Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Developmental psychology Differentiation (Developmental psychology) Child psychology'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Developmental psychology Differentiation (Developmental psychology) Child psychology.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Diederich, Marcia C. "Cultural determinants in Chinese and American preschool children's understanding of physical laws and social rules." Menomonie, WI : University of Wisconsin--Stout, 2008. http://www.uwstout.edu/lib/thesis/2008/2008diederichm.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Klein, Tamara Lynn Nezu Christine Maguth. "Differentiating between developmentally disabled child molesters and perpetrators of adult sexual molestation using penile plethysmography /." Philadelphia, Pa. : Drexel University, 2004. http://dspace.library.drexel.edu/handle/1860/400.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Gokaltun, Ayse Cici. "The impact of parental differentiation of self on positive family functioning." FIU Digital Commons, 2007. https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/3985.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this thesis was to investigate the impact of parental differentiation of self (less emotional reactivity, cutoff, fusion with others, and greater I position) on positive family functioning (family cohesion, adaptability, conflict and child attachment to mother). The participants included 47 mothers and their children between 5 and 13 years old who sought developmental assessment at the Youth and Family Developmental Program (YFDP) laboratory at Florida International University. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses revealed that less emotional reactivity predicted more family cohesion and adaptability, less emotional cutoff predicted more family cohesion, adaptability and less conflict, more fusion with others predicted more family cohesion and child attachment to mother, and greater I position predicted less family conflict. This study provides further support for parental influence on the family environment. Study results indicate that services for targeting families should target the parental differentiation of self as an influential factor for family functioning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Beurkens, Nicole M. "The impact of symptom severity on parent-child interaction and relationships among children with autism." ScholarWorks, 2010. https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations/812.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent estimates indicate that 1 in 100 children in the United States is diagnosed on the autism spectrum. Although research has demonstrated the bidirectional nature of parentchild relations, the effect of child autism symptom severity on parent-child interaction and overall relationships has not yet been explored. This study examined the impact of child symptom severity in children ages 4-15 years, as measured by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), on parent-child interaction, as measured by the Dyadic Coding Scale (DCS), and parent-child relationships, as measured by the Parent Child Relationship Inventory (PCRI). The bidirectional model of socialization informs the study, as it recognizes the mutual impact that parents and children have on one another in the context of parent-child relations. The sample included 25 parent-child dyads, each consisting of a child diagnosed on the autism spectrum and his/her primary parent. Relationships between autism symptom severity, parent-child interaction, and parent-child relationships were explored using multivariate regression analysis. Significant inverse relationships were found between autism symptom severity and parent-child interaction, but no relationship was found between symptom severity and parent-child relationship. Understanding the negative influence of autism symptom severity on parent-child interaction and the lack of impact on overall relationship allows for more effective treatment planning and monitoring of progress over time. Efforts to improve interactions and relationships for these children will lead to significant social change by increasing the effectiveness of treatment programs, enriching family relationships, and improving outcomes across the lifespan.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr, C. Shore, R. M. Bartlett, Page M. McIntyre, and K. E. Brakke. "Developmental Perspectives from the APA National Conference on Undergraduate Education in Psychology." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2009. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4934.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Perez, Daniela. "ADULTS’ KNOWLEDGE OF CHILD DEVELOPMENT AND CHILD GUIDANCE." CSUSB ScholarWorks, 2017. https://scholarworks.lib.csusb.edu/etd/438.

Full text
Abstract:
Decades of research studies suggest that the quality of parenting skills and parenting knowledge about children’s development have profound effects on children’s development. Studies to date show that most adults lack knowledge of child development and developmentally-appropriate child guidance. These studies, however have focused on white, middle-class, well-educated women and are limited in the range of issues addressed. The purpose of this study was to gain a more accurate understanding about adults’ knowledge of child development and child guidance by assessing female and male adults using an ethnically diverse population. Data from 705 adult participants showed that adults knew more than expected about child development, but lacked knowledge about developmentally-appropriate child guidance. Knowledge also varied by the amount of child development coursework taken. Results also showed that females were more knowledgeable than males, and knowledge varied somewhat by ethnicity with European-Americans being more knowledgeable in both child development and child guidance than other ethnic groups. These findings suggest that more work needs to be done to disseminate research- and evidence-based findings about child development and child guidance as the implications of this knowledge for child well-being are profound. The results of this study help to inform the design and content of parent education classes and related interventions to address gaps in knowledge and skills.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Syzdek, Brian M. "Adapting Parent-Child Interaction Therapy to Train Wilderness Therapy Camp Staff." Thesis, The Chicago School of Professional Psychology, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3637188.

Full text
Abstract:

Wilderness therapy camps have been found to be effective for treating a number of youth issues and for generally improving youth functioning. In addition, wilderness therapy camps appear to address current treatment needs of reducing stigma in treatment and providing other benefits, such as physical and social health benefits. However, currently wilderness therapy camps lack systematic training for staff that has been deemed efficacious, utilizing evidence-based techniques. Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT) is an evidence-based therapy (EBT) for use with children with a variety of issues and backgrounds and in diverse settings, useful for reducing child problematic behaviors. Efforts have been made to expand the use of PCIT in a variety of settings, with promising results.

This dissertation proposes to describe how PCIT might be adapted to train wilderness therapy camp staff in evidence-based methods for working with youth, especially those with mental health needs, such as behavioral issues. The literature concerning PCIT and wilderness therapy camps is reviewed. A needs assessment was conducted, consisting of interviews with key informants, experts in the field of wilderness therapy, PCIT, and training methods. Based on information obtained, a full program for training camp staff, called Counselor-Camper Interaction Training (CCIT) is proposed. Finally a proposal to evaluate the efficacy of this program is put forth. As part of the proposed evaluation, a financial assessment was conducted on the program, with the results presented.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Whitcher, Derick William. "The relationship between dyadic parent-child cohesion and adolescents' self-concept." Thesis, Alfred University, 2016. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10145712.

Full text
Abstract:

The current study investigated the relationships between mother-child, father-child, parent-cohesion, and children’s self-concept. Participants included 30 early adolescents (ages 10 to 15 years) from intact family arrangements. Children completed a modified version of the Family System Test (FAST) to assess levels of parent-child cohesion. The Piers-Harris 2 was used to evaluate dimensions of self-concept. A series of sequential multiple regressions were performed to determine the relationships parent-child cohesion variables had with self-concept constructs, after controlling for the children’s sex and typical grades. Only father-child cohesion scores significantly related to adolescents’ ratings on specific facets of self-concept (i.e., Behavioral Adjustment, Intellectual and School Status), suggesting that children who report feeling closer to their fathers also endorsed items reflecting less problematic behavior and more intellectual/academic competence. Findings suggest mothers and fathers contribute differentially to adolescents’ self-concept. Implications for parental education and ensuring children have a caring, male role model are discussed. Limitations to the study included issues with sample size, generalizability, and data collection.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Berman, Steven L. "Making life choices : facilitating identity formation in young adults." FIU Digital Commons, 1996. http://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/etd/1766.

Full text
Abstract:
This dissertation makes a contribution to the growing literature on identity formation by formulating, implementing, and testing the effectiveness of a psychosocial intervention, the Making Life Choices (MLC) Workshops, designed to facilitate the process of identity formation. More specifically, the MLC Workshops were designed to foster the development and use of critical cognitive and communicative skills and competencies in choosing and fulfilling life goals and values. The MLC Workshops consist of a psychosocial group intervention that includes both didactic and group experiential exercises. The primary research question for this study concerned the effectiveness of the MLC Workshop relative to a control condition. Effectiveness was evaluated on two levels: skills development and reduction of distress. First, the effectiveness of MLC in fostering the development of critical competencies was evaluated relative to a control condition, and no statistically significant differences were found. Second, the effectiveness of MLC in decreasing life distress was also evaluated relative to the control condition. While participants in the MLC workshop had no significant decrease in distress, they did have statistically significant improvement in life satisfaction in the Personal Domain.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bor, Elif. "Neighborhood quality, childcare quality, and children's early developmental outcomes." Fairfax, VA : George Mason University, 2007. http://hdl.handle.net/1920/2958.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (M.A.)--George Mason University, 2007.
Title from PDF t.p. (viewed Jan. 17, 2008). Thesis director: Adam Winsler. Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in Psychology. Vita: p. 90. Includes bibliographical references (p. 84-89). Also available in print.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Liles, Brandi D. "Social and emotional development in the young child| The effects of trauma exposure." Thesis, The University of Tulsa, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3622654.

Full text
Abstract:

Although the deleterious social and emotional effects of trauma exposure on adults and older children are well documented, less is known about the effects of trauma on young children. Preliminary studies have indicated that young children (a) are exposed to traumatic events (e.g. Perry et al., 1995; Mongillo et al., 2009) (b) experience social and emotional problems (e.g. Scheeringa et al., 2006; Bogat et al., 2006) and (c) are affected by the caregiver-child relationship (e.g. Osfosky, 2000). However, no known study has compared social and emotional problems among trauma-exposed young children to a comparison group of non trauma-exposed young children connected to mental health services. This study aimed to strengthen the research and clinical literature by examining unique social-emotional and trauma-specific symptoms of 47 trauma-exposed service-involved young children (18 to 69 months of age) compared to 25 non trauma-exposed, service-connected young children. Additionally, this study examined the predictive nature of child temperament and the caregiver-child relationship (i.e. caregiver affect, responsiveness and intrusiveness) on the severity of the child's internalizing symptoms. Seventy-two young children and their primary caregivers, mostly mothers (82%) participated in the current study. Trauma exposure, social/emotional problems, and trauma-specific symptoms were measured by a variety of caregiver reports and interviews. Further, the caregiver-child relationship was examined using the Crowell Modified Parent-Child Relationship Scale. Regarding trauma exposure, 65.2% of the sample was exposed to a traumatic event, with many children (54%) being exposed to multiple traumatic events. Among trauma-exposed children, approximately 10% were exhibiting trauma-related reactions. Among MANCOVA and ANCOVAs, no statistically significant differences emerged among trauma-exposed and non trauma-exposed young children for social and emotional problems, trauma-specific symptoms, or observed distress in the caregiver-child relationship. Child temperament and caregiver depressive symptoms statistically significantly predicted child outcomes. In a hierarchical multiple regression examining the predictive nature of the caregiver-child relationship, only child temperament emerged as a statistically significant predictor.

Despite null findings, this study served as a pilot study examining unique social/emotional problems among service-involved trauma-exposed and non trauma-exposed young children allowing future studies to determine recommended sample sizes (200 and above is needed). Issues related to assessment of trauma symptoms in young children are discussed. Recommendations are provided for clinicians. For example, both child temperament and caregiver distress should be a focus of assessment and intervention in trauma-exposed young children. Detailed recommendations are provided to improve the reliability and validity of the Modified Parent-Child Relationship Scale. Recruitment recommendations and future directions are offered.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Kallos-Lilly, A. Veronica. "A longitudinal study on the impact of maternal depression on child adjustment." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1997. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk3/ftp04/nq26126.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Kobayashi, Juichi 1960. "Parental deviance, parent-child bonding, child abuse, and child sexual aggression." Thesis, The University of Arizona, 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/278178.

Full text
Abstract:
Structural equation modeling was used to test a theoretical model of the etiology of the deviant sexual aggression by adolescents. The subjects were 117 juvenile male sexual offenders who had been referred from either criminal justice or social service agencies to a clinic that treated offenders. The tested theoretical model included several family factors: parental deviance, child physical and sexual abuse history, and children's bonding to their parents. The model as a whole fitted the data very well. As for the specific hypotheses in the model, physical abuse by the father and sexual abuse by males were found to increase sexual aggression by adolescents. Also, children's bonding to their mother was found to decrease their sexual aggression. These results are explained from the social learning perspective and parent-child attachment or social control perspective. Further, the directions for the future research are suggested.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Hendricks, Sarah Elizabeth. "An examination of parent-child interactions and developmental pathways of emotion regulation." Scholarly Commons, 2012. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/123.

Full text
Abstract:
Research examining emotion regulation has indicated that children's success at home and school is enhanced through adaptive emotion regulation skills (Eisenburg, Spinrad & Morris, 2002). This is particularly true in the areas of social competence and academic functioning (Harris, Robinson, Chang & Burns, 2007). Because the development of emotion regulation skills is supported by the scaffolding of adaptive strategies in children may through parental responsivity to needs (Robinson, Morris, Heller, Scheeringa, Boris, & Smyke, 2009), the current study examined pathways through which quality of parent-child interactions impacted later emotion regulation. The effect of attention regulation on emotion regulation was also considered. Participants in the analysis included families from the longitudinal National Institute of Child Health and Development Study of Early Child Care (NICHD-SECC). Variables in the study were measured from infancy through 3 rd grade. Results indicated that the quality of mother child interactions at 54 months was directly associated with both attention regulation at 1 st grade and emotion regulation at 3 rd grade. Results also suggested the presence of an indirect effect of maternal positive caregiving at 54 months on emotion regulation at 3 rd grade through attention regulation at 1 st grade. Father-child interactions were not found to be directly associated with attention regulation at 1 st grade or emotion regulation at 3 rd grade. The results of this study may be beneficial in supporting school psychologists and other clinicians in targeting specific components of parent-child interactions for intervention to support the development of proactive emotion regulation strategies in children.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Horner, Evan. "Marriage mentoring and functional differentiation." Theological Research Exchange Network (TREN), 2002. http://www.tren.com.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Petrakos, Hariclia. "The Parent-Child relationship: Developmental differences in parent-child dyadic interaction during early childhood." Thesis, McGill University, 2000. http://digitool.Library.McGill.CA:80/R/?func=dbin-jump-full&object_id=95591.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to examine differences in parent-child interactions when children were 3 and again at 4 years of age, the time period when gender identity is developing. Thirty-three children (17 boys and 16 girls) with their fathers and mothers were observed during these two time periods across two play contexts: a story-enactment pretend play session and a rough-and-tumble play session. The parent-child dyads were observed for frequency of dyadic parent-child physical and verbal exchange to capture overt parent-child exchanges of closeness (i.e., physical touch and verbal engagement). Results revealed that at age 3, mother-son dyads engaged in more dyadic physical and verbal exchange interaction than father-son dyads. By 4 years of age, father-son dyads engaged in more dyadic physical exchange than mother-son dyads. The reverse was observed for girls. At 3 years of age, father-daughter dyads engaged in more dyadic physical exchange than mother-daughter dyads, but by 4 years of age, mother-daughter dyads engaged in more dyadic physical exchange than father-daughter dyads. The findings are consistent with a psychoanalytic model of gender identity development.
Le but de cette présente étude est d'examiner les changements encourus par les parents lors de leurs interactions avec leurs enfants de 3 et 4 ans, pendant la période de la découverte de leur identité. Trente-trois enfants (17 garçons et 16 filles) ainsi que leurs pères et mères furent observés pendant deux activités: une était une histoire de jeu de comportement ou de fairesemblant , et l'autre, unjeu de tohu-bohu. Les résultats ont révélés qu'à l'âge de 3 ans, les garçons et leurs mères s'impliquent plus dans des échanges physiques et verbaux que les garçons avec leurs pères. Dès l'âge de 4 ans, les garcons et leurs pères s'engagent plus que les garçons avec leurs mères. À l'âge de 3 ans, les filles avec leurs pères s'impliquent plus au niveau physique que les filles avec leurs mères, et vers 4 ans, les filles et leurs mères s'engagent plus que les filles avec leurs pères. Ces conclusions supportent le modèle psychoanalitique du développement de l'identité de sexe de la personne. fr
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Dixon, Wallace E. "Twenty Studies That Revolutionized Child Psychology." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2015. http://amzn.com/0205948030.

Full text
Abstract:
Twenty Studies That Revolutionized Child Psychology gives students a systematic look at the process of child psychology research by examining the twenty most revolutionary scientific investigations in the field over the course of the last fifty years. For the second edition, author and child psychologist Wallace Dixon polled an expanded number of experts in the field to determine the most important studies to be included. The result is an updated collection of revolutionary studies that helps students to better understand the discipline of child psychology.
https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu_books/1024/thumbnail.jpg
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Frederick, Christina A. "Towards a more specific model of anorectic pathogenesis: Parental selfobject use of the pre-anorectic child." Thesis, University of Ottawa (Canada), 1995. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/10021.

Full text
Abstract:
Decades of research on the anorectic's family relationships have contributed a certain measure of depth and precision to our current understanding of this syndrome's pathogenesis. Yet none of the existing etiological formulations is specific as regards the predisposition to anorexia nervosa in that they also apply to other psychopathological conditions. Furthermore, many of these models do not address the precipitation and maintenance phases of the disorder, preferring to remain narrowly focused on premorbid personality factors. As well, existing etiological conjectures do not adequately explain several phenomena regularly encountered among anorectics, such as their relentless quest for perfection and their unusually intense autonomy strivings. In line with this problematic, the current work outlines a modal etiological pattern that is specific to core-group restrictive anorectics and that addresses the precipitation and maintenance phases of the disorder as well as predisposition. It also offers an explanation for their assiduous quest for perfection and their inordinately strong autonomy strivings. It does so by focusing on a hitherto largely overlooked aspect of relationships in the anorectic family, namely the particular ways in which the pre-anorectic child is called upon to compensate for parental emotional deficits. It is conjectured that the predisposition to anorexia nervosa arises in the context of her simultaneous function as a self-denying, merged selfobject to her mother and as an omnipotent, idealized selfobject to either or both parents. Such selfobject use of the pre-anorectic child is argued to leave her with a heightened sensitivity to environmental rejection experiences because of her inordinate dependence on external validation combined with the habit and expectation of being seen as "special". In addition, she finds herself with a uniquely polarized false-self system, particularly strong autonomy strivings, and a highly restricted repertoire of behaviours deemed acceptable, all of which eventuate in a particular proneness to shame and self-rejection. Moreover, this particular conjunction of family relationships and resulting cognitive and emotional distortions is seen to readily distinguish the anorectic from other psychodiagnostic groups with whom she shares certain clinical characteristics. It is the above combination of premorbid personality characteristics that is conjectured to place her at special risk for anorexia in the face of the normal developmental and environmental stresses of adolescence. More precisely, these personality factors place her at such risk, firstly, by compromising her ongoing self-esteem regulation either by sharply restricting the conditions under which she is able to maintain a relative sense of emotional well-being or by introducing an inherent tension or instability into her false-self system. Secondly, the adaptive mechanisms learned within the postulated selfobject relationships help to determine the coping strategies that she chooses in adolescence. That is, the anorectic's self-perfecting in the form of slimming is seen to exemplify an "adult" version of an earlier strategy for the avoidance of shame and self-rejection. As this strategy, however, essentially represents the misapplication of a local solution to a global problem, in contradistinction to the situation of the non-anorectic dieter, it ultimately fails. This, together with the family systemic factors so often cited in the literature and the strongly polarized, countervailing tendencies within her false-self system, are argued to eventuate in a series of intrapersonal and interpersonal vicious escalations that conspire to entrench her ever further in her symptoms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Weiss, Tobias C. "The Association Between Child-Rearing Practices and Child Self-Concept and Depressive Symptoms Reproduced." Xavier University / OhioLINK, 2003. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=xavier1383573193.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Keown, Louise June. "Parent-child relationships, peer functioning, and preschool hyperactivity." Thesis, University of Auckland, 2001. http://wwwlib.umi.com/dissertations/fullcit/3010005.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this thesis was to examine the parent-child relationships and peer functioning of community-identified, 4-year-old boys with hyperactive behaviour problems. The sample consisted of 33 pervasively hyperactive boys and 34 control children. Parenting and child behaviours, and family life factors were assessed at home using a range of measures including the Parental Account of Children's Symptoms Interview (PACS), the Parenting Scale, the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), and the Life Events Questionnaire. In addition, maternal directiveness and synchrony were coded from videotaped parent-child interaction during free play. Children's peer relations were assessed with teacher ratings on the Child Behavior Scale (CBS) and observer ratings of peer interactions at kindergarten. Results showed that parents of hyperactive boys used less effective parenting skills in disciplinary situations and in coping with child behaviour problems, and spent less time in positive parent-child interaction than comparison group parents. Mothers of hyperactive boys also engaged in fewer synchronous play interactions with their sons and gave more negative ratings on indices of life stress. Poor parent coping, father-child communication, maternal synchrony, negative disciplinary practices, and life stress were significantly associated with hyperactivity after adjusting for the effects of conduct problems. The best parenting predictor of hyperactivity was maternal coping. Compared with control children, the hyperactive boys received significantly higher ratings on exclusion by peers, aggressive, noncompliant, and non-social behaviours, as well as significantly lower ratings of prosocial behaviour and peer acceptance. These between-group differences in social functioning remained significant after statistical control for the effects of conduct problems. Further analysis suggested that the associations between hyperactivity and child social behaviours were partly or wholly explained by group differences in exposure to parenting behaviours that are important for children's social development. These findings highlight the need to examine more closely the role of parenting behaviours in shaping the course, prognosis and treatment outcomes in relation to the behavioural and social adjustment of preschool hyperactive children. The implications of these findings for early childhood intervention in hyperactive behaviour problems are discussed.
Subscription resource available via Digital Dissertations only.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Rubinsztein, Denise Vivian. "Developmental adjustment of the pre-school child to the divorce process." Thesis, Stellenbosch : Stellenbosch University, 1986. http://hdl.handle.net/10019.1/49633.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Montemayor, Ludivina. "Exploring the effectiveness of child-centered play therapy in young children| A quantitative single case research design." Thesis, Texas A&M University - Corpus Christi, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3620622.

Full text
Abstract:

Play therapy is a growing form of counseling that allows children to express themselves in the most natural way possible. Virginia Axline (1969) applied Roger's person centered theory and core conditions (empathic, genuineness and honesty, unconditional positive regard) to working with children in a therapeutic setting calling it Child Centered Play Therapy. Landreth (2002) further explored and developed child-centered play therapy concepts and techniques and emphasized the art of the relationship between the child and the counselor. Extant research on the effects of CCPT with children with different diagnoses, age groups, and settings is well-documented. However, there is a dearth of information concerning using CCPT in a school setting with children of ages 3-6 who were identified by the teachers as experiencing behavioral difficulties. Thus, the purpose of the study was to see if CCPT would help change behaviors with students that were identified as having behavior difficulties in the classroom. Three teachers observed and identified 12 students in grades pre-kinder 3, 4, and Kindergarten with behavioral difficulties in the classroom. Each of the 12 students was selected to receive an intervention using CCPT. For each of the 12 students, one parent/guardian completed the Child Behavior Check List forms each week, and the respective classroom teacher completed the Caregiver-Teacher Report form each week for the 12-week duration of the study. A single case research design was used, which included three weeks of baseline observation, followed by six weeks of CCPT with treatment twice per week, and three weeks of post baseline observation. Findings revealed that play therapy was a highly effective treatment for reducing negative behaviors in most students. Parents' ratings of behaviors indicated an 88.30% improvement in behaviors, and 50% of teachers' ratings indicated improved behavior change after receiving CCPT. In summary, 71% of all scores indicated change in behavior. Under further evaluation during the post baseline stage of the study, 90% of parents and 80% of teachers rated the student's behavior as improved. The results are indicative of CCPT being a very efficacious treatment intervention for students in grades pre-kinder 3, 4, and Kindergarten.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Ostrovsky, N., and Wallace E. Jr Dixon. "Child Behavior Questionnaire: Ukrainian Version." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2009. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4935.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Mistretta, Jacqueline M. "The Relationship between Type of Child Care Setting and Externalizing Behaviors in Kindergarten Students." Thesis, Alfred University, 2017. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10687289.

Full text
Abstract:

Past studies examining child care and externalizing behaviors have produced conflicting results. This study examined whether an association exists between type of child care that a child attended the year before kindergarten and externalizing problem behaviors as rated by the child’s kindergarten teacher. Ordinary least-squares regression was used to examine variables that impact ratings of externalizing behavior by evaluating data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 2010–2011 (ECLS-K:2011). The ECLS-K:2011 has up-to-date data that includes a nationally representative sample of children in the United States. Participants were 13,544 children. Additionally, SES and the total number of hours of weekly care were analyzed to see if they moderated the relationship between type of care and externalizing behaviors. Findings indicated that children who attended center care only exhibited more externalizing behaviors than children who attended other types or combinations of care. Children from lower SES families had significantly more externalizing behaviors than children from higher SES families. Findings also indicated that SES had less of an effect on externalizing behavior among children who attended relative and center care than children who attended center care only. The more hours a child spent in care each week, the greater their ratings of externalizing problem behaviors. Additionally, the effects of total hours on externalizing behaviors were lower for children who attended relative care only and relative and center care than those who attended center care only. Study implications for policymakers, parents, and researchers are discussed in depth. For instance, if parents wish to send their children to center care, they may want to incorporate an additional type of weekly care, which may act as a buffer to externalizing problem behaviors. Additionally, policymakers may want to facilitate greater access for child care other than center care only.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr. "How to Land That First Job (And How Not To)." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2019. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4903.

Full text
Abstract:
Member department chairs from the Council of Graduate Departments of Psychology (COGDOP), who also happen to be child development researchers, will advise up and coming SRCD scholars about the daunting process of academic job-seeking. Although panelists’ administrative experiences draw from their roles in academic departments of psychology, their experiences generalize to the academy broadly. In this Q&A panel format, chairs representing institutions of various sizes (see Table 1) will answer questions about the search process and give advice based on several decades of combined experience negotiating research start-up packages and making jobs offers. The panel symposium should be of great interest to graduate students, post-doctoral fellows, and seasoned professionals considering re-entering the academic job market. During Part 1 of the session, panelists will speak 4 minutes each to describe their institutional contexts, their experiences in hiring, and to share short stories about candidates they found especially impressive. Part 1 of the session will conclude with a brief period of broad Q&A. In Part 2 of the session, we will break into more focused Q&A groups based on the special interests of “larger” and “smaller” institutions. At the conclusion of Part 2, groups will report out to one another about particularly relevant topics that arose during small group discussions. At the conclusion of the session, attendees will have a better understanding of the factors department chairs take into consideration when offering jobs and start-up packages to new hires.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Clements, Andrea D., Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and Hannah Abel. "The Utility of a Very Brief Screen to Identify Difficult Children." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2008. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4937.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr, Martha Arterberry, Keith Crnic, H. Hill Goldsmith, Laura Scaramella, and Marsha Weinraub. "How to Land That New Job (And How Not To)." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2016. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4911.

Full text
Abstract:
The International Congress on Infant Studies (ICIS) joins forces with the Council of Graduate Departments of Psychology (COGDOP) to provide young ICIS scholars a unique opportunity to gain advice from acting Department Chairs about how best to write cover letters, select good referees, negotiate for job offers, secure competitive start-up allowances, and manage many other elements of the very anxiety-provoking process of landing a new job. In this Q&A panel format, multiple Department Chairs representing institutions ranging in size from very small to very large, and from private to public, will answer questions and give advice based on their several decades of combined experience negotiating and making jobs offers from the other side of table. This panel symposium should be of great interest to graduate students in all years of study, post-doctoral fellows, and even more seasoned professionals who are thinking of re-entering the academic job market.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Tucciarone, Joseph T., Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and Alissa Fleahman. "Recalling “Make-A-Gong”: What’s so Special About Target Action #4." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2012. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4927.

Full text
Abstract:
Enabling-relation action sequences (ERASs) have long provided researchers an index of infants’ short- and long-term memories. Reproduction of these sequences demonstrates that infants are capable of attending to, encoding, retaining, and retrieving modeled actions in the sequence. Bauer and colleagues (e.g., 2000) have established considerable literature on infants’ memorial capacities using this paradigm. There is little research, however, on the extent to which infants produce primacy- versus recency-type effects in reproducing specific target actions in the sequence.Researchers have also not considered whethe exogenous and endogenous factors contribute to infants’ reproduction of target actions, or their focus on early versus late actions in the sequence. Our investigation explored whether an exogenous distracter, alone or combined with endogenous (temperament) factors, accounts for infants’ reproduction of individual steps in an ERAS. Twenty-seven 21-month olds (15 girls) observed an experimenter in our lab model a version of the “make-a-gong” action sequence, comprising five steps: 1) extend the rod, 2) lay the rod across two hooks, 3) hang the gong on the rod, 4) assemble the drumstick, and 5) strike the gong with the drumstick. Half observed the model while distracted by a peripheral “Mister Monkey” toy. Each was administered the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire, assessing three temperament dimensions: Negative Affectivity, Surgency, and Effortful Control. When distracted, infants were significantly more likely to first attempt Target Action #4 (TA4) than any other target action [c2(1) = 4.14, p = .042], suggesting that when attentional resources are compromised, as when attending to an exogenous distracter, infants are likely to attempt recently observed steps in a modeled sequence. Success at TA4 did not differ between distracted and undistracted infants; however, we found that temperament was a significant predictor of success on TA4, but not on any other target action. Specifically, Negative Affectivity (r = -.48, p = .011) and Surgency (r = .40, p = .039) were associated with successful reproduction of TA4; however, distraction condition moderated neither effect. These results show that exogenous and endogenous factors can impact infants’ reproduction of ERASs, and perhaps infants’ memory-based performance more generally. Though this conclusion awaits replications in other settings, that the presence of an exogenous distraction produced a recency effect implies that infants may allocate their attention differently according to how distracting their surroundings are. Also, that surgency and negative affectivity correlated with infants’ success on one of the steps suggests that recall may be subject to the influence of infants’ temperamental reactivity. Future research should attempt to discern whether effects linked to TA4 are unique to that specific target action, or are instead a reflection of a recency effect.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Tucciarone, Joseph T., Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and Alissa N. Fleahman. "Recalling “Make-A-Gong”: What’s so Special About Target Action #4." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2012. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4924.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Siller, Christina. "A father's supportive presence: Understanding how fathers influence children's developmental outcomes." Scholarly Commons, 2012. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/125.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study focuses on how a father's supportive presence during interactions with his child influences his/her social outcomes in adolescence. Ethological theories of attachment provide a theoretical basis for the investigation of father-child interactions because they provide us with an explanation regarding how and why child-caregiver relationships function to influence a child's development and later social functioning. Structural equation modeling was used to construct a theoretical model by which fathering behaviors influence later psychosocial outcomes, particularly impulse control and risky behaviors during adolescence. For boys, supportive mothering behaviors had a greater influence on impulse control than supportive fathering behaviors. The opposite was true for girls. For girls, supportive fathering behaviors had a greater influence on impulse control than supportive mothering behaviors. Impulse control served a partial mediating effect between supportive parenting behaviors and risk-taking behaviors. For sons, supportive mothering behaviors had a significant positive impact on impulse control during adolescence. Conversely, for daughters, supportive fathering behaviors—but not supportive mothering behaviors—had a significant positive impact on impulse control during adolescence. In fact, supportive mothering behaviors had an insignificant effect on daughters' impulse control during adolescence,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Miller, Rachel. "Adolescent Political Development." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2018. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/1156.

Full text
Abstract:
The present research is on parent influence on adolescent political development. The study surveys parent political behavior, parent warmth and quality of relationship with their child, and adolescent knowledge of parent political behavior to understand how these factors affect a match in party affiliation in parent and adolescent. 547 family groups are included in the study. I hypothesized that an adolescent’s politics would be more likely to match that of their parent when the parent is politically involved and warm and the adolescent is aware of the parent’s political behavior. This study is important because individuals’ party affiliation determines their voting behavior, which determines many decisions made in this country. It is interesting to understand how people develop their party affiliation and what role parents have in this development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Brown, Margaux Hanes. "An Examination of Executive Function, Stress, and Adolescent Attachment to Caregivers in a Social Neuroscience Model Using the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD)." Thesis, The George Washington University, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3610769.

Full text
Abstract:

The goal of this study was to explore the relationship between stress and executive function (EF) in adolescence and to determine the extent to which the adolescent-caregiver attachment moderated the effects of stress on EF. EF are a set of meta-cognitive processes, including planning, that require coordinated neural activation in the prefrontal cortex and a number of other brain regions. Deficits in EF are associated with many mental health disorders. Large-scale, federally funded efforts are ongoing to understand more about EF and the brain.

Current adolescent brain research calls for further investigation of how regions coordinate in task-specific activities (Spear, 2010). The stress, or hypothalamic-pituitary axis (HPA), and attachment systems share underlying neural substrates that overlap with regions activated to perform EF. Adolescence is a sensitive period for changes in EF skills (Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006), the HPA axis (Romeo, 2011), and attachment (Allen, 2008). Therefore, this research was aimed at exploring how stress and attachment predict performance on an EF task in adolescence. Specifically, the researcher examined the extent to which attachment styles moderated the relationship between cortisol, a measure of HPA axis functioning, and performance on the Tower of London (TOL), a test of planning skills. The NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD) data were used to examine this overarching research question in a social neuroscience model.

While the TOL is one of the most frequently used measures of EF, its scoring methods vary across studies (Etnier & Change, 2009). Study 1 consisted of an exploratory factor analysis with data from 932 youth, and results supported a single factor model that best represented planning skills, which was consistent with the hypothesized structure based on an exploratory study with a small sample of college students (Berg, Byrd, McNamara, & Case, 2010). The factor score was then used as the criterion variable in Study 2, which included three moderated regression models that explored secure, preoccupied, and dismissing attachment styles. Though results suggested that cortisol and attachment were not predictive of planning in this sample, potential explanations for the lack of findings are proposed and recommendations for future research are included.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Zyga, Olena. "From Behavior to Biology: Examining Oxytocin, Social Cognitive Ability, and Parent-Child Interactions in Preschoolers with Autism Spectrum Disorder." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1554976738357849.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Clements, Andrea D., A. L. Acuff, Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and C. Snyder. "Maternal and Child Temperament and Parenting Style." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2008. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4936.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Chan, Mee-yin Becky. "Children's conceptualizations of health and illness: a developmental perspective." Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 1985. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B29648117.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

McGovern, T. V., L. Corey, Wallace E. Jr Dixon, J. D. Holmes, J. E. Kuebli, K. A. Ritchey, R. A. Smith, and S. Walker. "Psychologically Literate Citizens." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2009. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4887.

Full text
Abstract:
Book Summary: This title examines what our students need to know to be psychologically literate citizens of the contemporary world, caring family members, and productive workers who can meet today's challenges. It contains the expert opinions of a leading group on the topic, creates a powerful new model for educating psychologically literate citizens and provides a handbook of evidence-based practical pedagogy with substantive resource materials applicable to every campus and its faculty.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Portner, Laura Collier. "Observed Parenting Aspects of Child Compliance in Custodial Grandfamilies." Thesis, University of North Texas, 2016. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc862834/.

Full text
Abstract:
Custodial grandmothers and grandchild (aged 4 to 12) dyads (N = 170) completed self-report, other-report, and an observational task that captured child HI, expressive social support, and custodial grandmother-grandchild compliance variables. A multivariate analysis of covariance tested differences between high and low hyperactivity-inattention on observed parenting variables while controlling for child age. While overall results were not significant, there were significant differences between child age and observed parenting variables. A hierarchical regression model revealed that, when controlling for age, child hyperactivity-inattention does not moderate the relationship between commands given by a custodial grandmother and child compliance, but revealed that direct commands from the grandmother predicted compliance. A second hierarchical regression model suggested that encouragement and praise (versus criticism and discouragement) from a grandmother moderated the relationship between grandmother commands and child compliance, when controlling for child age. It appeared that when grandmothers gave indirect commands more frequently, encouragement and praise instead of criticism was associated with greater compliance. In dyads with frequent direct commands given, compliance was high, however dyads who scored high in direct commands with criticism and discouragement were most likely to comply. This study adds to the literature by providing insight into the challenges and strengths for this unique, growing population.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Weber, Jacqlyne D., Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and Jaima S. Price. "Executive Function Predictors of Preschoolers’ Talk." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2016. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4914.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Musacchio, Katherine, Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and William T. III Dalton. "Executive Function as a Moderator of Obesity in Infancy." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2012. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4926.

Full text
Abstract:
Childhood overweight and obesity have experienced epidemic growth in recent years. Excessive adiposity presents challenges to orthopedic, neurological, pulmonary, gastroenterological, endocrinal, and social development. Thus, the time course of weight development in early childhood is of special public health concern. A major goal of childhood obesity research is to identify mechanisms contributing to excess weight gain. In infancy, executive function has been identified as one possible contributor. Unfortunately, no studies have yet examined infant obesity risk with respect to executive function development. In this study, we present the first evidence that executive function may be linked to obesity risk in infancy. Thirty middle-class, primarily White infants visited the lab of the ETSU Program for the Study of Infancy at 18 and 21 months of age. Measures of infant weight and recumbent length were taken at both ages. Weightfor-length BMI z-scores were derived from international growth curves published by the World Health Organization. Executive function was measured at the 21-month visit, using the Multilocation Search Task. In this task, infants are shown the location of a cracker in one of five drawers. Once infants select the correct drawer on three successive trials, the cracker is moved to a new drawer in plain view of the child, and the child is asked to find the cracker again. The location of the cracker is changed on two more trials. Children’s perseverating responses to the old cracker location is viewed as an inverse measure of executive function. Results showed that from 18 to 21 months of age, BMI z-scores decreased significantly [t(30) = 2.63, p = .013]. This finding suggests that on average, infant BMI scores decreased across the three-month period. To explore whether executive function performance varied as a function of infant BMI, we divided our sample into two BMI groups via median split. A mixed-design ANOVA revealed that infants with the greatest decreases in BMI from 18 to 21 months (i.e., the “Hi Decrease” group), showed the greatest gains in performance across the three trials of the executive function task [F(2, 25) = 5.29, p = .012]. Specifically, by Trial 3 of the multilocation search task, Hi Decreasers were making an average of 0.57 perseverative errors, whereas the Lo Decreasers were making an average of 2.231 perseverative errors. These results are consistent with expectations. To the extent that executive function capacity helps regulate weight gain, it stands to reason that infants with greater executive function capacity would be advantaged in regulating their eating behaviors. Although we recognize that there are likely multiple contributors to infant and child obesity, findings from the present study supports the possibility that one of these contributors may be executive function. To our knowledge, this is the first study documenting a link between executive function and infant BMI.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Price, Jaima S., Lauren P. Driggers-Jones, Wallace E. Jr Dixon, and Natasha B. Gouge. "Temperament-Vocabulary Links During the Transition to First Word Production: Contrary to Expectations." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2017. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4910.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr, and Jaima S. Price. "Temperament Vocabulary Links in the Third Year." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2015. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4915.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Roque, Lisa. "Emotion regulation in child-mother dyads: A psychobiological approach." Doctoral thesis, ISPA - Instituto Universitário, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/10400.12/1112.

Full text
Abstract:
Tese de Doutoramento em Psicologia Aplicada (Psicologia do Desenvolvimento) apresentada ao Instituto Superior Psicologia Aplicada
Este trabalho tem como objectivo o estudo da regulação emocional em díades mãe-criança, sob uma perspectiva psicobiológica, ou seja, a análise das relações entre processos internos (temperamento e actividade adrenocortical) e externos (representações de vinculação maternas e comportamentos de base segura das crianças), durante diferentes contextos situacionais (medo, afecto positivo, frustração/raiva) e sociais (constrangimento e envolvimento maternos). Cinquenta e cinco crianças entre os 18 e os 26 meses da idade e respectivas mães participaram neste estudo. As estratégias comportamentais de regulação emocional, a expressividade e intensidade emocionais das crianças foram estudadas através do Paradigma de Regulação Emocional (Diener, & Mangelsdorf, 1999 a, b). Os comportamentos de base segura das crianças e as representações de vinculação das mães foram avaliadas através do “Attachment Behavior Q-Set” (Waters, 1995) e pelas Narrativas de Representação da Vinculação em Adultos (Waters, & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2004), respectivamente. O temperamento das crianças foi avaliado através do “Bate’s Infant Characteristics Questionnaire” (Bates, Freeland, & Lounsbury, 1979; adaptação portuguesa por Soares, Rangel-Henriques, & Dias, 2009). Finalmente, as respostas adrenocorticais das crianças e das mães foram avaliadas através de amostras de saliva e analisadas através de ensaios de luminoimunoiscência (LIA). Os resultados revelaram que, de um modo geral, as estratégias comportamentais das crianças variaram, significativamente, em função do contexto situacional (as crianças exibiram mais estratégias durante os episódios de afecto positivo e frustração/raiva, em comparação com os de medo) e envolvimento materno. A expressividade emocional das crianças variou em função do contexto situacional (as crianças exibiram maior expressividade emocional, positiva ou negativa, durante os episódios de medo e frustração/raiva e menos durante os de afecto positivo) e de interacções entre a expressividade emocional e o envolvimento materno. A intensidade emocional revelou variações em função de uma interacção entre o contexto e o envolvimento materno. As estratégias comportamentais e a expressividade emocional das crianças também se diferenciaram significativamente em função da qualidade da relação de vinculação às mães. As representações maternas sobre a vinculação além de serem predictoras dos comportamentos de base segura das crianças crianças, também influenciaram significativamente a expressividade e a intensidade emocionais destas. As respostas adrenocorticais das crianças e das mães variaram significativamente, em função da qualidade de vinculação das crianças. As representações maternas sobre a vinculação influenciaram significativamente os níveis de cortisol das mães, assim como os das crianças (de um modo marginal). A qualidade do temperamento das crianças revelou associações significativas com as estratégias comportamentais e com as respostas adrenocorticais das crianças e das mães. Os resultados são discutidos, analisando possíveis implicações, limitações e futuras linhas de investigação. ---------- ABSTRACT ---------- This work studies emotion regulation in child-mother dyads from a psychobiological perspective, particularly, the study of the relationships between internal (temperament and adrenocortical activity) and external processes (mothers’ attachment representations and children’s secure base behaviours), during different situational (fear, positive affect, frustration/anger) and social (mother constrained and involved) contexts. Fifty-five children between 18 and 26 months of age and their mothers participated in this study. Children’s emotion regulation behavioural strategies, emotional expressiveness and intensity were studied through the Emotion Regulation Paradigm (Diener, & Mangelsdorf, 1999 a, b). To assess children’s secure base behaviours and mothers’ attachment representations the Attachment Behaviour Q-Set (Waters, 1995) and the Adult Attachment Representation Narratives (Waters, & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2004) were used, respectively. Children’s temperament was evaluated by the The Bate’s Infant Characteristics Questionnaire (ICQ), (Bates, Freeland, & Lounsbury, 1979; portuguese adaptation by Soares, Rangel-Henriques, & Dias, 2009). Finally, children’s and mothers’ adrenocortical activity were assessed from salivary cortisol and analyzed through luminoimmunoassay (LIA) kits. Results revealed that overall, toddlers’ regulatory strategies varied as function of emotion-eliciting context (children exhibited more strategies during positive affect and frustration/anger episodes and less during fear episodes) and maternal involvement. Toddlers’ emotional expressiveness varied as function of emotion-eliciting context (children exhibited more emotional expressions either negative or positive, during fear and frustration/anger episodes and less during positive affect episodes) and as result of interactions between emotional expressiveness and maternal involvement. Emotional intensity varied as function of an interaction between context and maternal involvement. Children’s behavioural strategies and expressiveness also differed significantly as function of attachment security to their mothers. Mothers’ attachment representations not only predicted their children’s secure base behaviours, but also influenced their expressiveness and emotional intensity, in a significant way. Children and mothers’ adrenocortical responses were significantly influenced by children’s attachment security. Mothers’ personal attachment representations influenced significantly their own cortisol responses, as well as their children’s (in a marginal significant way). Children’s temperament quality showed significant associations with toddlers’ behavioural strategies and children and mothers’ adrenocortical activities. Possible implications, limitations and future research lines and discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Metindogan, Wise Aysegul. "Parenting, child mastery motivation, and children's school readiness to learn in Turkey a structural equation analysis /." Related electronic resource, 2007. http://proquest.umi.com/pqdweb?did=1407688751&sid=1&Fmt=2&clientId=3739&RQT=309&VName=PQD.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Altenburger, Lauren E. "Contributions of Observed Coparenting and Infant Temperament to Child Social-Emotional Adjustment." The Ohio State University, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1406176919.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Jamal, Kinza. "What Would Your Parents Say?!: A Cross-Cultural and Personality Study." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2016. http://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/754.

Full text
Abstract:
Culture is what influences us and shapes us into who we are and what we become later on in life, this study runs with this concept. This study includes three groups of participants: 60 Subcontinental adolescents, 60 (Subcontinental) Asian-American adolescents,60 European-American adolescents .The ages of these adolescents range from 17-23 years of age. The participants are asked to fill out measures pertaining to anxiety/depression, self identity , acculturation and a questionnaire at the end that asks about future marital plans. The studies hypotheses are that there is a stronger correlation between parenting technique and the degree to
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Warm, Anna. "The role of video game violence in hostile affect, cognitions and attributional style among adolescent players." Thesis, University of Central Lancashire, 1999. http://clok.uclan.ac.uk/19055/.

Full text
Abstract:
A central aim of the present research was to investigate the short-term influences of video game play on aggression-related psychological states (including affect, cognitions, attributions and behavioural tendencies). More specifically, efforts were directed at establishing psychological effects of video games, which are causally related to game violence. A series of experiments examined short-term changes in adolescent players following various types of video game play. Experiment 1 identified a number of important game dimensions (i.e., characteristics of video game play) and explored their relationship to overall game enjoyment using path analysis. Of particular interest was the finding that violence did not strongly influence game enjoyment. In Experiment 2, increases in affective hostility and anger were reported after both types of video game play. Significantly greater increases after violent video game play provided support for a video game violence-hostile affect relation. However, the findings of subsequent experiments produced contrasting evidence showing that affective changes following video game play do not predictably vary as a function of game violence, but appear to be linearly related to video game pace. Game violence was more strongly implicated in cognitive effects of video game play. Evidence that game violence affects cognitions emerged on a variety of measures. These cognitive effects were seen as being reflective of aggression priming and short-term disinhibition processes. Finally, the extent to which short-term effects of violent video game play dispose players towards aggression was investigated using attribution and response tendency measures. Whilst the majority of the analyses failed to produce effects of game violence on attributions and response tendencies, an interesting interaction emerged involving game violence effects in females. The nature of the inteaction was viewed as being best explained by modelling processes, though disinhibition explanations were also viewed as being compatible. The findings were interpreted within existing social-psychological theories of media-elicited aggression. A number of video game effects could be accounted for using Berkowitz's cognitive neo-associationist framework, whilst other findings implicated the usefulness of Zillmann's excitation-transfer theory for understanding video game effects. Ultimately, the results were conceptualised using Anderson's General Affective Aggression model. Overall, the research was fairly successful in highlighting a number of short-term affective and cognitive states that can result from video game play. However, these effects were generally not manifested in behavioural tendencies towards others. The few findings that did implicate increases in aggressive behavioural tendencies were difficult to place within Anderson's framework, as they did not parallel changes at earlier stages of the model (i.e. affective and cognitive changes). Modelling and/or possibly disinhibition effects were viewed as the most appropriate theoretical concepts for explaining the findings relating to behavioural tendencies. The implications of the findings in relation to previous research on video game and media effects and limitations to the generalisability of the findings are discussed. Finally, several recommendations for future research are outlined.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Davis, Heather A. "DEVELOPMENTAL TRAJECTORIES OF EXCESSIVE EXERCISE AND FASTING ACROSS THE MIDDLE SCHOOL YEARS." UKnowledge, 2015. http://uknowledge.uky.edu/psychology_etds/80.

Full text
Abstract:
Repeated excessive exercise (EE) fasting behavior, in the absence of binge eating and purging, are important eating disorder behaviors that are not captured by the current diagnostic system. Though they appear to be harmful and distressing for adults, little is known about these behaviors in youth. To begin to understand their development, I studied the course of the behaviors across the three years of middle school (n = 1,195). Both behaviors were present in middle school girls and boys, and youth progressed along different developmental trajectories of engagement in the behaviors. Youth involved in either behavior experienced elevated levels of depression and some forms of high-risk eating and thinness expectancies. Their distress levels did not differ from those of youth engaging in purging behavior or low levels of binge eating. EE and fasting behavior can be identified in the early stages of adolescence, youth differ in their developmental experience of these behaviors, and they are associated with significant distress very early in development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Bhatt, Pooja. "Differentiation of self and marital adjustment within the Asian Indian American population." Online version, 2001. http://www.uwstout.edu/lib/thesis/2001/2001bhattp.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr, William T. III Dalton, Jaima S. Price, Katelyn Todaro, and Matthew T. McBee. "Style of Parenting Contributes (At Least Statistically) to Infant Weight Status." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2014. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4916.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Dixon, Wallace E. Jr, K. Lingerfelt, R. Russell, and Andrea D. Clements. "Temperament Moderates the Learning of Pretend Play Sequences at 15 Months." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2009. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/4933.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography