Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Discourse connectives'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 34 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Discourse connectives.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Warner, Richard G. "Discourse connectives in English." New York : Garland Pub, 1985. http://books.google.com/books?id=LtFZAAAAMAAJ.
Full textDallie, Muhammed. "Discourse connectives in Syrian Arabic." Thesis, Bangor University, 1992. https://research.bangor.ac.uk/portal/en/theses/discourse-connectives-in-syrian-arabic(9c4f3197-acde-4c81-bac9-9e1876038c12).html.
Full textKammensjö, Helene. "Discourse connectives in Arabic lecturing monologue /." Göteborg : Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis, 2005. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=014821132&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.
Full textKammensjö, Helène. "Discourse connectives in Arabic lecturing monologue /." Göteborg : Göteborg university, 2005. http://catalogue.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/cb402440269.
Full textDemirsahin, Isin. "Connective Position, Argument Order And Information Structure Of Discourse Connectives In Written Turkish Texts." Master's thesis, METU, 2008. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12610160/index.pdf.
Full textHasan, 1976) and coherent (Mann &
Thompson, 1987, 1988). Mainly ignored in the field of linguistics until recently, the text and the discourse structure have been inquired from various points of view (Asher, 1993
Asher &
Lascarides, 1998
Grosz &
Sidner, 1986
Mann &
Thompson, 1987, 1988
Webber, 2004). D-LTAG is a discourse grammar work that extends a lexicalized sentence level grammar LTAG (Joshi, 1987) to low-level discourse (Webber, 2004
Webber &
Joshi, 1998). In this framework, discourse connectives such as coordinating conjunctions, subordinating conjunctions, parallel connectives and discourse adverbials are predicates of discourse structure that take text spans that can be interpreted as abstract objects (Asher, 1993). Turkish has a flexible word order in comparison to languages like English. In English, the discourse adverbials are noted for their ability to occupy positions unavailable to other discourse connectives. In Turkish, word order of other discourse connectives, coordinators and subordinators are not expected to be as restricted. This thesis examines the connective position, argument order and the information structure of five Turkish discourse connectives in their eleven uses. The analyses show that the examined features of discourse connectives are related to the syntactic group the connective belongs to. Discourse connectives of the same syntactic groups exploit similar connective position and argument order possibilities, and they tend to be included in similar information units.
Zeydan, Sultan. "The Analysis Of Contrastive Discourse Connectives In Turkish." Master's thesis, METU, 2008. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12610231/index.pdf.
Full textHutchinson, Ben. "The automatic acquisition of knowledge about discourse connectives." Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2005. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/852.
Full textHasebe, Yoichiro. "An Integrated Approach to Discourse Connectives as Grammatical Constructions." Kyoto University, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/2433/261627.
Full textLewis, Diana M. "Some emergent discourse connectives in English : grammaticalization via rhetorical patterns." Thesis, University of Oxford, 2000. http://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:137436a0-a0e7-4764-a667-6312d899f909.
Full textSimpkins, Benjamin G. "Connectives and causal relatedness in expository text." Click here to access thesis, 2005. http://www.georgiasouthern.edu/etd/archive/Fall2005/bsimpkin/simpkins_benjamin_g_200505_ms.pdf.
Full text"A thesis submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Georgia Southern University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Science" ETD. Includes bibliographical references (p. 54-57)
Sasamoto, Ryoko. "The limits of classification: a relevance theoretic re-assessment of Japanese causal discourse connectives." Thesis, University of Salford, 2006. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.492408.
Full textLu, Yuan. "The acquisition of Chinese connectives by second language learners." Diss., University of Iowa, 2017. https://ir.uiowa.edu/etd/5560.
Full textAndersson, Marta. "The Architecture of Result Relations : Corpus and experimental approaches to Result coherence relations in English." Doctoral thesis, Stockholms universitet, Engelska institutionen, 2016. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-126935.
Full textLounis, Hassane. "Discourse connectives in translation : a relevance-theoretic account with special reference to translation from and into Arabic." Thesis, University of Salford, 2010. http://usir.salford.ac.uk/26782/.
Full textOzhan, Didem. "A Comparative Analysis On The Use Of But, However And Although In The University Students." Phd thesis, METU, 2012. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12614235/index.pdf.
Full textModicom, Pierre-Yves. "L'énoncé et son double : recherches sur le marquage de l'altérité énonciative en allemand." Thesis, Paris 4, 2016. http://www.theses.fr/2016PA040113.
Full textThis study deals with three groups of discourse markers in German: (i) disjuncts and attitudinal adverbials (henceforth "perspectival adverbials"), (ii) focus particles ("paradigm-scanning adverbials") and (iii) modal particles. The starting point of the analysis is the heuristic notion of "Otherness at work", i.e. the constitutive role of coexisting heterogeneous viewpoints: the three groups of discourse markers, each of which has the status of a functional class with recurring distributional properties, correspond to three modes of Otherness. First, an utterance can be seen as expressing a biased and selective perspective on "What there is". It thus coexists with other, possible yet dispreferred perspectives. Modal adverbials, argumentative disjuncts and metalinguistic markers are various subtypes of these perspectival adverbials. Further, the utterance (or a constituent thereof) can be located into a set of conflicting alternatives thanks to various focussing strategies. It is necessary to distinguish between focus-sensitive adverbials as a whole, the functional class of paradigm-scanning adverbials, and the lexical set of contrastive adverbs that can access the aforementioned function. The traditional class of "focus particle" is thus deconstructed along three different, non-parallel levels. Finally, constitutive otherness can stand for conflicting or mixed views about the validity of the propositional content. This kind of Otherness, which can (but needs not) correspond to modal intersubjectivisation, is the semantic domain of modal particles, which are analyzed here in their interaction with sentence mood
Parra-Araujo, Beatriz Goaveia Garcia. "A trajetória de gramaticalização dos juntores concessivos "aunque", "a pesar de (que)" e "por mucho (que)" no espanhol peninsular /." São José do Rio Preto, 2020. http://hdl.handle.net/11449/192449.
Full textResumo: Este trabalho objetiva apresentar uma trajetória de gramaticalização para os juntores concessivos "aunque", "a pesar de (que)" e "por mucho (que)", adotando como aparato teórico os estudos em gramaticalização (TRAUGOTT, 1997; BYBEE, 2003, 2006; HOPPER; TRAUGOTT, 2003, entre outros) e o modelo da Gramática Discursivo-Funcional (GDF), de Hengeveld e Mackenzie (2008). Na análise sincrônica realizada por Parra (2016) sob a perspectiva da GDF, a autora observa que o juntor "aunque" pode marcar uma relação concessiva em três camadas distintas no espanhol peninsular atual: na do Conteúdo Proposicional, pertencente ao Nível Representacional, e nas camadas do Ato Discursivo e do Movimento, localizadas no Nível Interpessoal. A partir desse resultado, levantamos a hipótese de que, quando analisados diacronicamente, os usos de "aunque" podem revelar uma trajetória de gramaticalização que parte de um uso semântico para usos pragmáticos, e de que tal trajetória pode corresponder a um processo comum sofrido pelos juntores concessivos dessa língua. Assim, selecionamos para este estudo, além do juntor "aunque", os juntores "a pesar de (que)" e "por mucho (que)", por serem, dentre os juntores apontados por Flamenco García (1999), os que se mostraram mais frequentes nos dados e por advirem de origens distintas, tendo em vista as fontes para o desenvolvimento de juntores concessivos apontadas por König (1994). O universo de investigação desta pesquisa reúne ocorrências dos três juntores na fase ... (Resumo completo, clicar acesso eletrônico abaixo)
Abstract: This paper aims to present a path of grammaticalization for the concessive connectives "aunque", "a pesar de (que)" and "por mucho (que)" adopting as a theoretical apparatus the studies on grammaticalization (TRAUGOTT, 1997; BYBEE, 2003, 2006; HOPPER; TRAUGOTT, 2003, among others) and the model of Functional Discourse Grammar (FDG), by Hengeveld and Mackenzie (2008). In the synchronic analysis performed by Parra (2016) from the perspective of the FDG, the author notes that the connective "aunque" can mark a concessive relationship in three distinct layers in the current Peninsular Spanish: that of Propositional Content, belonging to the Representational Level, and the layers of the Move and Discourse Act, located at the Interpersonal Level. From this result, we hypothesize that, when analyzed diachronically, the uses of "aunque" may reveal a grammaticalization trajectory that starts from a semantic use for pragmatic uses, and that such trajectory may correspond to a common process suffered by the concessive connectives of that language. Thus, we selected for this study, in addition to the connective "aunque", the connectives "a pesar de (que)" and "por mucho (que)", since they are among the connectives pointed out by Flamenco García (1999), those who had the highest number of occurrences in a previous research, and because they come from different origins, considering the sources for the development of concessive joints pointed out by König (1994). The universe of this resear... (Complete abstract click electronic access below)
Doutor
Aktas, Berfin. "Computational Aspects Of Discourse Annotation." Master's thesis, METU, 2008. http://etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12610402/index.pdf.
Full textin fact the totality of discourse is more than the sum total of the sentences that constitute it. The property that differentiates discourse from a set of arbitrary sentences is defined as coherence. Coherence is established by the relations between the parts of discourse. We have a lexicalized approach to discourse, therefore in this study, discourse relations are considered to be set up by lexical items called discourse connectives. Systematic analysis of coherence requires an annotated corpus in which coherence relations are encoded. We developed an annotation environment to be used in an ongoing discourse level annotation project which aims to generate a theory-neutral source of coherence relations. We followed a data-driven methodology in design of the data structure employed in the annotation software. For this reason, we examined the predicate-argument structure of connectives. This analysis shows that stand-off annotation technique is more suitable than an inline method for such an annotation environment. This thesis also include a brief discussion on the formal implications of coherence relation constructions.
Brehm-Jurish, Eva Ute. "Connective ties in discourse : three ERP-studies on causal, temporal and concessive connective ties and their influence on language processing." Phd thesis, Universität Potsdam, 2005. http://opus.kobv.de/ubp/volltexte/2006/678/.
Full textQuestions
In four experiments the influence of lexical connectives such as " darum", therefore, " danach", afterwards, and " trotzdem", nevertheless, on the processing of short two-sentence discourses was examined and compared to the processing of deictical sentential adverbs such as " gestern", yesterday, and " lieber", rather. These latter words do not have the property of signaling a certain discourse relation between two sentences, as connective ties do. Three questions were central to the work:
* Do the processing contrasts found between connective and non-connective elements extend to connective ties and deictical sentential adverbs (experiments 2 and 3)?
* Does the semantic content of the connective ties play the primary role, i.e is the major distinction to be made indeed between connective and non-connective or instead between causal, temporal and concessive?
* When precisely is the information provided by connective ties used? There is some evidence that connective ties can have an immediate influence on the integration of subsequent elements, but the end of the second sentences appears to play an important role as well: experiments 2, 3, and 4.
Conclusions
First of all, the theoretical distinction between connective and non-connective elements does indeed have " cognitive reality" . This has already been shown in previous studies. The present studies do however show, that there is also a difference between one-place discourse elements (deictical sentential adverbs) and two-place discourse elements, namely connective ties, since all experiments examining this contrast found evidence for qualitatively and quantitatively different processing (experiments 1, 2, and 3).
Secondly, the semantic type of the connective ties also plays a role. This was not shown for the LAN, found for all connective ties when compared to non-connective elements, and consequently interpreted as a more abstract reflection of the integration of connective ties. There was also no difference between causal and temporal connective ties before the end of the discourses in experiment 3.
However, the N400 found for incoherent discourses in experiment 2, larger for connective incoherent than non-connective incoherent discourses, as well as the P3b found for concessive connective ties in the comparison between causal and concessive connective ties gave reason to assume that the semantic content of connective ties is made use of in incremental processing, and that the relation signaled by the connective tie is the one that readers attempt to construct.
Concerning when the information provided by connective ties is used, it appears as if connectivity is generally and obligatorily taken at face value. As long as the meaning of a connective tie did not conflict with a preferred canonical discourse relation, there were no differences found for varying connective discourses (experiment 3). However, the fact that concessive connective ties announce the need for a more complex text representation was recognized and made use of immediately (experiment 4). Additionally, a violation of the discourse relation resulted in more difficult semantic integration if a connective tie was present (experiment 2). It is therefore concluded here that connective ties influence processing immediately.
This claim has to be modified somewhat, since the sentence-final elements suggested that connective ties trigger different integration processes than non-connective elements. It seems as if the answer to the question of when connective ties are processed is neither exclusively immediately nor exclusively afterwards, but that both viewpoints are correct. It is suggested here that before the end of a discourse economy plays a central role in that a canonical relation is assumed unless there is evidence to the contrary. A connective tie could have the function of reducing the dimensions evaluated in a discourse to the one signaled by the connective tie. At the end of the discourse the representation is evaluated and verified, and an integrated situation model constructed. Here, the complexity of the different discourse relations that connective ties can signal, is expressed.
Konnektiva im Diskurs: Drei EKP-Studien zu kausalen, temporalen und konzessiven Konnektoren und ihrem Einfluss auf die Sprachverarbeitung.
Einführung und Fragestellung
In vier Experimenten wurde der Einfluss von lexikalischen Konnektiva wie " darum", " danach" und " trotzdem" auf die Verarbeitung von kurzen zweisätzigen Diskursen untersucht und mit der Verarbeitung von deiktischen Satzaderbien wie " gestern" und " lieber" verglichen, die nicht wie die Konnektiva die Eigenschaft haben, die Diskursrelation zwischen zwei Sätzen explizit auszudrücken.
Drei Fragen standen im Mittelpunkt der Arbeit:
* Findet sich der Kontrast zwischen konnektiven und nicht-konnektiven Elementen auch zwischen Konnektiva und deiktischen Satzadverbien wieder (Experimente 2 und 3)?
* Spielt der semantische Inhalt der Konnektiva die primäre Rolle: ist die Hauptunterscheidung zwischen konnektiven und nicht-konnektiven Elementen zu machen, oder zwischen kausalen (darum, deshalb), temporalen (danach, hinterher) und konzessiven (trotzdem, dennoch) Elementen (Experimente 3 / 4)?
* Wann genau wird die Information, die Konnektiva bieten, genutzt? Es gibt Evidenz dafür, dass Konnektiva einen sofortigen Einfluss haben, aber auch dafür, dass das Ende der Texte eine gewichtige Rolle spielt: Experimente 2, 3, und 4.
Konklusionen
Zunächst einmal hat die theoretische Unterscheidung zwischen konnektiven und nicht-konnektiven Worten tatsächlich " kognitive Realität" . Dies wurde bereits in früheren Studien gezeigt. Die aktuellen Experimente zeigten jedoch, dass es auch einen Unterschied zwischen einstelligen (deiktische Satzadverbien) und zweistelligen (Konnektiva) Diskursrelationen gibt, da alle Experimente, die diesen Kontrast untersuchten, qualitativ und quantitativ andere Verarbeitung für Konnektiva zeigten (Experimente 1, 2 und 3).
Zweitens spielt der semantische Typus der Konnektiva ebenfalls eine Rolle. Dieser Einfluss zeigte sich nicht für die LAN, die für alle Konnektiva im Vergleich mit nicht-konnektiven Elementen gefunden wurde, und die dementsprechend auch als Korrelat abstrakterer Integration von Konnektiva vorgeschlagen wird. Es zeigte sich ebenfalls kein Unterschied zwischen kausalen und temporalen Konnektiva vor dem Ende des Diskurses in Experiment 3. Die N400, gefunden für inkohärente Satzpaare in Experiment 2, größer für inkohärent konnektive als inkohärent nicht-konnektive Diskurse, wie auch die P3b, gefunden im Vergleich zwischen kausalen und konzessiven Konnektiva in Experiment 4, lieferten jedoch Grund zu der Annahme, dass die Bedeutung von Konnektiva in der inkrementellen Verarbeitung genutzt wird, und dass die Relation, die Konnektiva signalisieren, diejenige ist, die Leser versuchen zu erstellen.
Im Hinblick auf wann die Information, die Konnektiva liefern, genutzt wird, scheint es, also ob Konnektivität generell und obligatorisch für bare Münze genommen wird. So lange die Bedeutung der Konnektiva nicht mit einer präferierten kanonischen Diskursrelation konfligierte, zeigten sich keine Differenzen innerhalb verschiedener konnektiver Diskurse (Experiment 3), aber die Tatsache, dass konzessive Konnektiva die Notwendigkeit einer komplexeren Textrepräsentation voraussagen, wurde sofort erkannt und genutzt (Experiment 4). Zusätzlich resultierte eine Verletzung der Diskursrelation in Experiment 2 in erschwerterer semantischer Integration, wenn ein Konnektivum vorhanden war. Es wird daher gefolgert, dass Konnektiva die Verarbeitung sofort beeinflussen.
Diese Schlussfolgerung muss jedoch etwas abgeschwächt werden, da die satzfinalen Elemente darauf hinwiesen, dass Konnektiva andere Integrationsprozesse am Ende der Diskurse auslösten als nicht-konnektive Elemente. Es scheint, dass die Antwort auf die Frage, wann Konnektiva verarbeitet werden, weder sofort noch hinterher ist, sondern dass beide Sichtweisen korrekt sind. Es wird hier vorgeschlagen, dass vor dem Ende eines Diskurses Ökonomie insofern eine entscheidende Rolle spielt, als eine kanonische Relation angenommen wird so lange es keine gegenteilige Evidenz gibt. Ein Konnektivum könnte die evaluierten Dimensionen in einem Diskurs auf die signalisierten reduzieren. Am Ende des Diskurses wird die erstellte Textrepräsentation evaluiert und verifiziert, und ein integriertes Situationsmodell erstellt. Hier drückt sich dann die Komplexität der verschiedenen Diskursrelationen, die Konnktiva signalisieren können, aus.
Isambert, Paul. "Discours et grammaticalisation : étude de l'adverbe autrement." Phd thesis, Université de la Sorbonne nouvelle - Paris III, 2010. http://tel.archives-ouvertes.fr/tel-00912701.
Full textBachmann, Anne. "Locating Inter-Scandinavian Silent Film Culture : Connections, Contentions, Configurations." Doctoral thesis, Stockholms universitet, Institutionen för mediestudier, 2013. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-96162.
Full textSilva-Maceda, Gabriela. "Are better communicators better readers? : an exploration of the connections between narrative language and reading comprehension." Thesis, University of Wolverhampton, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/2436/311437.
Full textOlofsson, Simon. "$GME To The Moon : Mapping Memetic Discourse as Discursive Strategyin Reddit Trading Community r/WallStreetBets during the GameStop Short Squeeze Saga." Thesis, Stockholms universitet, JMK, 2021. http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:su:diva-196319.
Full textZhao, Ruilan. "Exploring Reading and Writing Connections in the Synthesis Writing of Multilingual Students in a Second Language Writing Classroom." The Ohio State University, 2015. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1433507195.
Full textBwire, David. "Meaning Across Difference: Exploring Intercultural Communication Strategies in an Alaska-Kenya Collaboration." The Ohio State University, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1469088653.
Full text尤雪瑛. "Connectives in Chinese narrative discourse." Thesis, 1990. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/66214261934202193487.
Full textShih, Yong-Siang, and 施詠翔. "Detection, Disambiguation, and Argument Identification of Chinese Discourse Connectives." Thesis, 2015. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/59072517561948350712.
Full text國立臺灣大學
資訊工程學研究所
103
Discourse relations represent how textual units logically connect with each other. Analyzing the discourse structure for texts could aid the understanding of the meaning behind paragraphs. There are many potential applications such as natural language interface and large-scale content-analysis. Although there are popular English discourse corpora for researchers, large-scale Chinese discourse corpora have not been available until recently. In addition, Chinese discourse analysis has many unique issues including the variety of discourse connectives, the common occurrences of parallel connectives, and the complex sentence structures. Discourse connectives are important clues for identifying discourse relations in Chinese texts. However, the ambiguity involved makes it a challenge to extract true connectives. In this thesis, we investigate four tasks regarding explicit discourse relations that are signaled by discourse connectives. Firstly, we deal with the extraction of explicit discourse connectives. Secondly, we investigate resolving linking ambiguities among connective components. Thirdly, we disambiguate the discourse relation type for each connective. Finally, we extract the arguments for each discourse connective. Several features are proposed to train Logistic Regression classifiers to disambiguate between discourse and non-discourse usages and the relation types for connectives. Additionally, we rank each connective candidate and develop a greedy algorithm to resolve linking ambiguities. Finally, the argument identification is formulated as a sequence labeling problem, and Conditional Random Fields are utilized to determine the argument boundaries. Besides explicit discourse relations, further investigation must be done to recognize implicit relations. Built upon these components, an end-to-end discourse parser for Chinese may be constructed in future studies.
Rysová, Magdaléna. "Diskurzní konektory v češtině.(Od centra k periferii)." Doctoral thesis, 2015. http://www.nusl.cz/ntk/nusl-350952.
Full textRysová, Magdaléna. "Alternativní vyjádření konektorů v češtině." Master's thesis, 2012. http://www.nusl.cz/ntk/nusl-304258.
Full textPoláková, Lucie. "Diskurzní vztahy v češtině." Doctoral thesis, 2015. http://www.nusl.cz/ntk/nusl-351690.
Full textZitová, Anna. "Výstavba odborného textu ve starší a v současné češtině: srovnávací syntaktický rozbor." Master's thesis, 2014. http://www.nusl.cz/ntk/nusl-338787.
Full textChen, Mei-Ling, and 陳美凌. "Ye, You, and Hai as connectives in Chinese narrative discoures." Thesis, 1993. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/90536677197881804563.
Full textBrehm-Jurish, Eva Ute [Verfasser]. "Connective ties in discourse : three ERP-studies on causal, temporal and concessive connective ties and their influence on language processing / Eva Ute Brehm-Jurish." 2006. http://d-nb.info/979508045/34.
Full textSamaras, Stephanie Ann. "#ThingsIHate:You: A study of problematic social media discourse and how we as leaders can teach to mitigate the harmful practices and effects on today’s children." Thesis, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/1828/4908.
Full textGraduate
0515
0710
0530
ssamaras@sd46.bc.ca