To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Cuddy.

Journal articles on the topic 'Cuddy'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Cuddy.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Pan, Yanfang, Hao Liu, Wenchao Yang, Bo Zhang, Hongqun Tang, Shuai Liu, and Yongzhong Zhan. "Phase equilibria of the Cu–Dy–Ti ternary system at 973 K." Powder Diffraction 30, no. 3 (June 9, 2015): 218–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0885715615000287.

Full text
Abstract:
The solid-state phase equilibria of the copper (Cu)–dysprosium (Dy)–titanium (Ti) ternary system at 973 K has been experimentally investigated. The existence of nine binary compounds, Cu4Ti, Cu3Ti2, Cu4Ti3, CuTi, CuTi2, CuTi3, CuDy, Cu2Dy, and Cu5Dy was confirmed. The controversial phase of CuTi3 was found in this work. The temperature range of Cu7Dy was determined to be from 1112 to 1183 K. The phase relations at 973 K are governed by ten ternary phase regions, 21 binary phase regions, and 12 single-phase regions. The solid solubility of Cu in Dy is undetectable. None of the other phase in this system reveals a remarkable homogeneity range at 973 K.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Steinberg, Jacob M., Noel A. Pelland, and Charles C. Eriksen. "Observed Evolution of a California Undercurrent Eddy." Journal of Physical Oceanography 49, no. 3 (March 2019): 649–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1175/jpo-d-18-0033.1.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractA California Undercurrent eddy (Cuddy) was repeatedly surveyed using multiple Seagliders for over three months. Found and tracked off of the Washington–Vancouver Island coasts, this Cuddy traveled over 400 km, remaining between the 1000- and 2000-m isobaths, as it was swept along in poleward flow of the California Current System. Three Seagliders made repeat bisecting transects of the Cuddy core capturing its detailed three-dimensional structure in time. Its evolution was analyzed through comparison of 11 independent Cuddy “snapshots.” A two dimensional Gaussian model fit to the geopotential anomaly field for each snapshot allowed computation of dynamic fields inaccessible in Seaglider profiles alone. Results indicate that the Cuddy decayed as its core waters became less isolated over time: Cuddy total mechanical energy (kinetic + potential), salt content, and the magnitude of the core potential vorticity anomaly decreased. Core spice and dissolved oxygen variance increased tenfold, and thermohaline fine structure, suggestive of lateral intrusions, was observed progressively closer to the eddy core. The estimated gradient-wind balanced velocity field similarly weakened as the Rossby number decreased to 0.32 from an initial value of 0.48. The observed changes in eddy properties occurred as the Cuddy was exposed to changes in the background stratification and Coriolis parameter as it translated alongshore. Idealized modeling of eddy adjustment indicates that both erosion and changing background conditions are required to explain the observed eddy changes. Adjustment in response to both effects simultaneously leads to changes in eddy properties qualitatively consistent with those observed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Bye, Hege H., and Henrik Herrebrøden. "Emotions as mediators of the stereotype–discrimination relationship: A BIAS map replication." Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 21, no. 7 (April 5, 2017): 1078–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430217694370.

Full text
Abstract:
A central theoretical assumption in the Behaviors from Intergroup Affect and Stereotypes (BIAS) map framework is that emotions mediate the relationships between stereotypes and intergroup behavior. Despite the BIAS map’s popularity, very few studies have tested the model’s mediation hypotheses and none have tested them by replicating the original study (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007, Study 1). We provide a replication in a Norwegian sample ( N = 244). The results supported that stereotype content is related to behavior tendencies, mediated through emotional prejudices. However, for each of the four behavior outcomes the effect of stereotype content was mediated through one emotion rather than two as predicted by the BIAS map. Our findings both converge and diverge from those of Cuddy and colleagues, and provide support for theoretical propositions unsupported by the original study. Overall, the study provides empirical support for the BIAS map framework and its cross-cultural validity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gifford, James. "Modernism: Keywords by Melba Cuddy-Keane, Adam Hammond, and Alexandra PeatMelba Cuddy-Keane, Adam Hammond, and Alexandra Peat. Modernism: Keywords. Wiley Blackwell. xviii, 266. $112.95." University of Toronto Quarterly 85, no. 3 (August 2016): 488–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.3138/utq.85.3.488.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Jakobson, Lorna S., Lola L. Cuddy, and Andrea R. Kilgour. "Time Tagging: A Key to Musicians' Superior Memory." Music Perception 20, no. 3 (2003): 307–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2003.20.3.307.

Full text
Abstract:
Years of formal music training and proficiency at a nonmusical skill—— verbal recall——are surprisingly associated (e.g., A. R. Kilgour, L. S. Jakobson, & L. L. Cuddy, 2000). The present study proposes an indirect mechanism to account for this association. It is proposed that music training strengthens auditory temporal-order processing, and that temporalorder processing then mediates the relationship between years of music training and prose recall.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Snaith, Anna. "Modernism: Keywords ed. by Melba Cuddy-Keane, Adam Hammond, Alexandra Peat." Modernism/modernity 23, no. 2 (2016): 478–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/mod.2016.0039.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Gifford, James. "Modernism: Keywords by Melba Cuddy-Keane, Adam Hammond, and Alexandra Peat." ESC: English Studies in Canada 42, no. 1-2 (2016): 244–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/esc.2016.0012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

박현순. "The Life of Korean-American, Susan Ahn Cuddy and her Gender Legacy." Women and History ll, no. 31 (December 2019): 283–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.22511/women..31.201912.283.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Tranter, Samuel. "Book Review: Romanus Cessario OP, Theology and Sanctity, ed. Cajetan Cuddy OP." Studies in Christian Ethics 29, no. 4 (October 10, 2016): 492–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0953946816658723a.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Sevillano, Verónica, and Susan T. Fiske. "Stereotypes, emotions, and behaviors associated with animals: A causal test of the stereotype content model and BIAS map." Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 22, no. 6 (September 2019): 879–900. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430219851560.

Full text
Abstract:
Using the stereotype content model (SCM; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002) and the behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes (BIAS) map (Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2007), two experiments tested the effect of animal stereotypes on emotions and behavioral tendencies toward animals. As a novel approach, Study 1 ( N = 165) manipulated warmth and competence traits of a fictitious animal species (“wallons”) and tested their effect on emotions and behaviors toward those animals. Stereotypical warm-competent and cold-incompetent “wallons” elicited fondness/delight and contempt/disgust, respectively. Cold-competent “wallons” primarily elicited threat but not awe. Warm-incompetent “wallons” were elusive targets, not eliciting specific emotions. The warmth dimension determined active behaviors, promoting facilitation (support/help) and reducing harm (kill/trap). The competence dimension determined passive behaviors, eliciting facilitation (conserve/monitor) and reducing harm (ignore/let them die off). Study 2 ( N = 112) tested the relation between animal stereotypes for 25 species and realistic scenarios concerning behavioral tendencies toward animals. Similar to Study 1, stereotypically warm (vs. cold) animals matched with active scenarios, eliciting more facilitation (i.e., national health campaign) but less harm (i.e., fighting animals). Stereotypically competent (vs. incompetent) animals matched with passive scenarios, eliciting more facilitation (i.e., restricted areas) but less harm (i.e., accidental mortality). Accordingly, stereotypes limited the suitability of scenarios toward animals. Although findings are consistent with the SCM/BIAS map framework, several unpredicted results emerged. The mixed support is discussed in detail, along with the implications of an intergroup approach to animals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

SEERY-MURPHY, STEPHANIE. "A Review of: “Melba Cuddy-Keane.Virginia Woolf, the Intellectual, & the Public Sphere”." Women's Studies 34, no. 3-4 (April 2005): 345–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00497870590964237.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Wise, Karen J., and John A. Sloboda. "Establishing an empirical profile of self-defined “tone deafness”: Perception, singing performance and self-assessment." Musicae Scientiae 12, no. 1 (March 2008): 3–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/102986490801200102.

Full text
Abstract:
Research has suggested that around 17% of Western adults self-define as “tone deaf” (Cuddy, Balkwill, Peretz & Holden, 2005). But questions remain about the exact nature of tone deafness. One candidate for a formal definition is “congenital amusia” (Peretz et al., 2003), characterised by a dense music-specific perceptual deficit. However, most people self-defining as tone deaf are not congenially amusic (Cuddy et al., 2005). According to Sloboda, Wise and Peretz (2005), the general population defines tone deafness as perceived poor singing ability, suggesting the need to extend investigations to production abilities and self-perceptions. The present research aims to discover if self-defined tone deaf people show any pattern of musical difficulties relative to controls, and to offer possible explanations for them ( e.g. perceptual, cognitive, productive, motivational). 13 self-reporting “tone deaf” (TD) and 17 self-reporting “not tone deaf” (NTD) participants were assessed on a range of measures for musical perception, cognition, memory, production and self-ratings of performance. This paper reports on four measures to assess perception (Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia), vocal production (songs and pitch-matching) and self-report. Results showed that the TD group performed significantly less well than the NTD group in all measures, but did not demonstrate the dense deficits characteristic of “congenital amusics”. Singing performance was influenced by context, with both groups performing better when accompanied than unaccompanied. The TD group self-rated the accuracy of their singing significantly lower than the NTD group, but not disproportionately so, and were less confident in their vocal quality. The TD participants are not facing an insurmountable difficulty, but are likely to improve with targeted intervention.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Eerola, Tuomas, Topi Jäärvinen, Jukka Louhivuori, and Petri Toiviainen. "Statistical Features and Perceived Similarity of Folk Melodies." Music Perception 18, no. 3 (2001): 275–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2001.18.3.275.

Full text
Abstract:
Listeners are sensitive to pitch distributional information in music (N. Oram & L. L. Cuddy, 1995; C. L. Krumhansl, J. Louhivuori, P.Toiviainen, T. Jäärvinen, & T. Eerola, 1999). However, it is uncertain whether frequency-based musical features are sufficient to explain the similarity judgments that underlie listeners' classification processes. A similarity rating experiment was designed to determine the effectiveness of these features in predicting listeners' similarity ratings. The material consisted of 15 melodies representing five folk music styles. A multiple regression analysis showed that the similarity of frequency-based musical properties could account for a moderate amount (40%%) of listeners' similarity ratings. A slightly better predictive rate (55%%) was achieved by using descriptive variables such as number of tones, rhythmic variability, and melodic predictability. The results suggest that both measures were able to capture some aspects of the structures that portray common salient dimensions to which listeners pay attention while categorizing melodies. Aikaisemmissa tutkimuksissa on osoitettu, ettää musiikin tilastollisilla tapahtumilla, kuten säävelten määäärillää ja tyypillisillää intervalleilla, on merkitystää, kun kuulijat muodostavat kääsityksiääään musiikin rakenteesta (N. Oram & L. L. Cuddy, 1995; C. L. Krumhansl, J. Louhivuori, P. Toiviainen, T. Jäärvinen, & T. Eerola, 1999). Nääiden piirteiden voidaan olettaa olevan täärkeitää myöös musiikin luokittelussa. Toistaiseksi ei kuitenkaan tiedetää, miten hyvin tilastollisilla piirteillää voitaisiin musiikin luokittelua selittääää. Täätää testattiin kuulijoille jäärjestetyn samanlaisuusarviointitehtäävään avulla. Tutkimuksen materiaali koostui 15 melodiasta, jotka edustivat viittää eri kansanmusiikkityyliää. Regressioanalyysi paljasti, ettää musiikin tilastollisten piirteiden samanlaisuus pystyi selittäämääään kohtuullisen määäärään (40%%) kuulijoiden antamista samanlaisuusarvioista. Hieman parempi selitysaste (55%%) saavutettiin kuvaavilla muuttujilla, joita olivat melodian laajuus ja ennakoitavuus sekää rytmin vaihtelevuus. Nääin ollen tulokset antavat aiheen olettaa, ettää musiikin tilastolliset piirteet ja kuvailevat muuttujat vaikuttavat kuulijoiden luokittelupäääätööksiin.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Wood, Andelys. "St. Cuthbert in Our Time: The Wind Eye and Cuddy." Children's Literature Association Quarterly 24, no. 1 (1999): 40–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/chq.0.1197.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

DE SÁ, RAFAEL O., JOÃO F. RIVA TONINI, HANNAH VAN HUSS, HUSSAM ZAHER, and CÉLIO F. B. HADDAD. "The unique traits of the subgenus Unicus within Chiasmocleis Méhely, 1094 (Anura: Microhylidae)." Zootaxa 4646, no. 3 (July 25, 2019): 585–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.11646/zootaxa.4646.3.8.

Full text
Abstract:
Chiasmocleis is the most speciose genus of Neotropical microhylids. The genus consists of three monophyletic clades that were recently recognized as subgenera. Within Chiasmocleis, the subgenus Unicus has a basal position in the phylogeny and contains a single species that occurs in the North Atlantic Forest of Brazil, isolated from the ranges of other Chiasmocleis. However, the subgenus Unicus lacked a formal description and consequently so far was a nomen nudum. Herein, we provide a diagnosis (i.e., morphological and molecular apomorphies) of the subgenus Unicus de Sá, Tonini, van Huss, Long, Cuddy, Forlani, Peloso, Zaher and Haddad, 2019b; for convenience, the name has also been registered in ZooBank.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Sevillano, Verónica, and Susan T. Fiske. "Animals as Social Objects." European Psychologist 21, no. 3 (July 2016): 206–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000268.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Nonhuman animals are typically excluded from the scope of social psychology. This article presents animals as social objects – targets of human social responses – overviewing the similarities and differences with human targets. The focus here is on perceiving animal species as social groups. Reflecting the two fundamental dimensions of humans’ social cognition – perceived warmth (benign or ill intent) and competence (high or low ability), proposed within the Stereotype Content Model ( Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002 ) – animal stereotypes are identified, together with associated prejudices and behavioral tendencies. In line with human intergroup threats, both realistic and symbolic threats associated with animals are reviewed. As a whole, animals appear to be social perception targets within the human sphere of influence and a valid topic for research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Nauts, Sanne, Oliver Langner, Inge Huijsmans, Roos Vonk, and Daniël H. J. Wigboldus. "Forming Impressions of Personality." Social Psychology 45, no. 3 (May 1, 2014): 153–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000179.

Full text
Abstract:
Asch’s seminal research on “Forming Impressions of Personality” (1946) has widely been cited as providing evidence for a primacy-of-warmth effect, suggesting that warmth-related judgments have a stronger influence on impressions of personality than competence-related judgments (e.g., Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007 ; Wojciszke, 2005 ). Because this effect does not fit with Asch’s Gestalt-view on impression formation and does not readily follow from the data presented in his original paper, the goal of the present study was to critically examine and replicate the studies of Asch’s paper that are most relevant to the primacy-of-warmth effect. We found no evidence for a primacy-of-warmth effect. Instead, the role of warmth was highly context-dependent, and competence was at least as important in shaping impressions as warmth.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Kutlu, Ethan, and Caroline Wiltshire. "Where do negative stereotypes come from? The case of Indian English in the USA." Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 5, no. 1 (March 23, 2020): 74. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v5i1.4669.

Full text
Abstract:
Language attitudes inform social stereotyping, which in turn affects linguistic judgments (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick 2007). Nonstandard varieties are particularly subject to negative stereotypes, being evaluated as “less friendly” and “hard to understand” (Giles & Watson 2013). In this study, we investigate attitudes towards Indian English, a variety of English spoken by one of the largest immigrant populations in the USA (approximately 2.4 million), to understand the roots of linguistic stereotyping towards this variety of English. We compared attitudes of American English speakers towards Indian English and British English. Our results show that while American English speakers do not explicitly indicate any communication problem with Indian English, they disfavor Indian English compared to British English. This disfavoring of Indian English aligns with Raciolinguistic theories, suggesting that post-colonialism, especially Whiteness, is a factor in language prestige and how different varieties are perceived.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Schellenberg, E. Glenn. "Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy." Music Perception 14, no. 3 (1997): 295–318. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285723.

Full text
Abstract:
Results from previous investigations indicate that the implication-realization (I-R) model (Narmour, 1990) of expectancy in melody may be overspecified and more complex than necessary. Indeed, Schellenberg's (1996) revised model, with two fewer predictor variables, improved predictive accuracy compared with the original model. A reanalysis of data reported by Cuddy and Lunney (1995) provided similar results. When the principles of the I-R model were submitted to a principal- components analysis, a solution containing three orthogonal (uncorrelated) factors retained the accuracy of the model but was inferior to the revised model. A separate principal-components analysis of the predictors of the revised model yielded a two-factor solution that did not compromise the revised model's predictive power. Consequently, an even simpler model of melodic expectancy was derived. These results provide further evidence that redundancy in the I-R model can be eliminated without loss of predictive accuracy.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Gagnon, Lise, Nathalie Gosselin, Véronique Provencher, and Nathalie Bier. "Perception and Emotional Judgments of Music in Dementia of the Alzheimer Type: A Short Case Study." Music Perception 29, no. 5 (June 1, 2012): 509–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.29.5.509.

Full text
Abstract:
dementia of the alzheimer type (ad) can affect emotional judgments of facial expression and prosody. Spared emotional judgments of music have been observed in early AD (Gagnon, Peretz, & Fülöp, 2009). The AD case study of Cuddy and Duffin (2005) showed relatively spared perception and memory for music. Through the single case study of MD, we again address the question of whether early AD might leave these abilities intact. The first experiment examined her musical emotional judgments in relation to mode and tempo. Overall results replicated the finding of a preserved response to changes in these structural properties. In a second experiment, we examined MD's recognition of musical and facial emotional expressions. She demonstrated a deficit only for the recognition of the facial expression of anger. Her performance adds to other empirical demonstrations that in early AD music perception and memory, as well as musical emotional judgments, may be relatively preserved.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Hassan, Bakhtaver, Mahua Bhattacharjee, and Shabir A. Wani. "Growth Optima and Supply-Chain of the Walnut Crop in India." Asian Journal of Agriculture and Rural Development 10, no. 2 (September 14, 2020): 708–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.18488/journal.ajard.2020.102.708.720.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper intends to study the Spatio-temporal growth of the walnut crop in Jammu & Kashmir, which holds a monopoly in walnut production in India. It also aims to assess the efficiency of the existing marketing channels of the walnut-crop in the region. A multi-stage random selection technique was used to collect primary data from three major walnut producing districts to identify the existing marketing channels and estimate their respective efficiencies. Compound-Annual-Growth-rate and Cuddy-Della-Valle index was used to estimate the growth of the walnut crop. Shepherd’s Marketing Efficiency Index was used to estimate the marketing efficiencies of the channels involved in the marketing of the crop. This paper found out very-high variability and slow growth in acreage, very-high variability, and high growth in production as well as in yield-per-hectare of the walnut crop.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Thompson, William F., and Lola L. Cuddy. "Perceived Key Movement in Four-Voice Harmony and Single Voices." Music Perception 9, no. 4 (1992): 427–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285563.

Full text
Abstract:
Listeners with a moderate amount of musical training rated the distance between the first and final key of short chorale excerpts under one of four presentation conditions. The distance between keys, or modulation distance, was either zero, one, or two steps in either the clockwise or counterclockwise direction on the cycle of fifths. Presentation conditions were four-voice harmonic sequences excerpted from the complete set of Bach chorales, single voices of the latter sequences, four-voice harmonic sequences simplified to block chords, and single voices of the latter sequences. Consistent with earlier findings (Thompson & Cuddy, 1989), judgments for both four- voice harmonic presentations and single-voice presentations revealed a close correspondence between modulation distance and judged distance. Ratings for harmonic sequences within a given key distance, however, showed influences of direction of modulation and of harmonic progression that were not reflected in ratings for single voices. The findings suggest that harmony and melody follow somewhat different principles in the process of identifying key change.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Ali Shah, Syed Asghar, and Alamgir Khalil Khalil. "Growth and Variability of Major Food Crops Production in Khyber Pakhtunkhwa, Pakistan." Journal of Applied and Advanced Research 2, no. 1 (February 8, 2017): 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.21839/jaar.2017.v2i1.39.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study was undertaken to investigate the growth and variability in major food crops production of Khyber Pakhtunkhwa. The study was based on secondary data, covers a period of about 30 years i.e. starting from 1984-85 to 2013-14, Whereas, the growth models has been employed to fit the best growth model and Cuddy Della Vella Index was applied to find variability in major food crops production i.e. wheat, maize, sugarcane and rice. Based on the results of analyzed data, it was found that in major food crops (wheat, maize, sugarcane, rice) Production, the growth models i.e. Cubic growth model, power growth model, cubic growth model, cubic growth model respectively were found suitable, based on the R2 criteria and fitted trend line. After selecting best fitted model for each major food crop, the growth rate was calculated by using the selected fitted models which were found to be 10.97%, 8.00%, 45.31% and 1.19% respectively. Moreover, the variability for each major food crop production was found to be 1.53%, 1.23%, 0.44% and 0.79% respectively.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Duran, Marek J. "Thomas and the Thomists: The Achievement of Thomas Aquinas and His Interpreters. By Romanus Cessario, O.P. and Cajetan Cuddy, O.P." Journal of the Society of Christian Ethics 39, no. 2 (2019): 423–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/jsce201939235.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Music, Graham. "Babies and bathwaters: attachment, neuroscience, evolution and the left." Soundings 73, no. 73 (December 1, 2019): 111–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.3898/soun.73.09.2019.

Full text
Abstract:
This article challenges thinkers and activists on the left who are over-suspicious of ideas heralding from disciplines such as interpersonal neurobiology, attachment theory, developmental psychology, and perhaps especially, evolutionary theory. Although scepticism is frequently warranted, especially as such discourses are often co-opted for neoliberal or far right ends, there is much in all of them that melds well with critiques of hegemonic social orders, providing potential fuel for those working for social change. Much work, for example that of Amy Cuddy, can be interpreted both conservatively and progressively. Work from within an attachment theory paradigm can play a crucial part in the battle of ideas: it has a huge amount to teach about how to create a more humane and egalitarian world, and in countering neoliberal beliefs that humans are innately primarily aggressive, competitive or selfish, or have selfish genes. The days are now over when the biological, psychological and the social need to be pitted against each other. Rather, they now have to be seen as mutually constituted. The brain is a social organ, embedded, embodied, enactive and extended, in large part a reflection of the social conditions in which it grows.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Kolar, Prasanna, P. K. Awasthi, and Ankita Sahu. "An economic analysis of cost, return and profitability of groundnut across leading states of India." INTERNATIONAL RESEARCH JOURNAL OF AGRICULTURAL ECONOMICS AND STATISTICS 11, no. 2 (September 15, 2020): 278–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.15740/has/irjaes/11.2/278-284.

Full text
Abstract:
The current study was undertaken with the objectives to estimate the dynamics of cost, returns, profitability and break-even production of groundnut across the leading states of India. For that secondary data were collected from Directorate of Economics and Statistics for the period 1996-97 to 2015-16. For statistical analysis tools like relative change, compound growth rate and cuddy della valle index were used. The study found that Cost A1, cost A2, cost B1 and cost C1 of groundnut in Gujarat and cost B2, cost C2 and cost C2 revised in Andhra Pradesh were found to be increased at higher annual growth rate during the study period than any other states. These cost concepts showed high instability for almost all the leading states. Groundnut was more profitable in Gujarat due to higher net income and B:C ratio than other states. Differential yield and the difference between the cost of production and minimum support price were found better among Gujarat and Tamil Nadu states. In other states, the actual yield was lower than break-even and the cost of production was higher than minimum support price leading to the losses for the groundnut growers.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Vijgen, Jörgen. "Thomas and the Thomists: The Achievement of Thomas Aquinas and His Interpreters ed. by Romanus Cessario, O.P., and Cajetan Cuddy, O.P." Nova et vetera 17, no. 1 (2019): 290–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/nov.2019.0017.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Turner, Jennifer R., and Jennifer T. Stanley. "YOUNG ADULTS STEREOTYPE OLDER SPEAKERS WHO ADOPTED A POWER POSE AS LESS COMPETENT COMPARED TO SUBMISSIVE OR CONTROL." Innovation in Aging 3, Supplement_1 (November 2019): S80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igz038.310.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Young adults (YA) frequently endorse age stereotypes (Levy, 2009). We examined whether older adult (OA) speakers influenced by embodied-cognition (“power posing”; Cuddy et al., 2015) would reduce YAs’ stereotype-related judgments. Following the Stereotype Content Model (SCM; Fiske et al., 2002), we hypothesized that OA who held a power pose prior to giving their speech would be rated as higher in Competency, Performance, and Electability, but not Warmth. Sixty-three YA viewed and rated 9 videos of OA performing speeches after modeling a pose (power, submissive, control). Within-subjects ANOVAs revealed embodiment condition differences for Performance (F2,124 = 207.76, ηp2 = .77). For ratings of Performance, speakers in the power condition were judged worse than either submissive or control (ps < .001). For Warmth ratings, power (M = 4.81, SD = .62) was worse than control (M = 5.07, SD = .89, p = .003, d = .34), but submissive (M = 4.97, SD = .87) was not significantly different from either group. These results suggest that YA may judge the Performance and Warmth of OA who adopted a power pose harsher because OA are not supposed to be powerful or adopt expansive postures (consistent with the SCM). In comparison, YA may be drawing upon the Representativeness Heuristic of OA in positions of power (e.g., Senators) when rating Electability and Competence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Pearce, Marcus T., and Geraint A. Wiggins. "Expectation in Melody: The Influence of Context and Learning." Music Perception 23, no. 5 (June 2006): 377–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2006.23.5.377.

Full text
Abstract:
The Implication-Realization (IR) theory (Narmour, 1990) posits two cognitive systems involved in the generation of melodic expectations: The first consists of a limited number of symbolic rules that are held to be innate and universal; the second reflects the top-down influences of acquired stylistic knowledge. Aspects of both systems have been implemented as quantitative models in research which has yielded empirical support for both components of the theory (Cuddy & Lunny, 1995; Krumhansl, 1995a, 1995b; Schellenberg, 1996, 1997). However, there is also evidence that the implemented bottom-up rules constitute too inflexible a model to account for the influence of the musical experience of the listener and the melodic context in which expectations are elicited. A theory is presented, according to which both bottom-up and top-down descriptions of observed patterns of melodic expectation may be accounted for in terms of the induction of statistical regularities in existing musical repertoires. A computational model that embodies this theory is developed and used to reanalyze existing experimental data on melodic expectancy. The results of three experiments with increasingly complex melodic stimuli demonstrate that this model is capable of accounting for listeners’ expectations as well as or better than the two-factor model of Schellenberg (1997).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Abid, Saleem, M. Asif Masood, M. Zubair Anwar, Saleem Zahid, and Irum Raza. "Trends and Variability of Wheat Crop in Pakistan." Asian Journal of Agriculture and Rural Development 8, no. 2 (January 17, 2019): 153–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.18488/journal.1005/2018.8.2/1005.2.153.159.

Full text
Abstract:
The main focus of the study was to analyze trends and variability of wheat crop in Pakistan. Semi-log trend model was used to find trends and growth rate in area, yield and production of wheat crop whereas the variability was measured by Cuddy-Della Valle index of variability. The findings of the study illustrate that wheat area in Punjab, Sindh and Baluchistan was increased over the time whereas cultivated area of wheat in Khyber Pakhtunkhwa province was marginally decreased during 1981-85 to 2011-15. The results show that there was substantial increase in wheat yield and production in all four provinces of Pakistan. The increase in wheat yield may due to the adoption of new varieties of wheat in the country over the time. It was also concluded from the results that area and yield of wheat in Baluchistan recorded the highest degree of variability whereas in Punjab province area and yield of wheat crop were noticed the lowest degree of variability. Similarly, the maximum variability in wheat production was recorded for Baluchistan province followed by Sindh, Khyber Pakhtunkhwa, and Punjab. Mostly the variability in wheat production was due to the variability in wheat area and their yield.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Sudhakarrao, More Sachin, Narendra Singh, and B. K. Bhatt. "Performance of Pulses in Gujarat: A District level Assessment." Current Agriculture Research Journal 6, no. 1 (April 7, 2018): 45–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.12944/carj.6.1.06.

Full text
Abstract:
A research was conducted to assess the performance of pulses in one of the most important agriculture states of India. Performance of pulse crops was judged on two important parameters i.e. growth and instability. Compound growth rate was estimated by fitting non linear model to the area, production and productivity data for the period from 1970-71 to 2011-12. The fitted model was analyzed using Marquardt algorithm. Instability was assessed by employing Cuddy-Della Valle instability index. The results show that, the production of pulses increased in the state during the entire study period. The increase in pulse crops in the state was due to area expansion coupled with marginal improvement in yield up to the year 1990 after that, increase in production was mainly from improvement in the yield of pulse crops as area was stagnated. Area under pulse crops increased consistently up to year 1990 afterwards it was stagnated. Consistent improvement in the yield of pulses was a notable feature which shows that improved technology has payoff in the state. High growth in area, production and yield of pulse crops was associated with high level of instability during first sub period. Yield variability in pulse crops was relatively higher than area variations which clearly indicated that yield instability was a major source of variation in the production of pulses. Therefore attempt should be made to stabilize the yield level in pulse crops.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

S More, Sachin, K. V. Deshmukh, and R. V. Chavan. "Has Production of Cotton in Maharashtra Shown Stable Growth Over the Years ?" Current Agriculture Research Journal 8, no. 3 (December 28, 2020): 224–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.12944/carj.8.3.08.

Full text
Abstract:
The growth and instability in area, production and yield of cotton in Maharashtra was assessed before and after introduction of Bt cotton varieties. The study aims to know the growth behavior of cotton production over the years. The contribution of area and productivity towards cotton production was measured by decomposing the cotton production series. The method proposed by Minhas and Vidhyanathan and reframed by Sharma was employed. The compound growth rate was estimated on the basis of fit using non-linear model i.e. exponential. The fitted equation was estimated using marquardt algorithms. Cuddy-Della Valle instability index was employed to estimate the instability in area, production and yield of cotton. The analysis was carried out using SAS macro available on IASRI web site with the help of SAS 9.3 software. Results revealed that, area, production and productivity of cotton were increased significantly after the introduction of Bt hybrids technology. The enhancement in productivity of cotton after the adoption of Bt hybrids technology by farmers was associated with the moderate instability. The productivity of cotton was moderately fluctuating from one year to another year. These fluctuations were also seen in cotton production. Bt cotton hybrids improved the yield and production and in some extent were also responsible for area expansion but the yield and production performance of cotton after the introduction of Bt was not stable. Enhancement in yield was the major factor responsible for improvement in cotton production in Maharashtra state compared to area.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Mathiasen, R. L., J. R. Allison, and B. W. Geils. "Western Dwarf Mistletoe Parasitizing Colorado Blue Spruce and Norway Spruce in California." Plant Disease 82, no. 3 (March 1998): 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1094/pdis.1998.82.3.351e.

Full text
Abstract:
Western dwarf mistletoe (Arceuthobium campylopodum Engelm.), a common parasite of ponderosa pine (Pinus ponderosa Dougl. ex Laws.) and Jeffrey pine (Pinus jeffreyi Grev. & Balf.), was found parasitizing planted Colorado blue spruce (Picea pungens Engelm.) and Norway spruce (Picea abies (L.) H. Karsten) in Upper Cuddy Valley, CA (Kern County, T. 9 N., R. 21 W., Sec. 25). One tree greater than 6 m in height of each spruce species was infected and both trees were within 12 m of a Jeffrey pine severely infected with western dwarf mistletoe. Five to 10 branches were infected on each tree and a few of these had abundant mistletoe shoot production, which allowed identification of the parasite. This is the first report of western dwarf mistletoe on Colorado blue spruce. Although this is the first report of natural infection of Norway spruce in California, this mistletoe/host combination has been reported by Weir from artificial inoculation (2) and collected by Russell in central Washington (1). We recommend that these spruce species not be planted within 15 m of pines infected with western dwarf mistletoe. Specimens of western dwarf mistletoe on Colorado blue spruce and Norway spruce were collected and deposited at the Deaver Herbarium, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff. References: (1) F. G. Hawksworth and D. Wiens. 1996. Dwarf Mistletoes: Biology, Pathology, and Systematics. USDA Agric. Handb. 709. (2) J. R. Weir. Bot. Gaz. 56:1, 1918.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Bisconti, Toni, Jennifer Sublett, and Alison Chasteen. "Benevolent Ageism: Exploring Its Boundary Conditions, Generalizability, and Correlates." Innovation in Aging 4, Supplement_1 (December 1, 2020): 568–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geroni/igaa057.1882.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Ageism is one of the few prejudices that is still socially condoned (Nelson, 2016). Given the aging population and the impact of internalizing ageist thoughts, this construct needs to be at the forefront for scientific examination. The long-term effects of ageism, particularly negative self-perceptions, lead to negative health and cognitive outcomes (Chasteen et al., 2015; Levy et al., 2002). One of the intricate components of ageism, however, is that it is often “benevolent”. Cuddy and colleagues developed the Stereotype Content Model (SCM) to describe how individuals are categorized based on varying degrees of warmth and competence. Unlike many devalued members of society who are viewed as low on both, older adults are viewed as having high warmth and low competence, leading to more overaccommodative treatment. The goal of the present symposium is to overview the ways in which researchers have dissected this more nuanced type of ageism. Specifically, two of the presenters will cover some of the boundary conditions of understanding age-based stereotypes and their malleability, examining them across ages and across genders. Additionally, one of our presenters will overview the validation of the Ambivalent Ageism Scale on a Chinese sample, lending support to its generalizability. Finally, our last presenter will overview the relationship between benevolent ageism and self-compassion to predict metamemory, given the pervasive stereotype that older adults suffer from severe cognitive decline. Themes and implications of these presentations will be discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Görtschacher, Wolfgang. "‘I start again with every story, listening’: Sound, silence and voice in two short stories by David Constantine." Short Fiction in Theory & Practice 11, no. 1-2 (June 1, 2021): 73–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1386/fict_00037_1.

Full text
Abstract:
This article examines sound and the sonic aspects of voice and silence in two short stories by David Constantine – ‘Tea at the Midland’ and ‘Under the Dam’ – to show that they are not only relevant for an analysis of his poetry but also for his short stories. Employing Jonathan Sterne’s definition of sonic culture as a theoretical starting point, the phonotextual (Garrett Stewart) multiplicity of patterns in each text is seen as an alternative to the protagonists-focalizers’ ‘silenced’ situation and is associated with their desired joys in life. In ‘Tea at the Midland’ the withheld soundscape (R. Murray Schafer) of the bay can only be watched but not heard. In the opening of ‘Under the Dam’ the auscultator (Melba Cuddy-Keane) Seth is completely oblivious of his sonic surroundings and effaces sound on the story level, but the narrator reintroduces sound on the level of discourse. Sylvia Mieszkowski’s distinction between the sound of the text and the sound in the text constitutes one of the fundamental concepts of the analysis. The findings and conclusions are interpreted in the context of Constantine’s own poetics as regards the writing of short stories. The sounds of the two short stories reinforce, through metrical, rhythmic, syntactic and sound patterns, the scenes’ withheld sonic qualities that are only perceived visually and sensed emotionally by the protagonists. These soundscapes represent alternative worlds desired by the protagonists in ‘Under the Dam’ and by the woman in ‘Tea at the Midland’.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

West, Michael A. "Interview with Mr. Jack Cuddy, Director, and Mr. Roger Jacobs, Deputy Director, Range and Training Area Management Division, Training and Education Command, U.S. Marine Corps." Federal Facilities Environmental Journal 16, no. 2 (2005): 15–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ffej.20051.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

RYAN, DOMINIC. "THOMAS AND THE THOMISTS: THE ACHIEVEMENT OF THOMAS AQUINAS AND HIS INTERPRETERS by Romanus Cessario and Cajetan Cuddy, Fortress Press, Minneapolis, 2017, pp. xvii + 151, £28.99, pbk." New Blackfriars 100, no. 1085 (December 12, 2018): 122–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nbfr.12426.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

OP, VIVIAN BOLAND. "THEOLOGY AND SANCTITY by Romanus Cessario OP, edited by Cajetan Cuddy OP, Sapientia Press of Ave Maria University, Ave Maria, Florida, 2014, pp. xiii + 277, £20.00, pbk." New Blackfriars 97, no. 1072 (October 5, 2016): 742–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/nbfr.6_12239.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Brown, Helen, David Butler, and Mari Riess Jones. "Musical and Temporal Influences on Key Discovery." Music Perception 11, no. 4 (1994): 371–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285632.

Full text
Abstract:
The intervallic rivalry model of key identification is outlined and evaluated in two experiments that use a completion judgment task. Experiment 1 replicates an earlier experiment by Cuddy and Badertscher (1987), in which the rare-interval hypothesis of the intervallic rivalry model was considered. In the present study, listeners with different levels of musical training rated probe tones in the context of three different melodic patterns: arpeggiated major triads, ascending major scales, and arpeggiated diminished triads. Results of Experiment 1 indicated that in both the C major triadic and the C major scalar contexts, listeners gave higher completion ratings to all three probes that were members of the presented C major triad than to the other probes, with the exception of F, thereby producing a jagged (multipeaked) profile. For the diminished triadic context, listeners rated the single probe C, that which corresponds to the tonal center in major mode for that group of three tones, as the best completion. Experiment 2 tested the temporal-order hypothesis of the intervallic rivalry model by reordering tones in all three contexts. Again jagged tone profiles appeared with major triadic and major scalar contexts, although in the former the tone F, a perfect fifth below the root of the presented C major triad, received the best completion rating. A single-peaked function appeared with probes in the diminished triadic context, where the major-mode tonic garnered the highest rating found in all conditions of both experiments. Data are interpreted as support for both the rare-interval hypothesis and the temporal-order hypothesis derived from the intervallic rivalry model of key discovery. Complementary findings consistent with the tonal hierarchy model are also discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Daisley, Racquel. "Theophylline disposition following parenteral feeding of malnourished patients P G CUDDY, J F BEALER, E L LYMAN, ET AL Truman Medical Center and Children's Mercy Hospital, Kansas, Missouri." Nutrition in Clinical Practice 9, no. 4 (August 1994): 157–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/088453369400900411.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Yang, Yaping, Katherine R. G. White, Xinfang Fan, Qiang Xu, and Qing-Wei Chen. "Differences in Explicit Stereotype Activation among Social Groups Based on the Stereotype Content Model: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence in Chinese Sample." Brain Sciences 10, no. 12 (December 17, 2020): 1001. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10121001.

Full text
Abstract:
The stereotype content model (SCM; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu, 2002) identifies four basic categories of stereotyped social groups: high warmth-high competence (HW-HC), high warmth-low competence (HW-LC), low warmth-high competence (LW-HC), and low warmth-low competence (LW-LC). However, many of these groups have not been directly examined in stereotype activation research. The purpose of the present research was to extend stereotype activation research to groups that more fully represent those identified under the SCM. Employing explicit sequential priming task, participants responded to prime-target stimulus pairs that were either congruent or incongruent with stereotypes of social groups from all four SCM quadrants in two studies in the current investigation. Study 1 was to determine the behavioral pattern of explicit stereotype activation among four quadrants (the sample included 60 Chinese undergraduate students, 51%—female). Study 2 further employed event-related brain potentials (ERPs) technique to track the time course and electrophysiological underpinnings of explicit stereotype activation (the sample included 22 right-handed Chinese undergraduate students, 76%—female). In Study 1, participants responded more quickly and accurately on stereotype congruent trials than incongruent trials for all social groups except LW-LC groups. This reverse priming effect on LW-LC social groups in RTs was also replicated in Study 2. ERPs findings further showed that incongruent targets elicited larger N400 amplitudes than congruent targets for all four SCM quadrants. Moreover, congruent targets elicited larger P2 than incongruent targets, but only found for the LW-LC social groups. In addition, congruent targets elicited larger amplitudes of late positive component than incongruent targets for the low warmth (LW-LC and LW-HC) groups. Together, these results highlight the unique processing that LW-LC groups receive throughout the cognitive stream, ultimately manifesting in distinctive behavioral responses. Unconscious activation of egalitarian goals, disgust, and distrust accounts are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Негм, Мостафа, and Mostafa Negm. "MULTI-PARAMETRIC FORECASTING AND RELATIVE ADVANTAGES OF WHEAT CROP PRODUCTION IN THE REGION FOR SUSTAINABLE DEVELOPMENT." Vestnik of Kazan State Agrarian University 14, no. 2 (July 29, 2019): 164–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.12737/article_5d3e16286dd0b2.92483340.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper proposes a model for determining the relative advantage and forecasting the contribution to total production and reducing the deficit at the regional level, based on data and economic indicators for the period from 1995 to 2015. The main objective of this work is to predict future production trends in the regions, which are presented in order to improve land management, productivity, reduce agricultural waste and create a strategic stock of wheat to solve local supply problems. The study uses descriptive and statistical data analysis methods to achieve research objectives. Using linear trend analysis, growth rates of acreage, productivity, production and consumption of basic food crops are estimated. Using the adjusted coefficients of variation, that were proposed by Cuddy-Della Valle. The food problem is seen as an important strategic issue, that attracts close attention at all levels. Its importance stems from the political and social and economic aspects. The study is based on data from the Egyptian regions and has a strong regional focus. Geographically, Egypt is divided into four regions (areas of wheat cultivation). In this regard, we propose a conceptual approach to the study of the balance of regional production of food wheat within the framework of factors, affecting the food security of the regions (Figure 1). These stages are logically related to each other, since it is impossible to determine the prospects for the development of the domestic wheat market without analyzing its development within a certain retrospective period. Both of these stages imply a deep study of the wheat market, both on the basis of qualitative analysis methods and using quantitative analysis methods that make it possible to develop management decisions to ensure its balance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Nathan, D. M., M. Davis, P. Cleary, and J. Lachin. "Response to Cundy." Diabetes Care 24, no. 4 (April 1, 2001): 795–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.2337/diacare.24.4.795.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Sugiura, Yuta, Takeo Igarashi, and Masahiko Inami. "Cuddly User Interfaces." Computer 49, no. 7 (July 2016): 14–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/mc.2016.196.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Martz, John D., and David J. Myers. "J. Cudd Brown." PS: Political Science & Politics 29, no. 03 (September 1996): 545. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1049096500045261.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Larkin, Marilynn. "Cuddly cats could cause ulcers." Lancet 351, no. 9114 (May 1998): 1495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(05)78883-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Lu, Yin, Aditya Ramachandra, Minh Pham, Yi-Cheng Tu, and Feng Cheng. "CuDDI: A CUDA-Based Application for Extracting Drug-Drug Interaction Related Substance Terms from PubMed Literature." Molecules 24, no. 6 (March 19, 2019): 1081. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/molecules24061081.

Full text
Abstract:
Drug-drug interaction (DDI) is becoming a serious issue in clinical pharmacy as the use of multiple medications is more common. The PubMed database is one of the biggest literature resources for DDI studies. It contains over 150,000 journal articles related to DDI and is still expanding at a rapid pace. The extraction of DDI-related information, including compounds and proteins from PubMed, is an essential step for DDI research. In this paper, we introduce a tool, CuDDI (compute unified device architecture-based DDI searching), for identification of DDI-related terms (including compounds and proteins) from PubMed. There are three modules in this application, including the automatic retrieval of substances from PubMed, the identification of DDI-related terms, and the display of relationship of DDI-related terms. For DDI term identification, a speedup of 30–105 times was observed for the compute unified device architecture (CUDA)-based version compared with the implementation with a CPU-based Python version. CuDDI can be used to discover DDI-related terms and relationships of these terms, which has the potential to help clinicians and pharmacists better understand the mechanism of DDIs. CuDDI is available at: https://github.com/chengusf/CuDDI.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Horton, John. "For the love of cuddly toys." Children's Geographies 16, no. 4 (March 31, 2018): 446–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14733285.2018.1457735.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Hopkins, Cindy. "The cuddly side of infection control." Nursing Standard 18, no. 47 (August 4, 2004): 31. http://dx.doi.org/10.7748/ns.18.47.31.s49.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Willett, Cynthia. "Analyzing Oppression, by Ann Cudd." Radical Philosophy Review 10, no. 1 (2007): 91–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/radphilrev200710118.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography