29 cm
Demaree, Heath Allan. "Analysis of Quantitative Electroencephalographic and Cardiovascular Responses to Stress Amoung Low- and High-Hostiles." Diss., Virginia Tech, 1997. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/30334.
Full textPh. D.
Shenal, Brian Vincent. "The Dynamic Cerebral Laterality Effect: Group Differences in Hostility, Cardiovascular Regulation, and Sensory Recognition." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/36661.
Full textMaster of Science
Comer, Clinton S. "Cerebral Laterality, Emotion, and Cardiopulmonary Functions: An Investigation of Left and Right CVA Patients." Diss., Virginia Tech, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/56981.
Full textPh. D.
Barrick, Thomas R. "Automatic image analysis methods for quantification of cerebral asymmetry : investigation of sexual dimorphism and schizophrenia." Thesis, University of Liverpool, 2004. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.399212.
Full textShore, Robert. "A functional characterisation of the PCSK6 locus associated with handedness." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/15719.
Full textCarr, Vaughan. "Electrodermal indices of information processing and functional cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia : a comparison with affective disorder /." Title page, table of contents and summary only, 1987. http://web4.library.adelaide.edu.au/theses/09MD/09mdc312.pdf.
Full textDorsaint-Pierre, Raquel. "Revisiting the function-structure polemic : examining the relationship between language lateralization and the neuroanatomical asymmetries in Heschl's gyrus, the planum temporale, and Broca's area." Thesis, McGill University, 2005. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=85153.
Full textWilliamson, John Bonar. "Differential Efficts of Hostility on Frontal Lobe Performance: A dual task approach with Fluency and Cardiovascular Regulation." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/9779.
Full textMaster of Science
Rodriguez, Lina Maria Perilla. "Efeitos de bandas de frequência espacial alta e baixa no reconhecimento de faces em campo visual lateralizado." Universidade de São Paulo, 2008. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-11092013-112028/.
Full textThis study was made with the objective of investigate the effects of high and low spatial frequency bands in face recognition in lateralized visual field. 40 participants were randomly distributed in two groups. The volunteers viewed fourteen non-filtered pictures of faces until they managed to memorize them. After that, fifty six spatial frequency filtered pictures of faces were presented randomly interspersed with pictures of faces previously showed. Each one of them was exhibited in the screen for three hundred milliseconds using the dichotic presentation procedure. The participant should answer whether the face presented belonged to the group of pictures initially viewed. The frequency of responses allowed to calculate the ROC (Receiver Operating Characteristic) Curves and the Az and da parameters praised by the Signal Detection Theory (Macmillan; Creelman, 2005) for natural faces, low spatial frequency composed faces and high spatial frequency composed faces. Results showed that original faces were better recognized than faces with high spatial frequencies (HSF) and low spatial frequencies (LSF). Differently from literature, the left hemisphere was more accurate than the right to recognize faces, regardless of the filter condition. The RH was equivalent to the LH to process LSF, but worse than the LH to process HSF. Concerning the performance of the genders, men and women judged faces in a very similar way when they used the RH. The performance of women agreed with the FE hypothesis, being faces with HSF recognized better than faces with LSF. Men, even using the LH, were more accurate to recognize faces with LSF than HSF.
Burghy, Cory A. "Family environmental risk, frontal brain asymmetry, and social-emotional functioning for children living in poverty." Laramie, Wyo. : University of Wyoming, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1317326181&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=18949&RQT=309&VName=PQD.
Full textAlves, Nelson Torro. "Assimetria cerebral na percepção de expressões faciais de valência positiva e negativa." Universidade de São Paulo, 2008. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-20092013-115902/.
Full textThe divided visual field technique was used to analize the patterns of brain asymmetry in the perception of positive and negative facial expressions. Eighty undergraduate students (65 female, 15 male) were distributed in five experimental groups in order to investigate separately the perception of expressions of happiness, surprise, fear, sadness, and neutral. In each trial, a target and a distractor expression were presented in a computer screen during 150 ms and participants had to determine the side (left or right) on which the target expression was presented using a mouse to respond to the stimuli. Time reaction and judgment errors analysis showed no differences between men and womens performance in experimental task. Results indicated that expressions of happiness and fear were identified faster when presented on the left visual field, suggesting an advantage of the right hemisphere in the perception of these expressions. Fewer judgment errors and faster reaction times were observed for the matching condition in which emotional faces were presented on the left visual field and neutral faces on the right visual field. Analysis of the pairs of faces indicated that neutral and happy faces were perceived faster and more accurately than faces of fear and happiness. Although an advantage of the right hemisphere was not occurred for the perception of all expressions, results tend to support the right hemisphere hypothesis for emotional processing.
Shtyrov, Yury. "New aspects of the cerebral functional asymmetry in speech processing as revealed by auditory cortex evoked magnetic fields." Helsinki : University of Helsinki, 2000. http://ethesis.helsinki.fi/julkaisut/hum/psyko/vk/shtyrov/.
Full textHiggins, Dane A. "Cardiovascular Reactivity to Speech Processing and Cold Pressor Stress: Evidence for Sex Differences in Dynamic Functional Cerebral Laterality." Thesis, Virginia Tech, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10919/35146.
Full textMaster of Science
Júnior, Rui de Moraes. "Reconhecimento de faces com filtragens de frequências espaciais altas e baixas nos hemicampos visuais direito e esquerdo." Universidade de São Paulo, 2012. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-17012013-211718/.
Full textThis study aimed to investigate whether face recognition occurs primarily by analytic or holistic processing in the cerebral hemispheres of men and women through the study of the spatial frequency spectrum. Therefore, in Experiment I, 40 volunteers (20 women) performed two sessions. In each of, 14 faces were memorized for a recognition task and each face was presented for 300 ms. Images were presented only in the right visual hemifield in a session, and in another only in the left visual hemifield by means of an adaptation of the method of divided visual field. The participants task was to assign the reliability of their response (confidence rating method) to discriminate the study faces from distractors. The recognition task stimuli were presented in three conditions: (1) at high spatial frequencies, FEAs, (2) at low spatial frequencies, FEBs, and (3) unfiltered, SFE. The frequencies of responses to the degree of reliability used to calculate ROC curves and parameters Az and da of the Signal Detection Theory compared the performance of face recognition in different bands of the spectrum. In a complementary way, an ANOVA was conducted to test response times differences in the recognition between filtering. There was no evidence of hemispheric specialization in face recognition with spatial filtering. But men had better performance in recognizing faces in FEBs and women faces in FEAs. To verify that this result was not in function of lateralized presentation, Experiment II was conducted, along the lines of an experimental session of Experiment I, but with central presentation. Twenty volunteers (10 women) participated in the experiment. Again, men and women were more sensitive to faces in FEBs and FEAs, respectively. Thus it follows that men use holistic resources and women analytical operations. The results provide no basis for the occurrence of hemispheric specialization of spatial frequencies in face recognition over long exposure times. The sex difference observed brings us to the need for control sample by sex in spatial frequency research.
Rodling, Wahlström Marie. "Severe cerebral emergency : aspects of treatment and outcome in the intensive care patient." Doctoral thesis, Umeå universitet, Anestesiologi och intensivvård, 2009. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-21065.
Full textBranco, Danielle Paes Machado de Andrade. "Hemisferectomia unilateral na vida adulta como modelo para o estudo das assimetrias motoras em camundongos." Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, 2009. http://www.bdtd.uerj.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=4685.
Full textA series of studies in humans have suggested that the planning and the control of movements are asymmetrically performed by the hemispheres. In general, lesions in the left hemisphere promote motor deficits that are more pronounced than similar lesions in the right hemisphere. In the present work, we used unilateral hemispherectomy to study the relative importance of each hemisphere in the control of movement in mice. Adult Swiss mice were submitted to right unilateral hemispherectomy (RH) or left unilateral hemispherectomy (LH) and sham surgery. Fifteen days after surgery, the motor coordination of each mouse was evaluated in the forced locomotion in rotating cylinder test (Rotarod). The latency to fall was significantly lower in right-hemispherectomized group as compared to control and did not differ from the left-hemispherectomized group. To help the interpretation of results obtained in the Rotarod test, a part of the sample was subjected to a additional battery of behavioral tests in the following sequence: open field test, qualitative assessment of sensory-motor asymmetry, foot fault test and tail suspension test. Interestingly, in the foot fault test, while the RH group showed significantly worse performance with the left hind limb (contralateral to the lesion) than with the right, the control group and the LH group showed no differences between both hind limbs. Similarly to what is observed in humans, our results suggest that the two hemispheres contribute asymmetrically to control of movement in mice.
Castells, Navarro Laura, W. Southwell-Wright, Keith Manchester, and Jo Buckberry. "Interpretation of a probable case of poliomyelitis in the Romano-British social context." Archaeological reviews from Cambridge, 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/10454/17511.
Full textThis paper provides the results of re-evaluation of a young adult individual from the Romano-British cemetery of 76 Kingsholm, Gloucester with club foot defomity by (Roberts et al 2004). Our reanalysis revealed an extensive bilateral asymmetry involving the lower and upper limb, spine and cranium and a right scoliosis, indicating more than the lower limb was affected. Consideration of the position and shape of the articulated club foot indicated a positional rather than a developmental condition, probably due to unilateral paralysis. Differential diagnosis considered congenital and acquired neuromuscular conditions; we argue that poliomyelitis is the most likely cause. Poliomyelitis is secondary to the infection with poliovirus that can affect the motor neurons from the spinal cord, causing a flaccid paralysis without sensory affection. Because the virus affects individual nerves, the paralysis is muscle-specific causing muscle imbalances and poor posture which can result in deformities and muscle disuse atrophy. Shortening of the leg is the most characteristic sign, however other typical deformations are in the lower limbs are external rotation of the knee, knee hyperestension, ankle and foot deformities (all observed in K131). The evaluation of K131’s entheses and bone structure suggests that, in life, this individual showed physical deformities consisting of a possibly visible atrophy of the left arm and leg, asymmetric gait, clubfoot and slight scoliosis which would have affected not only his appearance but also his ability to move and perform certain tasks. K131’s burial treatment is entirely normative for the period and the wider cemetery context. This could suggest that despite their impairment, this individual was not necessarily marginalised within their social context. However, historical sources account for extensive marginalisation and cruel treatment of the disabled and deformed in this period. So, whilst K131 was buried in a normative manner, it is difficult to reach definitive conclusions regarding how this individual was treated by their contemporaries.
Westergren, Amanda, and Tim Kammeborn. "Vänster- eller högerriktat ljus i reklamfilm, finns det en preferens? : En experimentell studie om ljussättningens laterala riktning i reklamfilm." Thesis, Högskolan Dalarna, Bildproduktion, 2019. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:du-30568.
Full textSousa, Bruno Marinho de. "Assimetria cerebral funcional e sua relação com a excentricidade no campo visual nos tamanhos percebidos em fundos sem e com gradiente de textura." Universidade de São Paulo, 2013. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-02112013-121640/.
Full textFunctional brain asymmetry (FBA) studies point out that there are differences between left (LH) and right (HD) brain hemispheres. LH is more specialized for processing language while HD for processing spatial information. Still, there may be a size overestimation in left visual field (LVF) compared to the right visual field (RVF).But men perform better on spatial tasks using LVF/RH, while women perform equivalently in each brain hemisphere. Also, there is evidence that men are less sensitive to stimuli context than women. However, it is not clear how the shape of the stimulus, variation of its distance from the center of the screen (eccentricity) and a texture gradient can affect FBA. Based on this, the aim of this study was to verify if eccentricity variation can influence size perception of two types of stimuli in LVF and RVF in men and women (Experiment I). Also (Experiment II) we investigate if a texture gradient can influence a possible effect observed in Experiment I. In both experiments we used the divided visual field technique associated with the method of constant stimuli (30 repetitions) with two alternative forced choice (\"what is the bigger?\"). Stimuli in Experiment I were circles (one standard and seven for comparison) and horizontal ellipses (one standard and seven for comparison). These stimuli were counterbalanced in both visual hemifields. Stimuli were presented at three eccentricities (2.5°, 5° and 7.5 °) for 100 ms on a gray background to right-handers (10 men and 10 women by stimulus type). In Experiment II circles were presented at 2.5° in a texture gradient (divided vertically and with same depth information on LVF and RVF), for 10 right-handed women. From data we calculated Relative Errors and psychometric slopes - discriminative sensitivity. Results of Experiment I show that relative error mean for 2.5° in RVF circles was greater than 5° and 7.5°. Women showed an overestimation for circles presented in RVF at 2.5° eccentricity. Slope coefficients were greater for 2.5 ° eccentricity and for circles. Men showed a difference in slope coefficients for eccentricity variation, with 2.5° mean greater than 5 ° and 7.5°. Women had higher coefficients for circles than ellipses. Mean circles coefficients were greater at 2.5° than 7.5° eccentricities. Mean ellipses coefficients were greater at 2.5° in general and in men. Men also showed a difference in RVF in which coefficients were greater at 2.5° than 7.5°. Experiment II showed only a difference for relative errors in comparison with Experiment I. These results show that FBA is not an absolute effect, but rely on stimuli characteristics and different strategies for men and women in the task. Although there are differences in discriminative sensitivity, they did not result in a perceptual distortion. This suggests that for FBA not only perceptual parameters should be analyzed, but also the psychometric slope and discrimination sensitivity. Furthermore, the effect of the gradient texture overlaps the FBA.
Prieur, Jacques. "Chimpanzees' and gorillas' intraspecific gestural laterality : a multifactorial investigation." Thesis, Rennes 1, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015REN1S056/document.
Full textWe studied intraspecific gestural laterality of captive chimpanzees and gorillas in real-life social-ecological relevant contexts. We evidenced that chimpanzees (respectively gorillas) exhibited a right-hand bias at the population level for the majority of the most frequent gestures of their specific natural communication repertoire. By designing and applying a multifactorial approach, we showed for the first time that intraspecific gestural laterality of primates was influenced by several factors and their mutual intertwinement: interactional context (visual fields of both signaller and recipient as well as emotional context), gesture characteristic (sensory modality, use of a communication tool, sharing degree, and gesture duration) and by some socio-demographic components in particular signaller’s hierarchical rank, and to a lesser extent signaller’s age. Similarities but also some discrepancies between chimpanzees and gorillas may be related to the lateralization of emotional processing, to communication strategies, and to social selection pressures related to the social structure and dynamics of the study species. Moreover, we compared manual laterality of tool use by chimpanzees in both non-communication actions and intraspecific gestures. Our multifactorial analysis showed that tool-use in gestures appear to be governed more by the left cerebral hemisphere than tool-use in non-communication actions. Our findings support Ghirlanda and colleagues’ (2009) model postulating that population-level bias could be explained by an evolutionary stable strategy based on intraspecific interactions. Our results also agree with previous reports evidencing predominant right-hand use by nonhuman primates for gestural communication and suggesting that gestural laterality would be a precursor of the left-brain specialization for language. Furthermore, our results support the hypothesis that some primate species may have a specific left-cerebral system processing gestures distinct from the cerebral system processing non-communication manual actions. From an evolutionary point of view, our findings emphasize the importance to study intraspecific laterality in detail by considering species varying in their degree of sociality and taking into account real-life social-ecological contexts and multiple potentially influential factors
Llorente, Espino Miquel. "Lateralidad Manual y Especialización Hemisférica en Chimpancés (Pan Troglodytes). Evaluación Observacional y Experimental." Doctoral thesis, Universitat Ramon Llull, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/9283.
Full textLas asimetrías funcionales manuales han sido ampliamente estudiadas en los primates no humanos y otras especies animales durante las últimas dos décadas. Resulta especialmente interesante observar si la especialización hemisférica cerebral, tan característica del ser humano, está o no presente y en qué grado dentro del reino animal. En chimpancés, encontramos pocos trabajos en los que se hayan evaluado las preferencias manuales en una única muestra desde un punto de vista observacional y experimental conjuntamente, y ninguno en ambientes intermedios (naturalizados). Nuestro objetivo ha sido comparar los patrones de preferencia manual en situaciones espontáneas y experimentales en una misma muestra de chimpancés y ver qué situaciones inciden sobre la dirección, intensidad y grado de asimetría individual versus poblacional en los individuos. En la fase observacional se estudiaron 53 comportamientos espontáneos unimanuales y bimanuales que hacían referencia a contextos tróficos, manipulativos-exploratorios, autodirigidos, posturales y sociales, entre otros. En la fase experimental evaluaron dos tareas: una unimanual (simple reaching: recogida de ítems pequeños de alimento con prensiones de precisión) y otra bimanual (tube task: extracción bimanual de alimento de un tubo). Durante la fase observacional se llevaron a cabo 2226 sesiones (2005 a 2008: 33 meses). La fase experimental tuvo una duración total de 4 meses (2007). La muestra estaba formada por un grupo de 14 chimpancés (Pan troglodytes) alojados en el Centro de Recuperación de Primates de Fundación Mona (Girona). En ambas fases se utilizó un muestreo focal con registro activado por transiciones. Globalmente, tanto durante la fase observacional como experimental, la mayor parte de la muestra presentó preferencias individuales laterales de manera significativa. Se compararon los índices de lateralidad para las cuatro condiciones de estudio: unimanual-espontáneo (UES), bimanual-espontáneo (BES), unimanual-experimental (UEX) y bimanual-experimental (BEX). No se encontraron diferencias en la dirección de las preferencias entre las cuatro condiciones aunque sí en la intensidad de la lateralización. De esta manera, la intensidad de la preferencia manual era mayor en las acciones bimanuales que en las unimanuales, y también en las tareas experimentales que en las espontáneas. El uso de instrumentos y el tipo de prensión también aumentaron la intensidad de las preferencias aunque no tuvieron ningún efecto sobre la dirección de éstas. Se detectó una asimetría diestra poblacional en las acciones bimanuales espontáneas aunque no en las experimentales. También se detectó una asimetría diestra a nivel poblacional al tener en cuenta todas las tareas conjuntamente. De esta manera, ha sido la primera ocasión que se evidencia que los chimpancés alojados en ambientes intermedios son diestros a nivel poblacional. En conclusión, los chimpancés exhibirían un patrón de lateralización homólogo al de los seres humanos, sobre todo en aquellas acciones que implican una complejidad en su ejecución (bimanualidad, instrumentalidad y precisión) y que son las más relevantes para entender el proceso de hominización conductual de este rasgo en Homo sapiens.
Manual functional asymmetries have been extensively studied in nonhuman primates and other animals for the last two decades. It is particularly interesting to see if brain hemispheric specialization, so characteristic human beings, is present or not and to what extent within the animal kingdom. In chimpanzees, there are few studies that have assessed hand preferences in a single sample from an experimental and observational standpoint, and none have been done within intermediate (naturalized) environments. Our objective was to compare the patterns of hand preference for spontaneous and experimental actions in the same sample of chimpanzees and observe what situations impact on the direction, strength and the degree of individual versus population asymmetry in individuals. In the observational phase, we studied 53 spontaneous unimanual and bimanual behaviors related to feeding contexts, manipulative-exploratory, self-directed, postural and social, among others. In the experimental phase, we evaluated two tasks: unimanual (simple reaching: collection of small items of food with precision grips) and bimanual (tube task: bimanual removal of food from a tube). During the observational phase, we conducted 2.226 sessions (from 2005 to 2008: 33 months). The experimental phase lasted a total of four months (2007). The sample was composed of a group of 14 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) housed at the Mona Foundation Primate Rehabilitation Center (Girona). In both phases, a focal sampling method was used. Overall, during both observational and experimental phases, the majority of the sample showed significant individual lateral preferences. We compared the handedness index for four study conditions: unimanual-spontaneous (UES), bimanual-spontaneous (BES), unimanual-experimental (UEX) and bimanual-experimental (BEX). There were no differences in the direction of preferences among the four conditions, but there were differences in the strength of lateralization. The strength of hand preference was higher in bimanual than in unimanual ones, and also in the experimental tasks than in the spontaneous ones. The use of tools and the type of grip also increased the strength of preferences, but had no effect on the direction of such preferences. We detected right-handedness at population-level for spontaneous bimanual actions, but not for the experimental ones. There was also a right asymmetry at the population level when considering all the tasks together. In this way, this was the first evidence that chimpanzees housed in an intermediate environment demonstrate right-handedness at population-level. In conclusion, chimpanzees exhibit a pattern of lateralization homologous to human beings, especially those actions that involve a complexity of execution (bimanual, tool use and precision grip) and that are most relevant for understanding the behavioral process of hominization of this trait in Homo sapiens.
Walker, Kenneth N. (Kenneth Neal). "Differential Effects of Biofeedback Input on Lowering Frontalis Electromyographic Levels in Right and Left Handers." Thesis, University of North Texas, 1990. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc331405/.
Full textSchmitz, Rémy. "Asymétries hémisphériques cérébrales dans la pseudonégligence, l'induction de faux souvenirs et l'apprentissage implicite: une approche cognitive et neuropsychologique." Doctoral thesis, Universite Libre de Bruxelles, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/2013/ULB-DIPOT:oai:dipot.ulb.ac.be:2013/209788.
Full textPetzold, Antje. "Lateralized Head Turning Bias in Humans – Cues to the Development of Human Cerebral Asymmetries." Bachelor's thesis, 2002. https://slub.qucosa.de/id/qucosa%3A629.
Full textLürken, Alexander [Verfasser]. "Funktionelle Asymmetrien und cerebrale Asymmetrien kortikaler Aktiviertheit / vorgelegt von: Alexander Lürken." 2004. http://d-nb.info/971725675/34.
Full text