To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Dissimilation (Phonetics).

Journal articles on the topic 'Dissimilation (Phonetics)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 15 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Dissimilation (Phonetics).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Jatteau, Adèle, and Michaela Hejná. "Gradient dissimilation in Mongolian: implications for diachrony." Papers in Historical Phonology 3 (September 6, 2018): 28–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.2218/pihph.3.2018.2821.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper explores the implications of 'gradient dissimilation' (Jatteau & Hejná 2016) for the diachronic implementation of long-distance dissimilation (e.g. ChVCh > CVCh). Since dissimilation is usually considered as lexically sporadic, cases where it applies regularly throughout the lexicon should result from lexical diffusion. Against this assumption, we explore the hypothesis that gradient dissimilation may represent the phonetic precursor of completed, regular dissimilatory processes. Such cases might then be reanalysed as Neogrammarian types of change. To assess this question, we gather and analyse new data from Halh Mongolian, a language reported to show gradient dissimilation of the aspiration feature, and compare it to two completed patterns of aspiration dissimilation reconstructed within the Mongolic family: Chahar Mongolian and Monguor. The results suggest that the gradient dissimilation in Halh may represent the phonetic precursor of Chahar, but also that gradient dissimilation may be bidirectional for some speakers. An interesting difference between our Halh Mongolian results and the other patterns of the Mongolic family resides in the behaviour of /s/, which in our data does not pattern as expected with the aspirated stops.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Dosuna, Julián Méndez. "Deconstructing ‘height dissimilation’ in Modern Greek." Journal of Greek Linguistics 3, no. 1 (2002): 83–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/jgl.3.05men.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractA phonetic process of ‘height dissimilation’ is universally reconstructed as an intermediate stage between e.g. AGk þþþþà [ennéa] and MGK îþþþþ [eþá]. The same process is reconstructed for some Ancient Greek dialects (e.g. Boeot. îþþþà traditionally interpreted as representing [ennía]) and for other languages: e.g. Vulgar Latin. Allegedly, the existence of ‘height dissimilation’ is warranted by some modern dialects which seem to preserve the stage [enía]. In a previous paper I dealt with the data of Ancient Greek challenging this explanation. This paper deals with the evidence found in modern vernaculars. On closer inspection, the data at issue turn out to be illusory and call for an alternative reconstruction. ‘Height dissimilation’ is a mere artifact which fails to match any universal process type and lacks any real phonetic motivation either synchronically or diachronically. Synizesis (glide formation) gives a more satisfactory explanation: [eo], [ea] > [o], [a] > [jo], [ja] with non-syllabic [] turning into an optimal [j]-glide. In spite of appearances, the outcomes like [enía] are not a compelling argument for ‘height dissimilation’. There are good reasons to think that this type resulted secondarily from a process of dieresis with accent retraction: [enjá] > [enía].
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Blust, Robert. "One mark per word? Some patterns of dissimilation in Austronesian and Australian languages." Phonology 29, no. 3 (December 2012): 355–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952675712000206.

Full text
Abstract:
Adequately accounting for patterns of dissimilation has challenged more than one linguistic theory. This paper brings together evidence for certain recurrent patterns of dissimilation in Austronesian and Australian languages. It does not claim to have found a definitive solution to why these patterns occur, but it does provide indications that avoidance of multiple markedness may be causally implicated. Although the emphasis is different, the proposal offered here thus has fundamental similarities with Alderete (1997) in arguing that where it applies to dissimilation the Obligatory Contour Principle is inseparably connected with marked elements. Its primary contributions are to provide further empirical support for this claim that may not be readily accessible to non-specialists, to generalise the claim to a larger class of data, to suggest that the explanation for such patterns may be cognitive rather than phonetic and particularly to draw attention to conditions under which markedness-triggered dissimilation is suppressed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Badejo, B. Rotimi. "A phonetico-semantic analysis of verb-noun contractions in Yoruba." Studies in African Linguistics 17, no. 1 (April 1, 1986): 86–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.32473/sal.v17i1.107493.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper highlights the various studies which have been carried out to determine the rules which govern the elision of one of the vowels in a verb-noun concatenation where the verb is vowel-final and the noun vowel-initial. It proposes that there are basically three phonological rules which come into play at the Deep Structure Level. However, at the Surface Structure Level, the paper recognizes that there is a Semantic Dissimilation Principle (SDP), which may block the application of otherwise well-motivated rules. The SDP, then, guarantees maximal perceptual distance between otherwise homophonous products of the phonological rules.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

De Angelis, Alessandro. "The “phonetic prehistory” of Grassmann’s law in Greek." Folia Linguistica 54, s41-s1 (December 1, 2020): 67–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/flih-2020-0003.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Grassmann’s Law (=GL) is generally regarded as a quintessential example of regular sound change, though dissimilation processes are generally said to be quite rare and sporadic. In the present paper, evidence is presented aiming to show “the phonetic prehistory” of Grassmann’s Law in Greek, namely a diachronic stage during which GL acts in all but regular fashion. In particular, especially in some dialectal areas, some forms retain the original diaspirate stem up until Classical epoque (V–IV c. BC). Moreover, the presence of an opposite process, that is the assimilation (generally regressive) between aspirated segments, is also documented. Both of these aspects seem to show a lexically constrained spread of such a change. Its relatively late regularity can be explained as a process implemented by phonological constraints, which intervene at some point in the linguistic change.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Faber, Alice. "On The Actuation of Sound Change." Diachronica 3, no. 2 (January 1, 1986): 163–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/dia.3.2.03fab.

Full text
Abstract:
SUMMARY Careful examination of the Semitic languages provides a substantial amount of evidence for an association of hushing sibilants with a class of segments having at its core /r/ and /m/. This association is manifested in a statistically elevated number of Proto-Semitic roots in which *r and *1 co-occur (compared with those in which *r and *s co-occur) , in the sporadic appearance of reflexes of *4 instead of *s in roots containing *r or *m, and, paradoxically, in the occasional preservation in Akkadian of /s/ rather than /š/ (in roots containing one of /r m b 1/). I argue that both the assimilatory and the dissimilatory changes can best be accounted for within the context of a model of the phonetics-phonology interface which gives due weight to individual differences among language users with regard both to perceptual acuity and fine articulatory control. When these inter-speaker differences are considered, seemingly random variation will, I contend, turn out to be systematic. RÉSUMÉ Un examen detaille des langues semitiques permet d'associer les chuintantes avec une classe des sons formee autour de /r/ et /m/. Cette association se manifeste dans: (1) le nombre statistiquement éleve des racines proto-sémitiques avec a la fois *r et *4 (compare a celles avec *r et *s qui apparaissent ensemble); (2) 1'occurrence sporadique des descendants de *4 plutot que *s dans des racines avec *r et *m; et, pa-radoxalement, (3) la preservation de /s/ en Akkadien au lieu de /2/ (dans des racines comprennant l'un des segments suivants: /r m b 1/). Je maintiens que les changements dfassimilation et de dissimilation s'expliquent au mieux dans le cadre dfun modele dfinterface phonético-phonologique qui mettrait en valeur les differences inter-individuelles vis-a-vis de leur acuité perceptive et de leur contrôtle fin d'articulation. Je soutiens qu'en tenant compte de ces differences inter-indivi-duelles, des variations qui paraissent aleatoires se réveleront motivees. ZUSAMMENFASSUNG Eine sorgfaltige Untersuchung der semitischen Sprachen erlaubt es, Zischlaute mit einer Klasse von Segmenten zu verbinden, die /r/ und /m/ als ihr Zentrum haben. Diese Verbindung manifestiert sich in statistisch hoher Zahl bei proto-semitischen Wurzeln, in denen *r und *ir gemeinsam auftreten (im Vergleich zu solchen, in denen *r und *s zusammen auftre-ten), in der seltenen Erscheinung von Reflexen von *3r anstelle von *s in Wurzeln, die *r oder *m enthaiten, und, paradoxerweise, in der gelegent-lichen Erhaltung von /s/ im Akkadischen anstelle von /s/ in Wurzeln, die einen von /r m b 1/ enthalten. In diesem Aufsatz wird der Argument vorge-bracht, das sowohl die assimilatorischen als auch die dissimilatorischen Prozesse am besten erklart werden konnen, wenn man ihnen ein Modell zu-grundelegt, in welchem phonetische und phonologische Gesichtspunkte be-riicksichtigt werden, die individuellen Unterschieden unter den Sprechern in Hinblick auf perzeptive Scharfe und genaue artikulatorische Kontrolle hinreichend Rechnung tragen. Wenn einmal die inter-individuellen Unter-schiede in Betracht gez.ogen worden sind, dann erweist sich, wie hier vor-geschlagen, die scheinbar zufallige Variation als in der Tat systematisch.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Marchand, James W. "Germanic umlaut from a physiological and phonological point of view: Prolegomena to a theory of sound change." American Journal of Germanic Linguistics and Literatures 2, no. 2 (July 1990): 109–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1040820700000445.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThis paper represents a return to the theme of Germanic umlaut, umlaut in general, and sound change in general. It invokes first the concept of the parameter, then the concept of parametric inertia. That is, since all parameters are produced by physical organs, they are governed by inertia, and this allows a (phonetic) explanation of assimilation (including umlaut) and dissimilation. Further, the concept of a hierarchy of parameters based on a phonetically most similar notion is developed to replace the unworkable phonetically similar criterion in classical phonemics.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

de la Fuente Iglesias, Monica, and Susana Perez Castillejo. "Phonetic interactions in the bilingual production of Galician and Spanish /e/ and /o/." International Journal of Bilingualism 24, no. 2 (February 10, 2019): 305–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367006919826868.

Full text
Abstract:
Aims and objectives/purpose/research questions: This study investigates the cross-linguistic phonetic interactions in the production of closed mid vowels of Galician-Spanish bilinguals in a semi-urban area in Galicia. The research questions were: (1) Do Galician-Spanish bilinguals produce /e/ and /o/ similarly in their two languages? (2) Does language dominance influence whether these sounds are produced more like Spanish or more like Galician? (3) Do age and gender influence the bilingual production of Galician and Spanish /e/ and /o/? Design/methodology/approach: Thirty Galician-Spanish bilinguals from the western coast of Galicia completed a sociolinguistic questionnaire and two reading tasks in both Galician and Spanish. Data and analysis: 4,728 vowels were acoustically analyzed and submitted to several mixed effect statistical models. Findings/conclusions: Bilinguals in this study produce distinct phonetic categories for their four Galician mid vowels, and additionally have created new phonetic categories to accommodate their L2 (Spanish) vowels, resulting in a more crowded mid-vowel phonetic space. Originality: This is the first study to examine closed mid vowel realizations in both Spanish and Galician by bilinguals who maintain a Galician four mid-vowel contrast, and to investigate their complex L1-L2 phonetic interactions. Significance/implications: This study offers new data to examine how early bilinguals organize their phonetic system(s) through cross-linguistic assimilation and dissimilation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Dmitrieva, Olga, Allard Jongman, and Joan A. Sereno. "The Effect of Instructed Second Language Learning on the Acoustic Properties of First Language Speech." Languages 5, no. 4 (October 26, 2020): 44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/languages5040044.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper reports on a comprehensive phonetic study of American classroom learners of Russian, investigating the influence of the second language (L2) on the first language (L1). Russian and English productions of 20 learners were compared to 18 English monolingual controls focusing on the acoustics of word-initial and word-final voicing. The results demonstrate that learners’ Russian was acoustically different from their English, with shorter voice onset times (VOTs) in [−voice] stops, longer prevoicing in [+voice] stops, more [−voice] stops with short lag VOTs and more [+voice] stops with prevoicing, indicating a degree of successful L2 pronunciation learning. Crucially, learners also demonstrated an L1 phonetic change compared to monolingual English speakers. Specifically, the VOT of learners’ initial English voiceless stops was shortened, indicating assimilation with Russian, while the frequency of prevoicing in learners’ English was decreased, indicating dissimilation with Russian. Word-final, the duration of preceding vowels, stop closures, frication, and voicing during consonantal constriction all demonstrated drift towards Russian norms of word-final voicing neutralization. The study confirms that L2-driven phonetic changes in L1 are possible even in L1-immersed classroom language learners, challenging the role of reduced L1 use and highlighting the plasticity of the L1 phonetic system.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Helms, Annie. "Bidirectionality of language contact: Spanish and Catalan vowels." Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 6, no. 1 (March 20, 2021): 159. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v6i1.4956.

Full text
Abstract:
The disproportionate number of studies in Barcelona and the Balearic Islands observing Spanish contact effects in Catalan production, rather than Catalan contact effects in Spanish production, is an oversight of bidirectionality and the probabilistic nature of social factors in situations of language contact. Accordingly, the present study analyzes both Catalan and Spanish mid front vowel production data from Barcelona to investigate whether Catalan contact effects occur in Spanish via a process of dissimilation, and whether such effects are strengthened in younger speakers due to the relatively recent implementation of Catalan linguistic policy in the educational and public spheres. The results are suggestive of dissimilation, where phonetic distinctions are maintained between Spanish /e/ and the two Catalan mid front vowels across both F1 and F2. Additionally, analyses of variance across F1 and F2 reveal that Spanish /e/ productions across F1 are more diffuse in younger speakers and Catalan mid front vowels across F2 are less diffuse, providing evidence of reciprocity in contact effects. These results underscore the bidirectional nature of language contact and advocate for the use of variance of F1 and F2 as a metric of phonological contact effects.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Kazlauskienė, Asta. "Consonants at the juncture of verb morphemes." Lietuvių kalba, no. 6 (December 28, 2012): 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.15388/lk.2012.22704.

Full text
Abstract:
The boundaries of morphemes constituting words in the Lithuanian language are not always clear, various phonological processes take place at a morpheme juncture: either front or back sounds are adjusted to the adjacent sounds of the other morpheme, sometimes morphemes overlap – sounds are skipped or contracted. The reasons of such variation are phonological (when geminates that are not characteristic of the Lithuanian language are eliminated, degemination, dissimilation, contraction take place) and phonetic (when sounds are switched or omitted to make pronunciation easier, metathesis, elimination take place). The former changes are more frequent.The research data for the article have been compiled from the Reverse Dictionary of Contemporary Lithuanian and the electronic Database of Morphotactics and Phonotactics which is compiled on the basis of coherent texts. All in all, about 1,300 examples from the dictionary and about 500 language use examples are analysed in the article.Although consonant variations are briefly discussed in more than one research work of Lithuanian linguists, all the classifications proposed in them have their drawbacks. The present article discusses the principles that could be followed in classifying consonant changes that take place at morpheme juncture in the Lithuanian language.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Abd. Rahman, Mohd Zaki. "Asimilasi Dan Disimilasi Dalam Bahasa Arab." Jurnal Teknologi, January 20, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.11113/jt.v46.310.

Full text
Abstract:
Asimilasi dan disimilasi ialah dua fenomena bahasa yang berlaku disebabkan penyelarasan dalam sebutan huruf bagi meringankan serta melancarkan proses pertuturan. Kedua–duanya melibatkan aspek fonologi. Asimilasi berkait rapat dengan hukum idgham dalam pembacaan al–Qur’an al–Karim. Oleh itu kebanyakan contoh mengenainya terdapat dalam al–Qur’an al–Karim. Nisbah kewujudan asimilasi jauh lebih besar berbanding disimilasi. Oleh sebab nisbah disimilasi yang begitu kecil, ia boleh dianggap sebagai kes terpencil dalam bahasa Arab. Kata kunci: Asimilasi, disimilasi, makhraj, sifat huruf, pola kata Assimilation and dissimilation are two language phenomenon which occur in the process of articulation. It is explicitly seen when a speaker tries to moderate pronunciation of letters in an effort to speak smoothly and fluently. Both assimilation and dissimilation involve phonetics and phonology. Assimilation in Arabic language is very closely related to the ‘idgham’ rules, which are used in reciting the al–Qur’an al–Karim. As such, most examples of assimilation are found in the Holy al–Qur’an al–Karim. It is also noted that the ratio of occurrence of assimilation is very much higher compared to dissimilation. Hence, dissimilation could be considered as negligible in Arabic language. Key words: Assimilation, dissimilation, point of articulation, attribute, metre
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Burns, Roslyn. "Persistent innovations and historical conspiracies as reanalysis and extension." Diachronica, May 11, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/dia.19034.bur.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This article explores temporally extended innovations as a form of phonological reanalysis and extension. Polabian (West Slavic) exhibits multiple dissimilatory innovations that target the reflexes of Late Common Slavic (LCS) *v/w. In this paper, I propose that the outputs of syllable structure changes in early West Slavic were reinterpreted as restrictions on the distribution of [w], thus introducing dissimilation to the language. The new grammar was not able to stop other innovations from occurring (prophylaxis) and instead restructured intermediate outputs from subsequent innovations into an acceptable phonetic form (repair) thereby extending dissimilation to new items. I demonstrate that (a) outputs of an earlier onset epenthesis grammar conform to the surface-true generalizations enforced by the reanalyzed dissimilation grammar and (b) a single grammar can account for both the dissimilation which developed during the West Slavic period and subsequent extensions which occurred after Polabian was fully differentiated from its relatives.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Faytak, Matthew. "Dissimilation by surface correspondence in Aghem velarized diphthongs." Proceedings of the Annual Meetings on Phonology 1, no. 1 (March 19, 2014). http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/amp.v1i1.52.

Full text
Abstract:
Aghem exhibits a static co-occurrence restriction involving an intrusive velar segment present in two of the language's falling diphthongs. I provide a brief descriptive overview of these unusual phonetic structures and an Agreement by Correspondence (ABC) analysis of the phenomenon as dissimilation with the intent of avoiding suboptimal correspondence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Aurrekoetxea, Gotzon. "Analysis of the morphological variation of Basque." Dialectologia et Geolinguistica 24, no. 1 (January 1, 2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/dialect-2016-0002.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe lack of homogeneous data recorded by similar methodologies has been a handicap to the development of more advanced studies on the dialectal variation of Basque. Now that the first five volumes of the Linguistic Atlas of the Basque Language have been published, researchers have access to a great amount of data and the possibility to use more sophisticated procedures to analyze the variation of the Basque language from a geolinguistic point of view.In this contribution, we use data taken from the fifth volume of this atlas, which is devoted to noun morphology. First, these data will be analyzed linguistically, and instead of using the phonetic representation, we will use the phonological or underlying representation. In order to do this, we will analyze the inventory of the morphological suffixes used in nominal inflexion cases and the phonological rules which appear in these cases (mainly assimilation, dissimilation, insertion and deletion), using the classical view of generative phonology.As far as the cartography of the data is concerned, we will use the recently created
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography