To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Consonant cluster reduction.

Journal articles on the topic 'Consonant cluster reduction'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 44 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Consonant cluster reduction.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Bayley, Robert. "Consonant cluster reduction in Tejano English." Language Variation and Change 6, no. 3 (1994): 303–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0954394500001708.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThis study examines the well-known process of consonant cluster reduction in the English of residents of a San Antonio, Texas, barrio. The study compares Tejano patterns of /-t, d/ deletion with the pan-English pattern summarized by Labov (1989). Results of VARBRUL analysis show that /-t, d/ deletion in Tejano English is constrained by many of the same factors as in other English dialects, including Los Angeles Chicano English. Results also suggest, however, a complex pattern of convergence and divergence. Younger Tejanos are converging toward other dialects of English with respect to
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Riekhakaynen, Elena I. "Realization of intervocalic consonant clusters in frequency words of the Russian language." Vestnik of Saint Petersburg University. Language and Literature 17, no. 4 (2020): 672–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.21638/spbu09.2020.411.

Full text
Abstract:
The article describes the realization of frequent words with the intervocalic consonant clusters [gd] and [ljk] in the oral speech of three groups of informants: adult native speakers of the Russian language, children aged four to six years and Chinese students learning Russian as a second language (929 realizations of 11 words). The data obtained confirm the hypothesis that the most frequent form of reduction of the analyzed combinations of consonants in Russian speech is the loss of the first consonant. However, the variants with the reduction of the consonant and without it are equally prob
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Jongstra, Wenckje. "Variable and Stable Clusters: Variation in the Realisation of Consonant Clusters." Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 48, no. 3-4 (2003): 265–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0008413100000670.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis article reports on between-individual and within-individual variation in consonant cluster reduction strategies (where C1C2 is realised as C( or C2) among young children. The empirical base of the study is a Dutch database with over 9,000 instances of C1 and C2 realisations of 23 word-initial consonant clusters from 45 children aged between two and three years old. The study finds that within-child variation is very limited, whereas between-child variation occurs. It is also shown that there are typological implications; that is, realising C2 in cluster y, implies realising C2 in
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Smit, Ann Bosma. "Phonologic Error Distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska Articulation Norms Project." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 36, no. 5 (1993): 931–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.931.

Full text
Abstract:
The errors on word-initial consonant clusters made by children in the Iowa-Nebraska Articulation Norms Project (Smit, Hand, Freilinger, Bernthal, & Bird, 1990) were tabulated by age range and frequency. The error data show considerable support for Greenlee’s (1974) stages in the acquisition of clusters: the youngest children show cluster reduction, somewhat older children show cluster preservation but with errors on one or more of the cluster elements, and the oldest children generally show correct production. These stages extended to three-element clusters as well. Typical cluster reducti
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Ceron, Marizete Ilha, Marileda Barichello Gubiani, Camila Rosa de Oliveira, and Márcia Keske-Soares. "Factors Influencing Consonant Acquisition in Brazilian Portuguese–Speaking Children." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 60, no. 4 (2017): 759–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2016_jslhr-s-15-0208.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose We sought to provide valid and reliable data on the acquisition of consonant sounds in speakers of Brazilian Portuguese. Method The sample comprised 733 typically developing monolingual speakers of Brazilian Portuguese (ages 3;0–8;11 [years;months]). The presence of surface speech error patterns, the revised percentage consonants correct, and the age of sound acquisition were evaluated using phonological assessment software. The normative values for these variables were reported using means and standard deviations. Results Age had a significant impact on phoneme production. Increasing
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Nikièma, Emmanuel. "Government-Licensing and Consonant Cluster Simplification in Quebec French." Canadian Journal of Linguistics/Revue canadienne de linguistique 44, no. 4 (1999): 327–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0008413100017461.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis article is a reanalysis of cluster simplification in Quebec French (QF) in terms of government-licensing, a condition which requires non-nuclear governing heads to be licensed by a following vowel. It is suggested, contra Côté (1997, 1998), that simplification is triggered by a structural constraint rather than a constraint on sonority. It is shown that in QF, simplification does not apply to word internal clusters such asappartementandvendredibecause the following vowel is realized, but applies to forms liketableandcasque, and converts them into [tab] and [kas] respectively at th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Minkyung Lee. "Korean consonant cluster reduction: focus on markedness-oriented PREC." Studies in Phonetics, Phonology, and Morphology 14, no. 3 (2008): 427–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.17959/sppm.2008.14.3.427.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

McLeod, Sharynne, Linda Hand, Joan B. Rosenthal, and Brett Hayes. "The Effect of Sampling Condition on Children’s Productions of Consonant Clusters." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 37, no. 4 (1994): 868–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3704.868.

Full text
Abstract:
An investigation was conducted to compare the effects of single word and connected speech sampling conditions on the production of consonant clusters. Speech samples were obtained from 40 children with speech sound impairments who were aged 3 years: 6 months to 5 years. The children’s productions of 36 commonly occurring consonant clusters were compared across the two sampling conditions. Overall, children’s productions were more similar than different. Differences between the sampling conditions were apparent for three of the eight phonological processes studied, namely, cluster reduction, fi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lee,Yong-Eun and 오세진. "Consonant Cluster Reduction in Spontaneous English Speech by Korean Learners of English." Jungang Journal of English Language and Literature 55, no. 2 (2013): 239–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.18853/jjell.2013.55.2.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bakovic, Eric. "Apparent ‘sufficiently similar’ degemination in Catalan is due to coalescence." Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 2 (June 12, 2017): 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v2i0.4037.

Full text
Abstract:
Cameron et al. (2010) and Fruehwald & Gorman (2011) present the pattern of homorganic consonant cluster reduction in Catalan as a challenge to Baković’s (2005) theory of antigemination, which predicts that any feature ignored in the determination of consonant identity for the purposes of antigemination in a given language must independently assimilate in that language. I argue that the pattern in Catalan is not a counterexample to this prediction if the reduction process is analyzed as coalescence, following Wheeler (2005), rather than as deletion.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Shuiskaya, Tatiana V. "SYLLABLE STRUCTURE OF WORDS IN THE SPEECH OF 3-YEAR-OLDS." Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, no. 1 (2017): 124–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.22250/2410-7190_2017_3_1_124_135.

Full text
Abstract:
The way children acquire syllable structure of words characterizes the level of their speech development. It is assumed that 3=year=olds without any disorders do not have any difficulties with constructing syllables. The current paper describes the results of an acoustic study of word syllable structure in the speech of twenty Russian 3=year=old subjects. 75% of them demonstrated from 3 to 7 syllable structure changes. The maximum of 13,2% of the total of 53 words were characterized by those changes. There were examples of word-initial single-consonant elision, syllable elision, syllable trans
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Young, Edna Carter. "The Effects of Treatment on Consonant Cluster and Weak Syllable Reduction Processes in Misarticulating Children." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 18, no. 1 (1987): 23–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.1801.23.

Full text
Abstract:
In a multiple baseline across behaviors design, two children with articulation errors were trained to suppress the phonological processes of weak syllable reduction and consonant cluster reduction. Results of this investigation indicated that the treatment was effective in increasing correct responses on target words, and that training generalized to phonetically similar untrained words.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Meline, Timothy. "Description of Phonological Patterns for Nineteen Elementary-Age Children with Hearing Losses." Perceptual and Motor Skills 85, no. 2 (1997): 643–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.85.2.643.

Full text
Abstract:
The speech productions of 19 hard of hearing children between 5 and 12 years of age were examined for errors related to phonological process categories. For comparison, the subjects were divided into groups of 9 with Profound and 10 with Moderate to Severe hearing losses. There was a significant relationship between hearing loss and phonological errors. Seven phonological processes were evident in at least 33% of obligatory contexts. Prevalent processes included final consonant deletion and cluster reduction. The most prevalent deficiencies included / r/ and /1/ phonemes. Subjects with Profoun
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

TORBERT, B. "TRACING NATIVE AMERICAN LANGUAGE HISTORY THROUGH CONSONANT CLUSTER REDUCTION: THE CASE OF LUMBEE ENGLISH." American Speech 76, no. 4 (2001): 361–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/00031283-76-4-361.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Schreier, Daniel. "Convergence and language shift in New Zealand: Consonant cluster reduction in 19th Century Maori English." Journal of Sociolinguistics 7, no. 3 (2003): 378–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9481.00230.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Kirk, Cecilia, and Laura Vigeland. "Content Coverage of Single-Word Tests Used to Assess Common Phonological Error Patterns." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 46, no. 1 (2015): 14–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2014_lshss-13-0054.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose This review evaluated whether 9 single-word tests of phonological error patterns provide adequate content coverage to accurately identify error patterns that are active in a child's speech. Method Tests in the current study were considered to display sufficient opportunities to assess common phonological error patterns if they provided at least 4 opportunities for each of 11 error patterns. The target phonemes for these error patterns had to occur as singletons (except for final consonant deletion and cluster reduction) and in stressed syllables (except for weak syllable deletion). Err
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Dodd, Barbara J., and Lydia K. H. So. "The Phonological Abilities of Cantonese-Speaking Children With Hearing Loss." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 37, no. 3 (1994): 671–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3703.671.

Full text
Abstract:
Little is known about the acquisition of phonology by children with hearing loss who learn languages other than English. In this study, the phonological abilities of 12 Cantonese-speaking children (ages 4:2 to 6:11) with prelingual hearing impairment are described. All but 3 children had almost complete syllable-initial consonant repertoires; all but 2 had complete syllable-final consonant and vowel repertoires; and only 1 child failed to produce all nine tones. Children’s perception of single words was assessed using sets of words that included tone, consonant, and semantic distractors. Altho
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

BOUGHTON, ZOË. "Social class, cluster simplification and following context: Sociolinguistic variation in word-final post-obstruent liquid deletion in French." Journal of French Language Studies 25, no. 1 (2013): 1–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0959269513000446.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThis article is a quantitative study of the variable deletion of post-obstruent /l/ and /R/ in word-final obstruent-liquid clusters (OLC) in French (capable [kapab(l)], cidre [sid(ʁ)]). The analysis of over a thousand tokens extracted from a corpus of interviews gathered in Nancy and Rennes shows that the reduction of word-final OLCs is a stable sociolinguistic marker in northern, standardised metropolitan French. Patterns of stylistic and social stratification in age, gender, and social class and interaction with following phonological context are attested, but OLC reduction does not
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Lowe, Robert J. "Phonological Process Analysis Using Three Position Tests." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 17, no. 1 (1986): 72–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.1701.78.

Full text
Abstract:
Phonological process analysis forms were developed for the Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation and selected items from the Templin-Darley Tests of Articulation. These forms were used to describe the articulation of a 3-year-old child with multiple articulation errors. Both forms identified backing, final consonant deletion, stopping, and cluster reduction as processes affecting the child's speech. Procedures for scoring the forms are provided. For clinicians not familiar with phonological processes, the use of these forms should prove helpful in deriving phonological information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Franklin, Amber, and Lana McDaniel. "Exploring a Phonological Process Approach to Adult Pronunciation Training." American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 25, no. 2 (2016): 172–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2015_ajslp-14-0172.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The production of speech sound classes in adult language learners is affected by (a) interference between the native language and the target language and (b) speaker variables such as time speaking English. In this article, we demonstrate how phonological process analysis, an approach typically used in child speech, can be used to characterize adult target language phonological learning. Method Sentences produced by 2 adult Japanese English language learners were transcribed and coded for phoneme accuracy and analyzed according to the percent occurrence of phonological processes. The r
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Andreassen, Helene N. "The behavior of secondary consonant clusters in Swiss French child language." Nordlyd 40, no. 1 (2013): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.7557/12.2498.

Full text
Abstract:
<!--[if gte mso 9]><xml> <o:OfficeDocumentSettings> <o:AllowPNG /> </o:OfficeDocumentSettings> </xml><![endif]--> <!--[if gte mso 9]><xml> <w:WordDocument> <w:View>Normal</w:View> <w:Zoom>0</w:Zoom> <w:TrackMoves /> <w:TrackFormatting /> <w:PunctuationKerning /> <w:ValidateAgainstSchemas /> <w:SaveIfXMLInvalid>false</w:SaveIfXMLInvalid> <w:IgnoreMixedContent>false</w:IgnoreMixedContent> <w:AlwaysShowPlaceholderText>false</w:AlwaysShowPlaceholderText> &
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Chwesiuk, Urszula. "Insertion of vowels in English syllabic consonantal clusters pronounced by L1 Polish speakers." Open Linguistics 7, no. 1 (2021): 331–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/opli-2021-0014.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The aim of this study was an attempt to verify whether Polish speakers of English insert a vowel in the word-final clusters containing a consonant and a syllabic /l/ or /n/ due to the L1–L2 transfer. L1 Polish speakers are mostly unaware of the existence of syllabic consonants; hence, they use the Polish phonotactics and articulate a vocalic sound before a final sonorant which is deprived of its syllabicity. This phenomenon was examined among L1 Polish speakers, 1-year students of English studies, and the recording sessions were repeated a year later. Since, over that time, they were
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Morrow, Alyse, Brian A. Goldstein, Amanda Gilhool, and Johanne Paradis. "Phonological Skills in English Language Learners." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 45, no. 1 (2014): 26–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2013_lshss-13-0009.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this study was to examine the English phonological skills of English language learners (ELLs) over 5 time points. Method Sound class accuracy, whole-word accuracy, percentage of occurrence of phonological patterns, and sociolinguistic correlational analyses were investigated in 19 ELLs ranging in age from 5;0 (years;months) to 7;6. Results Accuracy across all samples was over 90% for all sound classes except fricatives and increased for all sound classes across time. Whole-word accuracy was high and increased across time. With the exception of cluster reduction, stopping
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

SHUISKAYA, TATIANA V., and SVETLANA V. ANDROSOVA. "REALIZATION OF RUSSIAN CONSONANTS IN 3-YEAR-OLDS SPEECH." Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, no. 3 (2017): 94–108. http://dx.doi.org/10.22250/2410-7190_2017_3_4_94_108.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper presents an empirical study of errors of various types committed in consonant realization by 20 Russian male and female 3-year-olds. We aimed at ranging Russian consonants according to the difficulty of their articulation focusing on common error tendencies and idiosyncratic error features. The results of the acoustic study of phoneme opposition phonetic manifestation showed that /г/, /rV, /1/, /IV, /J7, /JV, /3/, /tJ7 were the most difficult for 3-year-olds performance. A further finding was high across-speaker variability in the studied age group as far as the level of native lang
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Barry, William, and Bistra Andreeva. "Cross-language similarities and differences in spontaneous speech patterns." Journal of the International Phonetic Association 31, no. 1 (2001): 51–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0025100301001050.

Full text
Abstract:
Quasi-spontaneous dialogues from six languages which, according to recent discussion of rhythmic types, belong to three rhythmic groups – Russian and Bulgarian as ‘stress-timed’, Italian and Greek as ‘syllable-timed’ and Polish and Czech as an intermediate ‘mixed’ type – were examined for the following segmental reduction phenomena: reduction of consonant clusters, weakening of consonant articulation, residual properties from elided consonants in the original context segments, phonetic schwa-isation and syllable elision. The hypothesis tested was that there are comparable reduction phenomena i
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Guy, Gregory R. "Explanation in variable phonology: An exponential model of morphological constraints." Language Variation and Change 3, no. 1 (1991): 1–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0954394500000429.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTVariationist treatments of phonological processes typically provide precise quantitative accounts of the effects of conditioning environmental factors on the occurrence of the process, and these effects have been shown to be robust for several well-studied processes. But comparable precision in theoretical explanation is usually elusive, at the current state of the discipline. That is, the analyst is usually unable to say why the parameters should have the particular values that they do, although one can often explain relative ordering of environments. This article attempts to give a p
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Daana, Hana A., and Sura M. Khrais. "The Acquisition of English and Arabic Onset Cluster: A Case Study." English Linguistics Research 7, no. 1 (2018): 13. http://dx.doi.org/10.5430/elr.v7n1p13.

Full text
Abstract:
This study has investigated the development of English and Arabic cluster in the speech of a bilingual child. Data was related to several recording sessions of spontaneous and non-spontaneous speech between the author and the child. The gradual development of English and Arabic onset cluster was traced in the production of a bilingual child aged between 1 year 04 months and 4 years 06 months. A comparative description of the repair strategy types used by the child to break English and Arabic consonants clustering in word-initial position has been provided. English onset clusters started to app
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Recasens, Daniel. "The effect of syllable position on consonant reduction (evidence from Catalan consonant clusters)." Journal of Phonetics 32, no. 3 (2004): 435–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2004.02.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Wiltshire, Caroline R. "The “Indian English” of Tibeto-Burman language speakers." English World-Wide 26, no. 3 (2005): 275–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/eww.26.3.03wil.

Full text
Abstract:
English as spoken as a second language in India (IE) has developed different sound patterns from other varieties of English. While most descriptions of IE have focused on the English of speakers whose first languages belong to the Indo-Aryan or Dravidian families, in this study, I examine the phonetic and phonological characteristics of the English produced by speakers of three Indian L1s from the Tibeto-Burman language family (Angami, Ao, and Mizo). In addition to describing aspects of Tibeto-Burman Indian English, a previously unreported Indian English variety, I also examine how and why thi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Goldstein, Brian A., Leah Fabiano, and Patricia Swasey Washington. "Phonological Skills in Predominantly English-Speaking, Predominantly Spanish-Speaking, and Spanish-English Bilingual Children." Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 36, no. 3 (2005): 201–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2005/021).

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose: There is a paucity of information detailing the phonological skills of Spanish-English bilingual children and comparing that information to information concerning the phonological skills of predominantly English-speaking (PE) and predominantly Spanish-speaking (PS) children. The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between amount of output (i.e., percentage of time each language was spoken) in each language and phonological skills in Spanish-English bilingual children and PE and PS children. Method: Fifteen typically developing children, ranging in age from 5;0 (years
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Childs, Becky, and Christine Mallinson. "African American English in Appalachia." English World-Wide 25, no. 1 (2004): 27–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/eww.25.1.03chi.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent studies of bi-ethnic enclave dialect communities in the American South suggest that earlier versions of African American speech both accommodated local dialect norms and exhibited a persistent substratal effect from the early African-European contact situation. We examine this hypothesis by considering the sociolinguistic situation in Texana, North Carolina, a small African American community in the Smoky Mountain region of Appalachia. Though its population is only about 150 residents, it is the largest African American community in the Smoky Mountains. This study considers diagnostic s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

van de Ven, Marco, and Mirjam Ernestus. "The role of segmental and durational cues in the processing of reduced words." Language and Speech 61, no. 3 (2017): 358–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0023830917727774.

Full text
Abstract:
In natural conversations, words are generally shorter and they often lack segments. It is unclear to what extent such durational and segmental reductions affect word recognition. The present study investigates to what extent reduction in the initial syllable hinders word comprehension, which types of segments listeners mostly rely on, and whether listeners use word duration as a cue in word recognition. We conducted three experiments in Dutch, in which we adapted the gating paradigm to study the comprehension of spontaneously uttered conversational speech by aligning the gates with the edges o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Vyniautaitė, Simona. "The geolect of Plungė in terms of regressive assimilation of vowels I, U." Lietuvių kalba, no. 14 (June 10, 2020): 1–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/lk.2020.22465.

Full text
Abstract:
Based on dialectometric methods, the article discusses the geolect of Plungė in terms of regressive assimilation of vowels i, u. The study material consists of about 9 hours of audio recordings, 57 sentences, recited by nine presenters of younger, middle and older generations. 6 words were chosen in which regressive assimilation of vowels can take place, i. e., the words with vowels i, u in accented, unaccented and shifted accent positions. Quantitative analysis of the material (sentences read by the presenters) was performed with the tools of the computer program Gabmap. Pseudo maps of networ
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Dargiel, Karolina. "Sylaba w gwarze Moravy e Epërme." Slavia Meridionalis 10 (August 31, 2015): 53–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.11649/sm.2010.005.

Full text
Abstract:
Syllable pattern in Morava e Epërme local dialect of AlbanianThis article summarizes the first part of a research on syllable pattern in Kosovo dialects. It attempts to answer the question, whether Kosovo dialects have one-peak syllable pattern, two-peak syllable pattern or maybe some other type. Facing many theories about the syllabic unit, that have been created until now, for our study we choose the sonority theory, which is a very comfortable model for formulating distributional rules of speech sounds.Standard Albanian language is untypical against other European languages. This is due to
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Kuņicka, Kristīne. "POLISH LANGUAGE IN REZEKNE TODAY. PHONETICS." Via Latgalica, no. 5 (December 31, 2013): 124. http://dx.doi.org/10.17770/latg2013.5.1641.

Full text
Abstract:
According to Population Census 2011, the estimated number of Poles in Latgale was 20,806 (7%). In the city of Rēzekne there were 795 Poles (2.5%) who constituted the third largest national minority after Latvians and Russians (CSP 2012). The Polish language spoken in Latvia belongs to the Northern-Peripheral Polish (in Polish ‘polszcszyzna północnokresowa’) that functions on the territory of the former Polish-Lithuanian Commonwealth (Ананьева 2004: 103). The aim of the paper is to describe and to analyse the major phonetic peculiarities of the Polish regiolect used by the Poles living in Rēzek
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Sagart (沙加爾), Laurent, та William H. Baxter (白一平). "A Hypothesis on the Origin of Old Chinese Pharyngealization (上古漢語咽化聲母來源的一個假設)". Bulletin of Chinese Linguistics 9, № 2 (2016): 179–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/2405478x-00902002.

Full text
Abstract:
It is proposed that oc pharyngealized onset consonants—that is, ‘type-A’ onset consonants—arose out of Proto-Sino-Tibetan plain consonants followed by geminate vowels separated by a pharyngeal fricative. When the first copy of the geminate vowel fell, the initial consonants formed clusters with the pharyngeal fricative, evolving into the oc pharyngealized consonants we reconstruct. In the Kuki-Chin branch of Tibeto-Burman, the pharyngeal fricative fell, and long vowels resulted. This proposal supposes a statistical correlation between Kuki-Chin long vowels and oc type-A words on the one hand,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Šekli, Matej. "Old Romance place names in early South Slavic and late Proto-Slavic sound changes." Linguistica 55, no. 1 (2015): 103–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.4312/linguistica.55.1.103-114.

Full text
Abstract:
The analysis of Old Romance geographical names in early South Slavic confirms that the majority of late Proto-Slavic sound changes were still operative in the period of the earliest Old Romance-Slavic language contacts in the Balkan Peninsula and eastern Alps from the second half of the 6th century and the beginning of the 7th century onwards. Phonetic substitutions of the type Rom. *kE, *gE → Sl. *c, *ʒ (Balk. Rom. *Kersu → Sl. *Cersъ, Balk. Rom. *Gīla → Sl. *Ʒiĺa) and Rom. *auC → Sl. *ovC (Balk. Rom. *Laurentiu > *Laurenču → Sl. *Lovręčь) point to the fact that the first palatalization of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Deterding, David, Jennie Wong, and Andy Kirkpatrick. "The pronunciation of Hong Kong English." English World-Wide 29, no. 2 (2008): 148–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/eww.29.2.03det.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper provides a detailed description of the pronunciation of English by fifteen fourth-year undergraduates at the Hong Kong Institute of Education. First, the occurrence of American features of pronunciation is considered. Then there is an analysis of the pronunciation of initial TH, initial and final consonant clusters, L-vocalisation, conflation between initial [n] and [l], monophthong vowels, the vowels in FACE and GOAT, vowel reduction, rhythm and sentence stress. Finally, the status of Hong Kong English is considered, particularly the extent of its continuing alignment with an exono
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

GOSLIN, JEREMY, and CAROLINE FLOCCIA. "Comparing French syllabification in preliterate children and adults." Applied Psycholinguistics 28, no. 2 (2007): 341–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716407070178.

Full text
Abstract:
The influence of development and literacy upon syllabification in French was evaluated by comparing the segmental behavior of 4- to 5-year-old preliterate children and adults using a pause insertion task. Participants were required to repeat bisyllabic words such as “fourmi” (ant) by inserting a pause between its two syllabic components (/fur/-/mi/). In the first experiment we tested segmentation over a range of 49 double intervocalic consonant clusters. A similar general segmentation behavior was observed in both age groups, with a pattern that fit the predictions from a legality principle-ba
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Carlson, Matthew T., and Alexander McAllister. "I’ve heard that one before: Phonetic reduction in speech production as a possible contributing factor in perceptual illusory vowel effects." Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 12, no. 2 (2019): 281–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/shll-2019-2013.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This study probed the relationship between productive phonotactic repair and speech production, by asking whether the natural variability found in speech, through phonetic reduction, may include apparent illicit sequences requiring repair, even though the target words are licit. Spanish productively repairs word-initial /s/-consonant clusters (#sC) with a prothetic [e] in both production and perception. We asked whether the initial vowel in Spanish #VsC words like espalda ‘back’ is prone to reduction, and whether or not /e/, which matches the default repair vowel, is more susceptible
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Vihman, Marilyn May, and Mel Greenlee. "Individual Differences in Phonological Development." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 30, no. 4 (1987): 503–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3004.503.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper reports the results of a study of the persistence of individual differences in the phonological development of 10 normally developing children observed at age 1 year and again at age 3 years. Data were based on ½-hr audio and video recordings of weekly spontaneous mother-child interaction sessions in the home between 9 and 17 months and at 36 months. In addition, phonological and cognitive probes were administered at age 3. At age 1 the children were compared at four times selected on the basis of the number of different word types used in a session. Preferences for particular phono
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Мір Фарук Агамад, Гаснаїн Імтіаз, and Хан Азизуддин. "Kashmiri: A Phonological Sketch." East European Journal of Psycholinguistics 5, no. 2 (2018): 32–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.29038/eejpl.2018.5.2.mir.

Full text
Abstract:
Kashmiri is an Indo-Aryan language spoken predominantly in the state of Jammu and Kashmir, India and in some parts of Pakistan. Some phonological and morphological features of this language make it peculiar among Indo-Aryan languages. This write-up provides a phonological sketch of Kashmiri. The description of Vowels and Consonants is given in order to build a general idea of the phonological system of the language. The process of nasalization is phonemic in Kashmiri. The aim of this write-up is to describe and show all the phonological features of the language, particularly those that are uni
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Souza, Ana Carla Filgueira de Souza e., Luciana Lyra Casais-e-Silva, and Eduardo Pondé de Sena. "The influence of prematurity on the development of phonological skills." Revista CEFAC 21, no. 4 (2019). http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-0216/201921413118.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACT Purpose: to check the use of phonological processes in preterm infants. Methods: phonological evaluation was performed through the ABFW Child Language Test in 40 children, aged two to four years, i.e., 20 preterm and 20 full-term children, matched according to age, gender and socioeconomic level. Preterm children were evaluated at the State Center for Prevention and Rehabilitation of People with Disabilities - CEPRED; full-term children were selected and evaluated in a municipal nursery in the city of Salvador, BA, Brazil. The pertinent statistical tests were applied adopting the leve
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Wegener, Heide. "Normprobleme bei der Pluralbildung fremder und nativer Substantive." Linguistik Online 16, no. 4 (2003). http://dx.doi.org/10.13092/lo.16.799.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper takes the German noun plural formation as an example for different cases of doubt and shows in which circumstances German speakers can have problems to create the "normal" plural form corresponding to the standard. The paper distinguishes between native and non native plural forms. The cases of doubt within the former can be shown to result from either natural change which leads to a reduction of plural classes and explains the decline of the er- and the umlaut plural, or from a strategy of compensation which replaces the non iconic 0-plural by forms in -n or -s. The problems with th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!