Academic literature on the topic 'Collective cognition'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Collective cognition.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Sitkin, Sim B. "SHAPING COLLECTIVE COGNITION AND BEHAVIOR THROUGH COLLECTIVE LEARNING." Academy of Management Proceedings 2000, no. 1 (August 2000): B1—B6. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/apbpp.2000.5535126.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Fadul, Jose A. "Collective Learning: Applying Distributed Cognition for Collective Intelligence." International Journal of Learning: Annual Review 16, no. 4 (2009): 211–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.18848/1447-9494/cgp/v16i04/46223.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Pulsifer, Rebecah. "Interwar Imaginings of Collective Cognition." Modernist Cultures 15, no. 2 (May 2020): 155–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/mod.2020.0287.

Full text
Abstract:
Scholarship on interwar understandings of ‘collective cognition’ – experiences of intellectual union with others – tends to focus on its capacity to threaten individuality. I counter this trend by investigating prose works by H.D., Olive Moore, Rebecca West, and H.G. Wells that champion collective cognition for its capacity to compose communities. I argue that these texts point to an underexplored strand that existed in and alongside modernism in which authors turned to collective cognition to imagine radically egalitarian communities that transcend hierarchies based on history, nationality, and species. After the Second World War, the cultural meanings of collective cognition narrowed, and ‘thinking together’ came to be strongly associated with loss of freedom and loss of self. This article shows that collective cognition emitted a powerfully hopeful potential for a significant cluster of interwar authors, who used it to imagine the peaceful and abundant possibilities of collectivity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Couzin, Iain D. "Collective cognition in animal groups." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, no. 1 (January 2009): 36–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Ryynänen, Harri, Kaisa Henttonen, and Risto Tapio Salminen. "Inter-organizational cognitive structures: network conception in MobileTV case." Journal of Business & Industrial Marketing 30, no. 5 (June 1, 2015): 662–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/jbim-08-2013-0187.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose – This paper aims to explore collective cognitive structures in business networks by analyzing the coherency of network pictures in a service development network. Design/methodology/approach – The authors use the network pictures construct as a tool to analyze collective cognitive structures in a service development network. The studied case and unit of analysis is a focal network developing a consumer mobile TV service. Findings – Based on the empirical evidence, the authors found that individuals’ cognitive structures vary extensively in the studied focal network. In addition, collective cognitive structures in intra- and inter-organizational settings differ, and thus should be distinguished. Research limitations/implications – The research findings are based on a single case study. This study is one of the first attempts to apply network pictures as a research device in industrial marketing. The concept of network conception is put forward, distinguishing intra- and inter-organizational levels of collective cognition. Practical implications – The employed conceptual tool is proposed for application also when forming a business network, where it is important to make all actors’ (i.e. persons and companies) perspectives on the emerging focal business network visible. Originality/value – To study the focal network-level collective cognitive structures further, the authors propose the concept of network conception to represent the phenomenon. The present study contributes to the research on collective cognitive structures in industrial marketing by extending understanding on individual- and organizational-level cognitions to a focal network-level collective cognition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Visscher, P. Kirk, and Scott Camazine. "Collective decisions and cognition in bees." Nature 397, no. 6718 (February 1999): 400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/17047.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Canonico, Lorenzo Barberis, Christopher Flathmann, and Nathan McNeese. "Collectively Intelligent Teams: Integrating Team Cognition, Collective Intelligence, and AI for Future Teaming." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 63, no. 1 (November 2019): 1466–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1071181319631278.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper we propose a new model for teamwork that integrates team cognition, collective intelligence, and artificial intelligence. We do this by first characterizing what sets team cognition and collectively intelligence apart, and then reviewing the literature on “superforecasting” and the ability for effectively coordinated teams to outperform predictions by large groups. Lastly, we delve into the ways in which teamwork can be enhanced by artificial intelligence through our model, finally highlighting the many areas of research worth exploring through interdisciplinary efforts.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Neves, Afonso Carlos. "SOCIAL COGNITION, COLLECTIVE IMAGINATION, ARTS AND ZEITGEIST." RECIMA21 - Revista Científica Multidisciplinar - ISSN 2675-6218 5, no. 2 (February 8, 2024): e524840. http://dx.doi.org/10.47820/recima21.v5i2.4840.

Full text
Abstract:
Cognition and imagination are human faculties that are often thought as individual processes. In recent decades, however, they have been studied as one collective experience. Cognition and imagination are so strongly linked to both mind and body that they characterize what it means to be human. Body, brain, cognition and imagination are, in turn, related to culture; culture is manifested by the Arts. We propose that community art reflects its own zeitgeist and changes when collective cognition changes. Thus, humans continuously modify their understanding of themselves and the world. Here, we describe the changing of Arts from the Middle Ages to the Renaissance, when Arts became known by their authors and each work individualized. Concomitantly, advances in Medicine lead to the developing of a detailed human Anatomy as Perspective became increasingly visual. Shifting trends from the Renaissance to the Baroque also accompanied the manifestation of body movement in Arts as Physiology emerged in Medicine, altogether with collective cognition and imagination changing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Watson, Richard, and Michael Levin. "The collective intelligence of evolution and development." Collective Intelligence 2, no. 2 (April 2023): 263391372311683. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/26339137231168355.

Full text
Abstract:
Collective intelligence and individual intelligence are usually considered to be fundamentally different. Individual intelligence is uncontroversial. It occurs in organisms with special neural machinery, evolved by natural selection to enable cognitive and learning functions that serve the fitness benefit of the organism, and then trained through lifetime experience to maximise individual rewards. Whilst the mechanisms of individual intelligence are not fully understood, good models exist for many aspects of individual cognition and learning. Collective intelligence, in contrast, is a much more ambiguous idea. What exactly constitutes collective intelligence is often vague, and the mechanisms that might enable it are frequently domain-specific. These cannot be mechanisms selected specifically for the purpose of collective intelligence because collectives are not (except in special circumstances) evolutionary units, and it is not clear that collectives can learn the way individual intelligences do since they are not a singular locus of rewards and benefits. Here, we use examples from evolution and developmental morphogenesis to argue that these apparent distinctions are not as categorical as they appear. Breaking down such distinctions enables us to borrow from and expand existing models of individual cognition and learning as a framework for collective intelligence, in particular connectionist models familiar in the context of neural networks. We discuss how specific features of these models inform the necessary and sufficient conditions for collective intelligence, and identify current knowledge gaps as opportunities for future research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Aceves, Pedro. "Linguistic Relativity, Collective Cognition, and Team Performance." Academy of Management Proceedings 2019, no. 1 (August 1, 2019): 17244. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/ambpp.2019.17244abstract.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Huebner, Bryce Prinz Jesse J. "Distributing cognition a defense of collective mentality /." Chapel Hill, N.C. : University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 2008. http://dc.lib.unc.edu/u?/etd,1540.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph. D.)--University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 2008.<br>Title from electronic title page (viewed Sep. 16, 2008). "... in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Philosophy." Discipline: Philosophy; Department/School: Philosophy.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Clément, Romain Jean Gilbert. "Collective cognition and decision-making in humans and fish." Doctoral thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Lebenswissenschaftliche Fakultät, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.18452/17605.

Full text
Abstract:
Das Zusammenleben in Gruppen ist im Tierreich ein weit verbreitetes Phänomen. Einer der Vorteile des Gruppenlebens könnte die sogenannte „Schwarmintelligenz“ sein, das heißt die Fähigkeit von Gruppen kognitive Probleme zu lösen, die die Problemlösekompetenz einzelner Individuen übersteigt. In der vorliegenden Dissertation untersuchte ich, ob die Gruppengröße beim Menschen und bei Fischen mit einer verbesserten Entscheidungsfindung einhergeht. Beim Menschen analysierte ich zunächst das Abschneiden von Einzelpersonen, die später als Teil einer Gruppe getestet wurden, in einfachen Einschätzungsaufgaben sowie komplizierteren Satz-Rekonstruktionstests. Meine Frage war, ob es Individuen in Gruppen gelingt bessere Entscheidungen zutreffen als das einem durchschnittlichen Individuum der Gruppe alleine möglich wäre und ob Gruppen sogar die Leistung ihres besten Mitglieds in den individuellen Tests überbieten könnten. Tatsächlich konnte ich zeigen, dass Gruppen die Leistung des besten Mitglieds übertreffen, wenn die Problemstellung für Einzelpersonen zu komplex ist oder sich häufig wiederholt. Weiterhin gelang mir zu zeigen, dass Gruppen von Menschen bei einer simulierten Prädationssituation, ähnlich wie es bereits für andere Tierarten beschrieben wurde, anhand von so genannten „Quorum“-Regeln durch non-verbale Kommunikation entscheiden, ob sie bleiben oder flüchten. Dabei dienen einfache Bewegungsmuster als Schlüsselreiz. Individuen einer Gruppe erhöhen durch diesen Mechanismus gleichzeitig ihre echt positiven und verringern ihre falsch positiven Entscheidungen. Beim Guppy, einem Süßwasserfisch aus Trinidad, untersuchte ich in deren natürlichem Habitat, ob die Fähigkeit einzelner Individuen zwischen einer genießbaren und einer ungenießbaren Futterquelle zu unterscheiden, mit der Gruppengröße ansteigt. Meine Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Guppys mit größerer Wahrscheinlichkeit eine genießbare Futterquelle identifizierten, sobald sie Teil einer größeren Gruppe waren.<br>Group living is a widespread phenomenon. One of its assumed advantages is collective cognition, the ability of groups to solve cognitive problems that are beyond single individuals’ abilities. In this thesis, I investigated whether decision-making improves with group size in both humans and fish, thus using the strengths of each system. In humans, I tested individual performance in simple quantity estimation tasks and a more difficult sentence reconstruction task first alone and then as part of a group. My question was whether groups were able to improve not only on average individual decisions, but also to beat their best members. Indeed, when a given problem is recurrent or too complex for individuals, groups were able to outperform their best members in different contexts. Furthermore, I showed that in a simulated predation experiment, groups of humans decided to stay or to escape using quorum thresholds based on movement behaviour without verbal communication, as has been shown in other animals. This simple movement mechanism allowed individuals in groups to simultaneously increase true positives and decrease false positives. In the guppy, a freshwater fish from Trinidad, I tested in their natural environment whether individuals’ ability to distinguish between an edible and a non-edible food item increases with group size. My results indicate that guppies had better chances to identify the edible food item when part of bigger groups. By investigating several populations with different ecological backgrounds, in particular differing in predation levels, I found that, despite a lower sampling activity in high predation habitats, predation did not affect the improvement of decisions in groups.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Alavi, Seyyed Babak Education Faculty of Arts &amp Social Sciences UNSW. "A multilevel study of collective efficacy, self-mental models, and collective cognition in university student group activities." Awarded by:University of New South Wales. Education, 2005. http://handle.unsw.edu.au/1959.4/33242.

Full text
Abstract:
The main goal of this study was to identify some determinants of collective efficacy in small groups. A multilevel approach was used to posit hypotheses and research questions relating individual and shared beliefs of collective efficacy to collective cognition activities, task interdependence, self-efficacy for group work, and collective orientation. A two-phase longitudinal design was employed. The sample comprised 270 university students, enrolled in seven courses and involved in 86 work groups in both phases of the study. All groups were required to perform interdependent academic tasks. The results of multiple regression analysis of aggregated variables provided some evidence that the more group members perceived themselves to be interdependent in the early stages of group work and assigned their tasks interdependently during group processes, the more likely they developed high collective efficacy in the final stages of group work. Collective efficacy was also related to the group average of self-efficacy for group work when task interdependence was high. Multilevel analysis was also used. These results showed that variation at the individual level was considerable, and there was significant but relatively little variation at the group level, with small effect sizes, for a few variables including collective efficacy. Structural equation modelling was used to confirm the theoretical framework at the individual level after accounting for group level variation. The results suggested that integration and constructive evaluation of ideas during group processes and self-efficacy for group work may have been determinants of collective efficacy at the individual level. Moreover, collective efficacy at the individual level was related to an interdependent perception of self in relation to other group members. The results suggest that helping group members learn how to evaluate and integrate each other???s ideas during group activities, and perceive themselves to be interdependent may enhance group capabilities for performing tasks. In addition, improving students??? self-efficacy for group work was identified as a key factor, as it may enhance a sense of interdependence among group members, improve the extent to which group members participate in integrating and evaluating ideas, and increase the whole group???s capabilities for performing tasks.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Woods, Richard David. "Collective responses to acoustic threat information in jackdaws." Thesis, University of Exeter, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/10871/25978.

Full text
Abstract:
Navigating the physical world may present only a small fraction of the challenges faced by social animals. Sociality brings with it numerous benefits, including access to important information that may have otherwise been harder to come by. However, almost every aspect of these apparent benefits may also entail additional cognitive challenges, including how to interpret signals from conspecifics, who to attend to, and how to incorporate knowledge about signallers when deciding how to respond. One approach to understanding the cognitive abilities associated with social function is to investigate social species that take part in potentially costly group behaviours, where individual decisions must be made in a social context. In this thesis I explore how jackdaws (Corvus monedula), a highly sociable corvid species, use acoustic information to coordinate collective anti-predator responses. In Chapter Two I showed using playback experiments that the magnitude of collective responses to anti-predator recruitment calls known as “scolding” calls depends on the identity of the caller, with larger responses to familiar colony members than unfamiliar individuals. In Chapter Three I then used habituation-dishabituation experiments to show that this vocal discrimination operates at the level of the individual, with jackdaws discriminating between the calls of different conspecifics, regardless of their level of familiarity. In Chapter Four, I examined whether aspects of call structure conveyed information about threat levels. Here, I found that high rates of scolding calls were associated with elevated threats, and playback experiments suggested that this information might result in larger group responses. The finding that jackdaws are capable of mediating their response to alarm calls based on the identity of the individual caller, and on structural variation in call production, raised the question of whether jackdaws employed similar forms discrimination between acoustic cues made by predators in their environment. I investigated this in Chapter Five, using playback experiments to show that jackdaws responded not only to the vocalisations of resident predators, but that this ability extended to novel predators, and that responsiveness was mediated by the phase of the breeding season in which predators were heard. Together, these findings provide insights in to how discrimination among acoustic cues can mediate group behaviour in species that respond collectively to threats.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Gallotti, Mattia Luca. "Naturally we : a philosophical study of collective intentionality." Thesis, University of Exeter, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10036/2997.

Full text
Abstract:
According to many philosophers and scientists, human sociality is explained by our unique capacity to ‘share’ the mental states of others and to form collective intentional states. Collective intentionality has been widely debated in the past two decades, focusing especially on the issue of its reducibility to individual intentionality and the place of collective intentions in the natural realm. It is not clear, however, to what extent these two issues are related, and what methodologies of investigation are appropriate in each case. In this thesis I set out a theory of the naturalization of collective intentionality that draws a line between naturalizability arguments and theories of collective intentionality naturalized. The former provide reasons for believing in the naturalness of collective intentional states based on our commonsense understanding of them; the latter offer responses to the ontological question about the existence and identity of collective as distinct from individual intentionality. This model is naturalistic because it holds that the only way to establish the place of mental entities in the order of things is through the theory and practice of science. After reviewing naturalizability arguments in philosophy, I consider an influential research program in the cognitive sciences. On the account that I present, the irreducibility of collective intentionality can be derived from a theory of human development in scientific psychology dealing with phenomena of sociality like communication, recently refined by Michael Tomasello.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Doi, Stephanie. "Collective Memory and History: An Examination of Perceptions of Accuracy and Preference for Biased “History” Passages." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2017. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/cmc_theses/1633.

Full text
Abstract:
Collective memory is a socially shared representation of the past. History, contrastingly, strives to be an unbiased, objective, and critical account of the past. Many researchers have argued that the so-called “history” found in school textbooks and curriculums align more with collective memory; however, many individuals do not know of the pervasiveness of collective memory in supposed “history” texts. To examine perceptions of accuracy and preference of American “history” textbook passages, individuals from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (n= 404) participated in an online study where they were randomly assigned to read one passage that was either negatively biased, neutral, or positively biased regarding the U.S. dropping the atomic bombs over Hiroshima and Nagasaki during World War II. Participants rated their emotional valence of the event and their perceptions of accuracy and preference for the passage. The results suggest that individuals perceive negatively biased passages as less accurate and less preferable, even if their emotional valence matches the bias within the text. Individuals also showed the hypothesized interaction for preference; those who perceived the event as not negative preferred the positive text to the neutral and negative texts. The findings support evidence that individuals are motivated to prefer history passages consistent with their attitudes and rate higher accuracy among positive and neutral texts. The results have broader implications on reporting or dismissing human rights violations within collective memory.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Oberhauser, Felix Benjamin [Verfasser], Jürgen [Akademischer Betreuer] Heinze, and Tomer J. [Akademischer Betreuer] Czaczkes. "Individual cognition and collective behaviour in ants / Felix Benjamin Oberhauser ; Jürgen Heinze, Tomer J. Czaczkes." Regensburg : Universitätsbibliothek Regensburg, 2019. http://d-nb.info/1201884284/34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Thomas-Antérion, Catherine Laurent Bernard. "La mémoire collective, mémoire des évènements publics et des célébrités apport des batteries EVE 30 et TOP 30 /." Lyon : Université Lumière Lyon 2, 2007. http://demeter.univ-lyon2.fr/sdx/theses/lyon2/2006/thomas_anterion_c.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Guizzo, Francesca. "How Sexual Objectification Affects Women: Self-objectification, Cognitive Performance and Collective Action." Doctoral thesis, Università degli studi di Padova, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/11577/3427240.

Full text
Abstract:
Sexual objectification occurs whenever a person is treated like a sexual object, reduced to a body (or sexual body parts) and used for the pleasure and consumption of others (Bartky, 1980; Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). According to Objectification Theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) women are the main targets of sexual objectification, which is mainly associated with the male objectifying gaze in two contexts: the exposure to sexually objectifying mass media (e.g. television, movies, magazines, advertisements in which women are depicted as sexual objects) and during social interactions. According to the objectification theoretical framework, sexually objectifying experiences are not devoid from important negative effects on women’s psychological well-being. Indeed, the theory proposed that the first direct consequence of sexually objectifying experiences is self-objectification, which leads women to internalize the objectifying observer’s perspective on the physical self, thus reducing the self to an object (Bartky, 1980; Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). In turn self-objectification has been linked to increased body shame, appearance anxiety, and proclivity to eating, depression, and sexual disorders (Moradi & Hefflick, 2008). We started our work noticing that, although sexually objectifying experiences are the precursors of self-objectification and its subsequent adverse outcomes on women, very few researchers have actually tested this causal chain by directly manipulating the exposure to sexually objectifying experiences. Therefore, the general aim of the present work was to contribute to the objectification theoretical framework by testing the causal role of sexually objectifying experiences on women’s psychological, cognitive and social responses. Therefore, in Chapter 1 we will present a brief introduction and review of previous research conducted within the objectification theoretical framework. In Chapter 2, we will present a set of three studies that have investigated the effects of the objectifying male gaze experienced in interpersonal encounters and the exposure to sexually objectifying visual media on women’s psychological outcomes and cognitive performance. In particular we aimed at overcoming possible limitations in the interpretation of previous research that have studied cognitive performance (i.e. Stereotype Threat); taking into account important moderators (i.e. Internalization of sociocultural beauty standards, Social appearance anxiety), we will provide evidence that supports the notion that the exposure to sexually objectifying experiences indeed affects women’s cognitive resources. In addition, we will also investigate possible mechanisms (i.e. Task Intrusive Thoughts, Flow experience) underlying the decrements in performance. Finally, we will bring novel evidence that sexually objectifying experiences are causally linked with adverse psychological outcomes for women (i.e. self-objectification and body dissatisfaction). Our results generally supported the idea that sexually objectifying experiences are the causal precursor of both adverse psychological and cognitive outcomes for women and that important moderators of such causal chain should be taken into account. In Chapter 3, we will present a final study that has investigated the effects of exposure to sexually objectifying television, as well as a reasoned critique of such media content, on gender collective action inclination and behavioral intentions to participate in activism. The results demonstrate, for the first time, that exposure to a reasoned critique of sexually objectifying television motivates women, but not men, to react and to participate in collective actions that aim at reducing such degrading TV portrayals. The results bring novel evidence that the promotion of a critical view of TV through, for example, sensitizing campaigns, might represent an effective intervention to promote social activism and contrast the sexual objectification vicious cycle. Finally, in Chapter 4 in light of the objectification theoretical framework we will discuss the implications of the present findings and suggest future directions.<br>L’Oggettivazione Sessuale si verifica ogni volta che una persona è trattata come un oggetto sessuale, ridotta ad un corpo (o alle sue parti sessuali) e utilizzata per il piacere e il consumo altrui (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). Secondo la Teoria dell’Oggettivazione (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), le donne sono i target principali dell’oggettivazione sessuale, che è principalmente messa in atto attraverso lo sguardo oggettivante maschile soprattutto in due contesti: l’esposizione a mass media sessualmente oggettivanti (e.g. televisione, film, riviste, pubblicità) e durante le interazioni sociali. Secondo il modello teorico dell’oggettivazione, le esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti non sono prive di importanti effetti negativi sul benessere delle donne. La prima conseguenza diretta è l’Auto-Oggettivazione, che porta le donne ad interiorizzare la prospettiva dell'osservatore oggettivante sul sé fisico, riducendo così il sé ad un oggetto (Bartky, 1980; Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997). L’auto-oggettivazione è stata a sua volta collegata all’aumento di vergogna per il proprio corpo, all’ansia legata all’apparenza e all’insorgenza di disordini alimentari, depressivi e sessuali (Moradi e Hefflick, 2008). Partendo da questo quadro teorico, nella presente dissertazione ho focalizzato l'attenzione sulle conseguenze delle esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti sull’auto-oggettivazione delle donne, sulle loro prestazioni cognitive e sulla loro volontà di partecipare ad attivismo sociale. Pertanto, nel primo capitolo presenterò una breve rassegna dei precedenti lavori che hanno indagato il processo di oggettivazione sessuale e i suoi effetti. Abbiamo iniziato il nostro lavoro notando che, sebbene le esperienze oggettivanti sessualmente siano il precursore dell’auto-oggettivazione e dei suoi successivi risultati negativi sul benessere delle donne, pochissime ricerche hanno effettivamente verificato questa catena causale manipolando direttamente l'esposizione a esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti. Pertanto, l'obiettivo generale del presente lavoro tesi è stato quello di espandere ulteriormente il quadro teorico dell’oggettivazione testando la catena causale tra le esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti e le risposte psicologiche, cognitive e sociali delle donne. Pertanto, nel Capitolo 1 presenteremo una breve introduzione e rassegna delle precedenti ricerche condotte all’interno del quadro teorico dell’oggettivazione. Nel capitolo 2, presenteremo una serie di tre studi che hanno indagato gli effetti dello sguardo maschile oggettivante vissuto in interazioni interpersonali, nonché l'esposizione a media sessualmente oggettivanti sugli esiti psicologici e le prestazioni cognitive delle donne. In particolare, si è cercato di superare i possibili limiti interpretativi delle precedenti ricerche che hanno studiato le prestazioni cognitive (i.e. Stereotype Threat) e, tenendo conto di importanti moderatori (i.e. Internalizzazione del canone di bellezza socio-culturale, Ansia sociale legata all’apparenza), offriremo evidenze a sostegno dell'idea che l'esposizione ad esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti di fatto diminuisce le risorse cognitive delle donne. Inoltre, approfondiremo possibili meccanismi (i.e. Pensieri intrusivi durante il compito, Esperienza di flusso) che stanno alla base dei decrementi della prestazione. Infine, forniremo nuove dimostrazioni del fatto che esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti causano conseguenze psicologiche negative per le donne (i.e. Auto-oggettivazione e Insoddisfazione per il proprio corpo). Più in generale, i risultati sostengono l'idea che le esperienze sessualmente oggettivanti sono il precursore causale di conseguenze sia psicologiche che cognitive sfavorevoli per le donne e che importanti moderatori di tale catena causale devono essere presi in considerazione. Nel Capitolo 3, presenterò un quarto studio che ha indagato gli effetti dell'esposizione alla televisione sessualmente oggettivante, e di una critica ragionata di tali contenuti multimediali, sulla propensione alle azioni collettive e sulle intenzioni comportamentali a partecipare all’attivismo sociale. I risultati dimostrano, per la prima volta, che l'esposizione a una critica ragionata alla televisione sessualmente oggettivante motiva le donne, ma non gli uomini, a reagire e a partecipare ad azioni collettive finalizzate a ridurre le rappresentazioni femminili degradanti della TV. I risultati suggeriscono che la promozione di una visione critica della TV attraverso, per esempio, campagne di sensibilizzazione, potrebbe rappresentare un intervento efficace per promuovere un comportamento di protesta per contrastare il circolo vizioso dell'oggettivazione sessuale. Infine, nel Capitolo 4, alla luce del quadro teorico dell’oggettivazione, si discuteranno le implicazioni dei risultati trovati e le direzioni di ricerca future.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Gratier, Maya. "Rythmes et appartenances culturelles : étude acoustique des échanges vocaux entre mères et bébés autochtones et migrants." Paris 5, 2001. http://www.theses.fr/2001PA05H048.

Full text
Abstract:
Cette étude s'appuie sur des méthodes d'analyse acoustique pour éclairer le lien entre la qualité de la négociation dynamique au cours de l'interaction mère-bébé et le sentiment d'identité de la mère. Nous montrons que l'expressivité et la contingence de l'interaction vocale mère-bébé sont particulièrement affectées lorsqu'une mère qui a émigré n'a plus la même perception de soi ni la même confiance en soi qu'avant. Soixante enregistrements d'interactions spontanées mère-bébé ont été effectués en Inde, en France et aux Etats-Unis avec des bébés âgés entre 2 et 6 mois. Leur analyse a révélé une hiérarchie rythmique universelle : toutes les dyades mère-bébé s'appuient sur une pulsation et sur des sensibilités musicales innées pour exprimer conjointement des narrations dans le temps. D'importantes différences culturelles ont également été observées entre les dyades françaises, indiennes et américaines concernant des règles culturelles spécifiques de la négociation dans le dialogue. Ces différences nous ont amenée à proposer que dès l'âge de 2 mois le bébé commence à incorporer les rythmes et styles expressifs qui constituent sa "culture primaire". La comparaison entre 30 dyades migrantes et 30 dyades autochtones a montré que les migrantes sont, de manière générale, moins expressives et moins harmonieuses, ou moins "musicales", dans la coordination rythmique de leurs interactions. Des études de cas et des analyses qualitatives éclairent les résultats quantitatifs. L'effet de la migration n'est cependant pas uniforme et un sous-groupe de mères migrantes présentant des facteurs de risque à été identifié. L'analyse acoustique éclaire, au niveau micro, la manière dont le sens est partagé dans un espace intersubjectif entre la mère et le bébé grâce à un équilibre subtil entre régularité rythmique et variation expressive. Nous explorons la façon dont, comme dans la musique de jazz, le succès du partage intersubjectif significatif dépend des qualités spontanées dynamiques d'expressions négociées. Ainsi, le processus de l'appartenance dépend peut-être d'une négociation musicale sensible au cours d'interactions vives. Ce travail souligne l'importance de la prise en charge précoce et de la prévention parmi les populations migrantes et propose d'employer la "musicalité" comme critère diagnostique<br>This study uses acoustic analysis methods to shed light on the relationship between the dynamics of negotiation in mother-infant interaction and the mother's sense of identity. We show that the expressiveness and contingency of vocal interaction is particularly affected when the mother's sense of self lacks clarity and confidence. This study focuses, in particular, on the effects of immigration on the mother's sense of self. Sixty recordings of spontaneous mother-infant interaction were made in India, France and the United States with infants aged between 2 and 6 months. Our analysis revealed universal hierarchies of rhythm : all of the mothers and infants relied on a beat and on innate musical sensitivities to express jointly created narratives in time. (. . . )
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Curtin, Kevin, and Daniel R. Montello. Collective Spatial Cognition. London: Routledge, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003202738.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Georgiadès, Kimon. Propos sur la vie collective. Paris: Edition d'auteur, 1986.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Bietti, Lucas M. Discursive remembering: Individual and collective remembering as a discursive, cognitive, and historical process. Berlin: De Gruyter, 2014.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Brewer, Marilynn B., Roderick Moreland Kramer, Geoffrey J. Leonardelli, and Robert W. Livingston. Social cognition, social identity, and intergroup relations: A festschrift in honor of Marilynn B. Brewer. Hoboken: Taylor & Francis, 2011.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Goldman, Ellen. As Others See Us. London: Taylor & Francis Inc, 2003.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

A, Fine Mark, ed. Understanding and helping families: A cognitive-behavioral approach. Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum, 1994.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Hewstone, Miles. Causal attribution: From cognitive processes to collective beliefs. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1989.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Collective Spatial Cognition. Routledge, 2023.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Collective Spatial Cognition. Routledge, 2023.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Culture, society, and cognition: Collective goals, values, action, and knowledge. New York: Mouton de Gruyter, 2008.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Book chapters on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Arima, Yoshiko. "Social Cognition." In Psychology of Group and Collective Intelligence, 1–36. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-84698-5_1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Wallace, Rodrick, and Mindy T. Fullilove. "Consciousness And Distributed Cognition." In Collective Consciousness and its Discontents, 15–23. Boston, MA: Springer US, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-76765-9_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Comfort, Louise K., and Mary Lee Rhodes. "Collective Cognition in Complex Systems." In Global Risk Management, 1–21. New York: Routledge, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003253280-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Ertan, Güneş, and Ali Çarkoğlu. "Cognition, Communication, and Collective Action." In Global Risk Management, 141–60. New York: Routledge, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003253280-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Nickerson, Jeffrey V. "Collective Design: Remixing and Visibility." In Design Computing and Cognition '14, 263–76. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-14956-1_15.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Dwyer, Paul. "Measuring Collective Cognition in Online Conversations." In Proceedings of the 2010 Academy of Marketing Science (AMS) Annual Conference, 268. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11797-3_155.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Balunović, Filip. "Consolidation of Discourses through Collective Cognition." In The Revival of the Left in the Balkans, 115–44. London: Routledge, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003342212-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Collins, Harry. "Collective and Distributed Knowledge." In The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy and Implicit Cognition, 202–14. London: Routledge, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781003014584-19.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Miranda, Luis A. Pérez. "Individual and Collective Rationality in a Social Framework." In Truth, Rationality, Cognition, and Music, 223–41. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0548-6_12.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Kimura, Risa, and Tatsuo Nakajima. "Gathering People’s Happy Moments from Collective Human Eyes and Ears for a Wellbeing and Mindful Society." In Augmented Cognition. Human Cognition and Behavior, 207–22. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-50439-7_14.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Farhat, Nabil H., and Haroon Babri. "Cognitive neural networks: collective computing with diverse attractors." In OSA Annual Meeting. Washington, D.C.: Optica Publishing Group, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/oam.1991.mii1.

Full text
Abstract:
Nearly all neural networks for pattern recognition being dealt with today are associative classifier or identifier networks that are not cognitive. To be cognitive a net must be able to distinguish, on its own, between familiar and unfamiliar or novel sensory signals present at its input, and this can not be done by associative classifiers. It will be argued and shown that, to be truly cognitive, a network must be nonlinear and dynamic and able to manifest bifurcation. This means it should be able to carry out phase space computations with more than one type of attractor and to switch between these depending on whether the sensory input is familiar or novel. Cognition implies, therefore, bifurcation and computing with diverse attractors. Our reasons for adopting this view, which stemmed from known biophysical observations and from our neuromorphic target identification work, are discussed. An example of a cognitive network that computes with both stationary (limit point) and dynamic (periodic) attractors is given to illustrate our thesis. The elements of a neuromorphic radar target identification system which employs these concepts and is capable of distortion invariant recognition of three targets with perfect score is presented. The work presented elucidates the role of periodic attractors in feature binding and cognition and the significance of cognition in autonomous systems.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lee, Ju Hyun, Mi Jeong Kim, and Mary Lou Maher. "Designing for interactive and collective mobile creativity." In C&C '13: Creativity and Cognition 2013. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2466627.2466667.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Siangliulue, Pao. "Intelligent Systems to Support Large-Scale Collective Creative Idea Generation." In C&C '15: Creativity and Cognition. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2757226.2764764.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Smart, Paul R., Katia Sycara, and Darren P. Richardson. "Exploring the dynamics of collective cognition using a computational model of cognitive dissonance." In SPIE Defense, Security, and Sensing, edited by Barbara D. Broome, David L. Hall, and James Llinas. SPIE, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1117/12.2016397.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Wei, Yufei, Toshiyuki Yasuda, and Kazuhiro Ohkura. "Collective cognition: A case study of evolutionary swarm robotics in the collective foraging problem with poison." In 2017 IEEE/SICE International Symposium on System Integration (SII). IEEE, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/sii.2017.8279331.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Niizato, Takayuki. "Multiplicity of Interpretation in an Asynchronous Updating Rule: Emergence of Collective Cognition." In European Conference on Artificial Life 2013. MIT Press, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.7551/978-0-262-31709-2-ch104.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Aerts, Diederik, Massimiliano Sassoli De Bianchi, Sandro Sozzo, and Tomas Veloz. "QUANTUM COGNITION GOES BEYOND-QUANTUM: MODELING THE COLLECTIVE PARTICIPANT IN PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENTS." In II International Workshop on Quantum Mechanics and Quantum Information. Physical, Philosophical and Logical Approaches. WORLD SCIENTIFIC, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/9789813276895_0017.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Niizato, Takayuki. "Multiplicity of Interpretation in an Asynchronous Updating Rule: Emergence of Collective Cognition." In European Conference on Artificial Life 2013. MIT Press, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/978-0-262-31709-2-ch104.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hiraga, Motoaki, Yufei Wei, and Kazuhiro Ohkura. "Evolving Collective Cognition of Robotic Swarms in the Foraging Task with Poison." In 2019 IEEE Congress on Evolutionary Computation (CEC). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/cec.2019.8790103.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Mamonov, M., and T. Nikonova. "COLLECTIVE PEDAGOGICAL ACTIVITY AS A COGNITIVELY-DEVELOPING ENVIRONMENTAL AND UPBRINGING SPACE ADDITIONAL EDUCATION OF CHILDREN." In THE TRANSCENDENCE OF BEING: THE IDEALS OF THE COGNITION OF TRUTH, 85–89. FSBE Institution of Higher Education Voronezh State University of Forestry and Technologies named after G.F. Morozov, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.58168/being2024_85-89.

Full text
Abstract:
Based on the scientific and disciplinary specifics of each area of additional education for children and based on the cognitive priorities of pupils, pedagogical activity is didactically actualized by the interdisciplinary educational status of creativity and the humanistic expediency of personal development and professional self-determination of pupils. In this context, one of the key methodological qualities and conditions for the organization of pedagogical activity should include the principle of pedagogical cooperation, on the basis of which the educational trajectory of interaction among teachers, pupils and between teachers and pupils (and their families) is built. The reflection presented in the article is aimed at highlighting the above-mentioned pedagogical aspects of education and training in the system of additional education.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Collective cognition"

1

Martin, Edward A. Cognitive Probe Project: Development of a Testbed for Collecting Cognitive Model Parameterization and Validation Data. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, July 2002. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada406707.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Pinchuk, O. P., V. A. Tkachenko, and O. Yu Burov. AV and VR as Gamification of Cognitive Tasks. CEUR Workshop Proceedings, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.33407/lib.naes.718697.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper presents a comparative analysis of the functionality of mobile applications of the augmented reality Da Vinci Machines AR, Electricity AR, Bridges AR, Geometry, the collection of VR models VictoryVR Science Curriculum and the digital collection Mozaik. The possibility of using these tools for educational purposes is explored, in particular, to construct cognitive tasks for students during the study of subjects in the natural and mathematical cycle. The indicated shortcomings are stated, didactic requirements for such educational activities are formulated. Among others, attention is focused on the following indicators: hardware, usability, variability of model parameters, interactivity, interdisciplinary use, and the ability to activate certain cognitive actions of students, degree/form of gamification. The educational potential of using interactive models and video is analyzed for both group and individual work with students. Examples of methodical developments are given.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Belzil, Christian, Jorgen Hansen, and Julie Pernaudet. Les déterminants cognitifs et non-cognitifs du choix de filière et leur impact sur la phase initiale du cycle professionnel. CIRANO, May 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.54932/zmct9599.

Full text
Abstract:
Grâce à la collecte de données nous permettant de relier les trajectoires éducatives des individus à différentes mesures de compétences, nous étudions les déterminants des choix de filières au Québec et dans le reste du Canada et en particulier, le rôle des compétences cognitives et non-cognitives. Nous évaluons l’impact des études en Sciences, Technologie, Ingénierie, et Mathématiques (STIM) ainsi que l’effet des facteurs cognitifs et non-cognitifs sur un grand nombre de mesures de performance sur le marché du travail. Nos résultats indiquent que les performances individuelles dans le test EIACA (semblable au test PISA) n’ont pratiquement aucun pouvoir prédictif sur la probabilité de compléter un programme scientifique mais jouent un rôle déterminant sur les salaires à 30 ans. La fréquentation d’un programme STIM est principalement expliquée par la compétence académique en mathématiques mesurée par les notes obtenues à l'âge de 18 ans. Le second déterminant le plus important est de loin le facteur non-cognitif mesurant le degré de motivation pendant les études. Toutes choses égales par ailleurs (à compétences égales), les Ontariens ont une probabilité d’obtenir un diplôme STIM plus élevée que les Québécois.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Detulio, Kenneth, and David Skipper. Digital Integrated Collection Environment (DICE)/Cognitive Reasoning Engine (CORE) Intelligent Threat Architecture Study. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, May 2003. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada473159.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Tarasenko, Roman A., Viktor B. Shapovalov, Stanislav A. Usenko, Yevhenii B. Shapovalov, Iryna M. Savchenko, Yevhen Yu Pashchenko, and Adrian Paschke. Comparison of ontology with non-ontology tools for educational research. [б. в.], June 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.31812/123456789/4432.

Full text
Abstract:
Providing complex digital support for scientific research is an urgent problem that requires the creation of useful tools. Cognitive IT-platform Polyhedron has used to collect both existing informational ontology- based tools, and specially designed to complement a full-stack of instruments for digital support for scientific research. Ontological tools have generated using the Polyhedron converter using data from Google sheets. Tools “Search systems”, “Hypothesis test system”, “Centre for collective use”, “The selection of methods”, “The selection of research equipment”, “Sources recommended by Ministry of Education and Science of Ukraine”, “Scopus sources”, “The promising developments of The National Academy of Sciences of Ukraine” were created and structured in the centralized ontology. A comparison of each tool to existing classic web-based analogue provided and described.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Magaloni, Beatriz, Sofía Marinkovic Dal Poggetto, Tommy E. Murphy, Florencia Pucci, and Beatriz Serra Fernández. Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for Gender-based Violence Prevention: Evidence from a Randomized Trial in Mexico. Inter-American Development Bank, May 2025. https://doi.org/10.18235/0013519.

Full text
Abstract:
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) has become a powerful and effective tool to deal with violence in many at-risk areas in the world. However, its use for gender-based violence (GBV) and dating violence, although promising, has been limited and used as a response service for survivors, rather than for prevention. To understand to what extent such interventions can help provide tools and skills to young people in their impressionable years to produce behavioral changes that prevent GBV, we carried out such an intervention among high school students in the municipality of Ecatepec in Mexico. We assessed the intervention with a randomized control trial. We introduce the novelty of collecting objective measures from automated neuropsychological tests to explore whether CBT might be functioning through the development of subjects' executive functions. Results from this intervention fail to show any clear change in self-reported violence. They do show, however, impacts on executive functions related to violence, such as emotional recognition and inhibitory control skills.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Coleman-Jensen, Alisha, and Matthew P. Rabbit. Analysis of the Current Population Survey Food Security Supplement Split-Panel Test. Washington, D.C: Economic Research Service, U.S. Department of Agriculture, September 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.32747/2023.8134354.ers.

Full text
Abstract:
The U.S. Department of Agriculture has monitored the extent and severity of food insecurity in U.S. households for more than 25 years. Data on food security is collected annually as part of the Current Population Survey Food Security Supplement (CPS-FSS). USDA, Economic Research Service undertook a process to update the survey instrument used for the CPS-FSS data collection. An updated survey instrument was created through a process of expert content review and cognitive testing. The revised survey instrument was implemented by the U.S. Census Bureau in a split-panel data collection test in September 2020. The split panel formed two samples that were weighted to represent the U.S. population. Estimates for food spending, food security, and participation in Federal and community nutrition assistance were compared across the two samples. The analysis confirmed that the minor changes to the food security section are unlikely to affect the measurement of food insecurity, or affect comparability of estimates from year-to-year
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Morkun, Volodymyr S., Сергій Олексійович Семеріков, and Svitlana M. Hryshchenko. Use of the system Moodle in the formation of ecological competence of future engineers with the use of geoinformation technologies. Видавництво “CSITA”, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.31812/0564/718.

Full text
Abstract:
At present the information and communication technologies in education can be a catalyst in solving important social problems connected with increasing the educational resources and services availability and quality, real and equal opportunities in getting education for citizens despite their residence, social status and income. One of the most important education tasks is to develop students’ active cognitive attitude to knowledge. Cognitive activity in universities is a necessary stage in preparing for further professional life. The solution of task of formation of ecological competence of mining profile engineer requires the reasonable selection of the means of information and communication technologies conducing formation of ecological competence. Pressing task is constructive and research approach to preparation of future engineers to performance of professional duties in order to make them capable to develop engineering projects independently and exercise control competently. The relevance of the material covered in the article, due to the need to ensure the effectiveness of the educational process in the preparation of the future Mining Engineers. We analyze the source with problems of formation of ecological competence. The article focuses mainly general-purpose computer system support learning Moodle, which allows you to organize individual and collective work of students to master the specialized course teaching material used in teaching special course "Environmental Geoinformatics" in the implementation of educational research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lavrentieva, Olena O., Lina M. Rybalko, Oleh O. Tsys, and Aleksandr D. Uchitel. Theoretical and methodical aspects of the organization of students’ independent study activities together with the use of ICT and tools. [б. в.], September 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.31812/123456789/3244.

Full text
Abstract:
In the article the possibilities and classification of ICTs and tools that can be used in organizing students’ independent study activities of higher education institutions has been explored. It is determined the students’ independent study activities is individual, group, collective activity and is implemented within the process of education under the condition of no pedagogy’s direct involvement. It complies with the requirements of the curriculum and syllabus and is aimed at students’ acquisition of some social experiences in line with the learning objectives of vocational training. The analysis of the latest information and technological approaches to the organization of students’ independent study activities made it possible to determine the means of realization of the leading forms of organization for this activity (independent and research work, lectures, consultations and non-formal education), to characterize and classify the ICTs and tools that support presentation of teaching materials, electronic communication, mastering of learning material, monitoring of students’ learning and cognitive activity, such as ones that serve for the sake of development and support of automated training courses, systems of remote virtual education with elements of artificial intelligence, which implement the principle of adaptive management of learning and the organization of students’ independent study activities. The paper provides the insight into the essence of the conducted investigation on the assesses of the effectiveness of ICTs and tools in the process of organizing students’ independent study activities.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bernardo, Allan, Jose Ramon Albert, Jana Flor Vizmanos, and Mika Muñoz. Toward Measuring Soft Skills for Youth Development: A Scoping Study. Philippine Institute for Development Studies, December 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.62986/dp2023.28.

Full text
Abstract:
Rapid technological advancements and shifting economic paradigms in the 21st century also continuously change the nature of work, wherein more complex and sophisticated skill sets are required. There is a growing recognition of soft skills' pivotal role in preparing the youth for this evolving environment. However, a notable gap remains in identifying what comprises these soft skills or Transversal Competencies (TVC). The study aims to contribute to policy discussions to support the government in enhancing its understanding of soft skills building and formulating strategies to cultivate a well-prepared workforce for the future. The study utilized different interrelated methodological approaches: scoping review and key informant interviews (KIIs), with distinct data-collection methods. The results of the scoping review and KIIs suggest there was no clear common definition of the concept or its dimensions. However, the Philippine articulations of TVCs commonly identify these three categories: (a) critical thinking and other cognitive skills, (b) interpersonal skills, and (c) intrapersonal skills. While these dimensions are prioritized, the data were less clear about the priorities in which TVC concepts and skills should be assessed. To lay the groundwork for potential assessments, the study's recommendation involves formulating a multicomponent assessment of soft skills aligned with basic and higher education curricula. Co-creating a systematic approach to developing assessments of TVCs may include stakeholder consultations, cost-benefit analyses, and meticulous test development phases to ensure technical expertise and appropriateness to local contexts. These assessments may also be leveraged for human resource development and learning in various employment sectors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!