Academic literature on the topic 'Cross-serial dependencies'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Cross-serial dependencies.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Cross-serial dependencies"

1

Ojeda, Almerindo E. "A linear precedence account of cross-serial dependencies." Linguistics and Philosophy 11, no. 4 (November 1988): 457–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00668683.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Fukui, Naoki. "A Note on Weak vs. Strong Generation in Human Language." Studies in Chinese Linguistics 36, no. 2 (December 1, 2015): 59–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/scl-2015-0004.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This paper argues that various important results of formal language theory (e.g., the so-called Chomsky Hierarchy) may in fact be illusory as far as the human language faculty is concerned, as has been repeatedly emphasized by Chomsky himself. The paper takes up nested dependencies and cross-serial dependencies, the two important dependencies that typically show up in the discussion of the central classes of grammars and languages, and specifically shows that the fact that nested dependencies abound in human language while cross-serial dependencies are rather limited in human language can be naturally explained if we shift our attention from dependencies defined on terminal strings to abstract structures behind them. The paper then shows that nested dependencies are readily obtained by Merge, applying phase-by-phase, whereas cross-serial dependencies are available only as a result of copying Merge, which requires a constituency of the relevant strings. These results strongly suggest that dependencies are possible in human language only to the extent that they are the results from the structures that can be generated by Merge, leading to the conclusion that it is Merge-generability that determines various dependencies in human language, and that dependencies defined on the terminal strings are indeed illusory. A possible brain science experiment to demonstrate this point is also suggested.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kirov, Christo, and Robert Frank. "Processing of nested and cross-serial dependencies: an automaton perspective on SRN behaviour." Connection Science 24, no. 1 (March 2012): 1–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09540091.2011.641939.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Kaan, Edith, and Nada Vasić. "Cross-serial dependencies in dutch: Testing the influence of NP type on processing load." Memory & Cognition 32, no. 2 (March 2004): 175–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03196850.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Runge, Jakob, Vladimir Petoukhov, and Jürgen Kurths. "Quantifying the Strength and Delay of Climatic Interactions: The Ambiguities of Cross Correlation and a Novel Measure Based on Graphical Models." Journal of Climate 27, no. 2 (January 15, 2014): 720–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1175/jcli-d-13-00159.1.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Lagged cross-correlation and regression analysis are commonly used to gain insights into interaction mechanisms between climatological processes, in particular to assess time delays and to quantify the strength of a mechanism. Exemplified on temperature anomalies in Europe and the tropical Pacific and Atlantic, the authors study lagged correlation and regressions analytically for a simple model system. A strong dependence on the influence of serial dependencies or autocorrelation is demonstrated, which can lead to misleading conclusions about time delays and also obscures a quantification of the interaction mechanism. To overcome these possible artifacts, the authors propose a two-step procedure based on the concept of graphical models recently introduced to climate research. In the first step, graphical models are used to detect the existence of (Granger) causal interactions that determine the time delays of a mechanism. In the second step, a certain partial correlation and a regression measure are introduced that allow one to specifically quantify the strength of an interaction mechanism in a well interpretable way that enables the exclusion of misleading effects of serial correlation as well as more general dependencies. The potential of the approach to quantify interactions between two and more processes is demonstrated by investigating teleconnections of ENSO and the mechanism of the Walker circulation. The article is intended to serve as a guideline to interpret lagged correlations and regressions in the presence of autocorrelation and introduces a powerful approach to analyze time delays and the strength of an interaction mechanism.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Ammermann, Peter A., and Douglas M. Patterson. "The cross-sectional and cross-temporal universality of nonlinear serial dependencies: Evidence from world stock indices and the Taiwan Stock Exchange." Pacific-Basin Finance Journal 11, no. 2 (April 2003): 175–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0927-538x(02)00113-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

de Vries, Meinou H., Karl Magnus Petersson, Sebastian Geukes, Pienie Zwitserlood, and Morten H. Christiansen. "Processing multiple non-adjacent dependencies: evidence from sequence learning." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367, no. 1598 (July 19, 2012): 2065–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0414.

Full text
Abstract:
Processing non-adjacent dependencies is considered to be one of the hallmarks of human language. Assuming that sequence-learning tasks provide a useful way to tap natural-language-processing mechanisms, we cross-modally combined serial reaction time and artificial-grammar learning paradigms to investigate the processing of multiple nested (A 1 A 2 A 3 B 3 B 2 B 1 ) and crossed dependencies (A 1 A 2 A 3 B 1 B 2 B 3 ), containing either three or two dependencies. Both reaction times and prediction errors highlighted problems with processing the middle dependency in nested structures (A 1 A 2 A 3 B 3 _B 1 ), reminiscent of the ‘missing-verb effect’ observed in English and French, but not with crossed structures (A 1 A 2 A 3 B 1 _B 3 ). Prior linguistic experience did not play a major role: native speakers of German and Dutch—which permit nested and crossed dependencies, respectively—showed a similar pattern of results for sequences with three dependencies. As for sequences with two dependencies, reaction times and prediction errors were similar for both nested and crossed dependencies. The results suggest that constraints on the processing of multiple non-adjacent dependencies are determined by the specific ordering of the non-adjacent dependencies (i.e. nested or crossed), as well as the number of non-adjacent dependencies to be resolved (i.e. two or three). Furthermore, these constraints may not be specific to language but instead derive from limitations on structured sequence learning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kuhlmann, Marco. "Mildly Non-Projective Dependency Grammar." Computational Linguistics 39, no. 2 (June 2013): 355–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/coli_a_00125.

Full text
Abstract:
Syntactic representations based on word-to-word dependencies have a long-standing tradition in descriptive linguistics, and receive considerable interest in many applications. Nevertheless, dependency syntax has remained something of an island from a formal point of view. Moreover, most formalisms available for dependency grammar are restricted to projective analyses, and thus not able to support natural accounts of phenomena such as wh-movement and cross–serial dependencies. In this article we present a formalism for non-projective dependency grammar in the framework of linear context-free rewriting systems. A characteristic property of our formalism is a close correspondence between the non-projectivity of the dependency trees admitted by a grammar on the one hand, and the parsing complexity of the grammar on the other. We show that parsing with unrestricted grammars is intractable. We therefore study two constraints on non-projectivity, block-degree and well-nestedness. Jointly, these two constraints define a class of “mildly” non-projective dependency grammars that can be parsed in polynomial time. An evaluation on five dependency treebanks shows that these grammars have a good coverage of empirical data.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Abels, Klaus. "Towards a restrictive theory of (remnant) movement!" Linguistic Variation Yearbook 2007 7 (December 31, 2007): 53–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/livy.7.04abe.

Full text
Abstract:
A restrictive theory of syntax needs both a restrictive theory of structures and a restrictive theory of operations.Much recent effort has gone into narrowing the class of allowable structures and a lot has been learned. This paper proposes that operations are linearly ordered on an essentially constituent by constituent basis. A universal constraint on the ordering of operations in language is proposed whose function is to fix the order in which operations apply. This constraint is deployed using a generalized prohibition against improper movement. The proposal captures some but not all effects of what has traditionally been called the freezing principle. It is argued that empirically exactly the right cut is made. It is further argued that the proposal rules out an entire class of remnant movement derivations, including the analysis of cross-serial dependencies in Nilsen (2003) and the analysis of order-preservation in Koopman and Szabolcsi (2000).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Schubert, Christian, Mechthild Neises, Kurt Fritzsche, Willi Geser, Francisco M. Ocana-Peinado, Dietmar Fuchs, Ralf Hass, Gerhard Schmid-Ott, and Christina Burbaum. "Preliminary Evidence on the Direction of Effects Between Day-to-Day Changes in Cellular Immune Activation, Fatigue and Mood in a Patient with Prior Breast Cancer: A Time-Series Analysis Approach." Pteridines 18, no. 1 (February 2007): 139–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/pteridines.2007.18.1.139.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This study on a patient with prior breast cancer investigated the cause-effect relations between immune system activation, fatigue and mood under real-life conditions. The 60-year old woman (primary diagnosis 5 years ago, relapse one year ago, currently no signs of recurrence) collected her entire urine for 31 days in 12 hour intervals for the determination of immune activation marker neopterin and creatinine concentrations by HPLC. This study used only the daytime urinary neopterin concentrations (from approx. 8 a.m. to 8 p.m.) and cross-correlated them with daily data from mood (3-Skalen-EWL) and fatigue (VAS) questionnaires which the patient filled in for 31 days. Serial dependencies were controlled for by applying moving average smoothing. When averaged over the whole observation period, daily fatigue, daily mood and daily urinary neopterin (average 173 μmol per mol creatinine) were within normal limits. Cross-correlational analyses revealed that increases in urinary neopterin significantly preceded increases in fatigue by 24 hours (lag 1: r = +0.424; p <0.05). The opposite direction of effects, however, existed between mood and neopterin, i.e., decreases in mood preceded increases in neopterin by 96 hours (lag 4: r = −0.697; p <0.05). The results in this patient suggest a disturbed stress system activity where negative emotions are associated with increases instead of decreases in the degree of cellular immune activation and where cellular immune activation may trigger central nervous system symptoms such as fatigue. Despite some restrictions in data quality this study shows the feasibility of gathering evidence on naturally occurring psychoimmunological cause-effect relations based on extensive serial data collection and time series analysis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Cross-serial dependencies"

1

Dehdari, Jonathan M. "Crossing Dependencies in Persian." Diss., CLICK HERE for online access, 2006. http://contentdm.lib.byu.edu/ETD/image/etd1441.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Cross-serial dependencies"

1

"Cross-Serial Dependencies in Dutch." In The Syntactic Process. The MIT Press, 2001. http://dx.doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6591.003.0009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Cross-serial dependencies"

1

Rentier, Gerrit. "Dutch cross serial dependencies in HPSG." In the 15th conference. Morristown, NJ, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 1994. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/991250.991278.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Vogel, Carl, Ulrike Hahn, and Holly Branigan. "Cross-serial dependencies are not hard to process." In the 16th conference. Morristown, NJ, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/992628.992658.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography