Academic literature on the topic 'Contextual color cues'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Contextual color cues.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Contextual color cues"
Blair, Christopher David, and Gideon Paul Caplovitz. "The Effect of Attention on Context Dependent Synesthetic Experiences." Seeing and Perceiving 25, no. 6 (2012): 619–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/18784763-00002392.
Full textAzadi, Reza, and Mark R. Harwood. "Visual cues that are effective for contextual saccade adaptation." Journal of Neurophysiology 111, no. 11 (June 1, 2014): 2307–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00894.2013.
Full textKrouchev, Nedialko I., and John F. Kalaska. "Context-Dependent Anticipation of Different Task Dynamics: Rapid Recall of Appropriate Motor Skills Using Visual Cues." Journal of Neurophysiology 89, no. 2 (February 1, 2003): 1165–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00779.2002.
Full textHoward, Ian S., Daniel M. Wolpert, and David W. Franklin. "The effect of contextual cues on the encoding of motor memories." Journal of Neurophysiology 109, no. 10 (May 15, 2013): 2632–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00773.2012.
Full textCecala, Aaron L., Ivan Smalianchuk, Sanjeev B. Khanna, Matthew A. Smith, and Neeraj J. Gandhi. "Context cue-dependent saccadic adaptation in rhesus macaques cannot be elicited using color." Journal of Neurophysiology 114, no. 1 (July 2015): 570–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00666.2014.
Full textRuba, Ashley L., Christopher A. Thorstenson, and Betty M. Repacholi. "Red Enhances the Processing of Anger Facial Configurations as a Function of Target Gender." Social Cognition 39, no. 3 (June 2021): 396–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2021.39.3.396.
Full textMonteon, Jachin A., Marie Avillac, Xiaogang Yan, Hongying Wang, and J. Douglas Crawford. "Neural mechanisms for predictive head movement strategies during sequential gaze shifts." Journal of Neurophysiology 108, no. 10 (November 15, 2012): 2689–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00222.2012.
Full textBarcelo, Francisco, Carles Escera, Maria J. Corral, and Jose A. Periáñez. "Task Switching and Novelty Processing Activate a Common Neural Network for Cognitive Control." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, no. 10 (October 2006): 1734–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1734.
Full textChen, Hung-Tao, and Benjamin D. Horne. "Effects of Priming on Online Discussion Behavior." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 62, no. 1 (September 2018): 335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1541931218621077.
Full textKarabanowicz, Ewa, Ernest Tyburski, Karol Karasiewicz, Adrianna Bober, Leszek Sagan, Monika Mak, and Wioletta Radziwiłłowicz. "Higher-Order Language Dysfunctions in Individuals with Alcohol Use Disorder." Journal of Clinical Medicine 10, no. 18 (September 16, 2021): 4199. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jcm10184199.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Contextual color cues"
Addou, Touria. "Mécanismes psychophysiques et neuronaux de la compensation dynamique de multiples champs de force : facilitation et anticipation liée à des indices de couleur." Thèse, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/1866/15996.
Full textIn this thesis, we addressed motor control by two experimental approaches: psychophysical studies in human subjects and neurophysiological recordings in non-human primates. We identified unresolved issues concerning interference in motor learning during adaptation of subjects to two or more anti-correlated force fields. We designed paradigms in which arbitrary color stimuli provided contextual cues that allowed subjects to predict the nature of impending external force fields before encountering them physically during arm movements. This contextual knowledge helped to facilitate adaptation to the force fields by reducing this interference. According to one computational model of motor learning (MOdular Selection And Identification model for Control; MOSAIC), the color context cues made it easier for subjects to build “internal models” of each force field, to recall them and to switch between them with minimal interference. In our first experiment, four groups of human subjects performed elbow flexion/extension movements against two anti-correlated viscous force fields. We combined two different colors for the computer monitor background with two forces: resistive (Vr) and assistive (Va). The first two groups were control subjects. In those subjects, the color of the computer monitor changed at regular intervals but the force field remained constant; Vr was presented to the first group while the second group only experienced Va. As a result, the color cues were irrelevant in the two control groups. All control subjects adapted well to the single experienced force field (Vr or Va). In the two experimental groups, in contrast, the anti-correlated force fields and the monitor colors changed repeatedly between short blocks of trials. In the first experimental group (Reliable-cue subjects), there was a consistent relationship between the force and the stimulus (color of the monitor) - the red colour always signalled the resistive force while the green colour always signalled the assistive force. Adaptation to the two anti-correlated forces for the Reliable-cue group was significant during 10 days of training and almost as good as in the Irrelevant-cue groups who only experienced one of the two force fields. Furthermore, the Reliable-cue subjects quickly demonstrated predictive adaptive changes in their motor output whenever the monitor color changed, even during their first day of training, showing that they could use the reliable color context cues to recall the appropriate motor skills. In contrast, the monitor color also changed regularly between red and green in the second experimental group, but the force fields were not consistently associated with the color cue (Unreliable-cue group). These subjects took longer to adapt to the two force fields than the other three groups, and could not use the unreliable color cue change to make predictive changes to their motor output. Nevertheless, all Unreliable-cue subjects developed an ingenious strategy of making a specific “default” arm movement to probe the type of force field they would encounter in the first trial after the monitor color changed and used the proprioceptive feedback about the nature of the field to make appropriate predictive changes to their motor output for the next few trials, until the monitor color changed again, signifying the possibility of a change in force fields. This strategy was effective since the force remained constant in each short block of trials while the monitor color remained unchanged. This showed that the Unreliable-cue subjects were able to extract implicit and explicit information about the structure of the task from the color stimuli and use that knowledge to reduce interference when adapting to anti-correlated forces. The results of this first study encouraged us to advance our understanding of how subjects can recall multiple motor skills coupled to color context stimuli can be recalled, and how this phenomenon can be reflected by the neuronal activity in monkeys. Our aim was to elucidate how neurons of primary motor cortex (M1) can contribute to adaptive compensation for a wide range of different external forces during single-joint elbow flexion/extension movements. At the same time, we aimed to test the hypothesis evoked in the MOSAIC model, whereby multiple controller modules located in the cerebellum may predict each context and produce appropriate adaptive output signals for a small range of task conditions. Also, according to this hypothesis, M1 neurons may receive inputs from many specialized cerebellar controllers and show appropriate response modulations for a wide range of task conditions. We trained two monkeys to adapt their flexion/extension elbow movements against 5 different force-field conditions: null field without any external force disturbance, two anti-correlated viscous forces (assistive and resistive), which depended on movement speed and resembled that used in the human psychophysical study, a resistive elastic force which depended on elbow-joint position and finally, a visco-elastic field that was the linear sum of the elastic and viscous forces field. Each force field was reliably coupled to 5 different computer monitor background colors. The monkeys properly adapted to the 5 different force-field conditions and used the color context cues to recall the corresponding motor skill for the force field associated with each color, so that they could make predictive changes to their motor output before they physically encountered the force fields. EMG recordings eliminated the possibility that a co-contraction strategy was used by the monkeys to adapt to the force fields, since the EMG patterns were appropriate to compensate for each force-field condition. In parallel, M1 neurons showed systematic changes in their activity at the single-neuron and population level in each force-field condition that could signal the required changes in the direction, magnitude and time course of muscle force output required to compensate for the 5 force-field conditions. The patterns of response changes in each force field were consistent enough across M1 neurons to suggest that most M1 neurons contributed to the compensation for all force field conditions, in line with the predictions of the MOSAIC model. Also, these response changes do not support a strongly modular organization for M1.
Prestera, Gustavo E. "Effects of contextual color on recall border color as a lesson and postest cue for factual and conceptual information presented in computer-based instruction /." 2003. http://etda.libraries.psu.edu/theses/approved/WorldWideIndex/ETD-333/index.html.
Full textBooks on the topic "Contextual color cues"
Prestera, Gustavo E. Effects of contextual color on recall: Border color as a lesson and posttest cue for factual and conceptual information presented in computer-based instruction : a thesis in instructional systems. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Dissertation Services, 2004.
Find full textConference papers on the topic "Contextual color cues"
Giaccone, P. "Segmenting image sequences by embedding motion and colour cues within a contextual relaxation scheme." In IEE Colloquium on Motion Analysis and Tracking. IEE, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1049/ic:19990588.
Full text