Academic literature on the topic 'Chimpanzees Locomotion Uganda Budongo Forest'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Chimpanzees Locomotion Uganda Budongo Forest.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Chimpanzees Locomotion Uganda Budongo Forest"

1

Plumptre, A. J., and V. Reynolds. "Censusing chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda." International Journal of Primatology 17, no. 1 (February 1996): 85–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02696160.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Tweheyo, Mnason, and Kare A. Lye. "Patterns of frugivory of the Budongo Forest chimpanzees, Uganda." African Journal of Ecology 43, no. 4 (December 2005): 282–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2028.2005.00566.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E. "The diet of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda." African Journal of Ecology 37, no. 3 (September 1999): 344–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2028.1999.00186.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Arnold, Kate, and Andrew Whiten. "POST-CONFLICT BEHAVIOUR OF WILD CHIMPANZEES (PAN TROGLODYTES SCHWEINFURTHII) IN THE BUDONGO FOREST, UGANDA." Behaviour 138, no. 5 (2001): 649–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853901316924520.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractSince de Waal & van Roosmalen (1979) first documented the occurrence of reconciliation between former opponents in captive chimpanzees, the study of the post-conflict behaviour of primates has provided valuable information about some of the details of primate social organisation. The vast majority of these studies have been carried out on captive subjects and it has been assumed that these findings are representative of wild primates. We set out to investigate whether this was true for the Sonso community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Budongo Forest, Uganda, using controlled procedures comparable with those used in captive studies. We found that these chimpanzees were much less likely to reconcile than their captive counterparts. Only one dimension of relationship quality had an effect on the likelihood of reconciliation. Individuals which were highly compatible, in terms of time spent affiliating, reconciled conflicts more often than those with weak relationships. Captive chimpanzees have also been shown to 'console' one another (de Waal & van Roosmalen, 1979; de Waal & Aureli, 1996), where uninvolved bystanders initiate affiliative contacts with victims of aggression. This study did not confirm that consolatory behaviour was characteristic of wild chimpanzee post-conflict behaviour. Nor did these chimpanzees use explicit gestures during post-conflict interactions as they have been shown to do in two out of three captive studies. We conclude that the post-conflict behaviour of chimpanzees is more variable than has previously been thought and is likely to be dependent on the prevailing social environment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

McLennan, Matthew R., David Hyeroba, Caroline Asiimwe, Vernon Reynolds, and Janette Wallis. "Chimpanzees in mantraps: lethal crop protection and conservation in Uganda." Oryx 46, no. 4 (October 2012): 598–603. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0030605312000592.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractA main concern of farmers worldwide is how to reduce crop losses to wildlife. Some potentially lethal crop protection methods are non-selective. It is important to understand the impact of such methods on species of conservation concern. Uganda has important populations of Endangered eastern chimpanzees Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Farmers sometimes use large metal mantraps to guard their fields against crop-raiding wildlife, particularly baboons Papio anubis and wild pigs Potamochoerus sp.. Chimpanzees that range onto farmland also step in these illegal devices and without rapid veterinary invention face severe injury or eventual death. Unlike inadvertent snaring of great apes in African forests, the problem of mantraps in forest–farm ecotones has received little attention. We report 10 cases of entrapped chimpanzees in the cultivated landscape surrounding Uganda's Budongo Forest during 2007–2011, undoubtedly only a portion of the actual number of cases. Mantraps currently present a substantial threat to ape populations in this important conservation landscape. Our data underscore the need for conservation programmes to consider the techniques used by rural farmers to protect their livelihoods from wild animals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E. "Termite eating and food sharing by male chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda." African Journal of Ecology 37, no. 3 (September 1999): 369–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2028.1999.00187.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Emery Thompson, M., N. E. Newton-Fisher, and V. Reynolds. "Probable Community Transfer of Parous Adult Female Chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda." International Journal of Primatology 27, no. 6 (December 5, 2006): 1601–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10764-006-9098-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Whiten, Andrew, and Kate Arnold. "Grooming Interactions Among the Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda: Tests of Five Explanatory Models." Behaviour 140, no. 4 (2003): 519–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156853903322127968.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractPatterns of allogrooming among the Sonso community of chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda, were examined and found to closely resemble those at other study sites. Strong affiliative bonds among males were reflected in high levels of grooming compared with other sex combinations. Adult males groomed, and received grooming most often from, other adult males and also adolescent females which were the only females with regular oestrous cycles during the study. Males had a wider diversity of grooming partners than females and groomed more equitably. However, males concentrated the majority of their effort on a very small number of partners compared with other sites. Grooming reciprocity was found among all age/sex combinations with the exception of adult male-female dyads once immediate reciprocation in the form of synchronous mutual grooming was removed from the analysis. Since grooming among males is thought to play a major role in servicing relationships and agonistic coalitions that can improve dominance status, competition for high-ranking grooming partners was predicted to influence the distribution of grooming among males. Grooming was indeed directed up the male hierarchy and closely ranked males groomed each other more often than those that were distantly ranked. However, when only adult males were considered, rank had little effect on grooming distributions. High rank appeared to influence access to females, but did not attract more female grooming partners. Grooming distributions in this average-sized community did not fit a number of alternative priority of access models which assume competition for high-ranking grooming partners that Watts (2000b) found to have some explanatory value in one very large community of chimpanzees, but not in a smaller, more representative one. Although rank is highly likely to influence coalition partner choice, whether such relationships depend upon strategic grooming partner choices in wild chimpanzees is presently unclear.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E. "The home range of the Sonso community of chimpanzees from the Budongo Forest, Uganda." African Journal of Ecology 41, no. 2 (June 2003): 150–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2028.2003.00408.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E., Melissa Emery Thompson, Vernon Reynolds, Christophe Boesch, and Linda Vigilant. "Paternity and social rank in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from the Budongo Forest, Uganda." American Journal of Physical Anthropology 142, no. 3 (December 23, 2009): 417–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21241.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Chimpanzees Locomotion Uganda Budongo Forest"

1

Zommers, Zinta A. "Impact of human disturbance in Budongo Forest, Uganda, on four communities of chimpanzees." Thesis, University of Oxford, 2010. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.543012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Mullins, Caroline. "Coordination of joint activities and communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Budongo Forest, Uganda." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/11387.

Full text
Abstract:
Evidence from captive studies suggests that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) lack the motivation to complete cooperative tasks and there is little evidence for communication that coordinates their behaviour during joint actions or whether individuals have a shared motivation towards the same goal. Two types of behaviour where individuals are argued to engage in joint activity are investigated in the Sonso community of wild chimpanzees in Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda: joint travel and group hunting. This study presents evidence from naturalistic observations, and also a playback experiment to further investigate hunting behaviour. Joint travel appears to have no coordinating signal or behaviour on a local level, although the use of pant-hoot vocalisations to coordinate long-distance group travel is supported. In group hunting, bark vocalisations are associated with initiating a group hunt, indicating their potential role in coordination of joint action. Bark production also appears to be affected by the social composition of other hunters, with more barks being produced when other hunters are closer affiliates, suggesting flexibility in call production. The field experiment exploring joint action in group hunts utilises the alarm calls of the chimpanzees' prey species- the Colobus monkey (Colobus guereza). Alarm calls produced by the monkeys when they were being hunting by chimpanzees and in response to an eagle stimulus were played back to dyads of affiliated males, and their vocal and behaviour responses recorded. Results firstly indicate that chimpanzees recognise different Colobus predator alarm. Furthermore, half of the trials with the Colobus hunting alarm elicited a vocal response of barks from both of the males in the experiment, along with joint movement in the direction of the playback. These initial results from a novel field experiment suggest that the bark vocalisation is associated with the initiation of joint action in the context of group hunting.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Bates, Lucy. "Cognitive aspects of travel and food location by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2005. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/2697.

Full text
Abstract:
Finding food in tropical forests poses a potentially major problem for chimpanzees, whose ranging is thought primarily to be directed at locating suitable food resources: (1) chimpanzees are frugivorous, large bodied and live in large home ranges; (2) they lack specialised sensory or locomotor abilities, and terrestrial travel is known to be costly; but (3) fruits are randomly distributed in space and time. Evidence from studies of captive individuals suggests chimpanzees are capable of remembering the locations of out of sight resources and can compute least distance routes to these resources, but whether this ability translates to the natural foraging behaviour of wild chimpanzees has never been investigated. My observational study was designed to assess how the chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Budongo Forest, Uganda, locate these patchy resources. I mapped the routes of 14 focal individuals over a 12-month period. I considered how these foraging routes were structured by breaking the path into segments of travel between resources. Consecutive segments of travel between resources were found not to be independent, but assembled into "super-segments" that take in a number of resources along one trajectory. These super-segments are not necessarily directed towards feeding resources, however: travel is not always food directed. Comparisons of actual chimpanzee routes with randomly generated simulations suggest most individuals do not attempt to minimise their travel distances. There is evidence to suggest energetically stressed individuals can remember the locations of recently visited food resources and return to these patches in order to minimise travel distances when necessary, but overall, food is not difficult to find for this community of chimpanzees. I propose this is because males defend a territory with super-abundant food resources, meaning availability is not a limiting factor of foraging. Male chimpanzees can be characterised as convenience feeders, taking food whilst satisfying other, social needs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Munn, Julie. "The impact of injuries on free-living chimpanzees : how injury effects the social behaviour, mother-dependant behaviour and the locomotion of adult female chimpanzees from the Sonso community, Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda." Master's thesis, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1885/147906.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Chimpanzees Locomotion Uganda Budongo Forest"

1

Reynolds, Vernon. "The future of Budongo's chimpanzees and of the chimpanzees of Uganda as a whole." In The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, 223–42. Oxford University Press, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198515463.003.0013.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Newton-Fisher, Nicholas E. "Relationships of male chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda." In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos, 125–37. Cambridge University Press, 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511606397.013.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography