To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Educational psychology; Cognitive psychology.

Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Educational psychology; Cognitive psychology'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Educational psychology; Cognitive psychology.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Clark, Kevin Michael. "An embodied cognitive analysis of social situativity." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2005. http://wwwlib.umi.com/dissertations/fullcit/3163017.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Indiana University, Dept. of Counseling and Educational Psychology, 2005.
Adviser: Donald J. Cunningham. Title from dissertation home page (viewed Oct. 18, 2006). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 66-02, Section: A, page: 0486.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Vitális, Emese Éva. "Mental imagery, learning styles, and text comprehension : studies in educational and cognitive psychology /." [ Nijmegen ] : [ Quickprint ], 2004. http://catalogue.bnf.fr/ark:/12148/cb40223804j.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Salzman, Stephanie Ann. "Meta-Analysis Of Studies Investigating The Effects Of Father Absence On Children's Cognitive Development." Scholarly Commons, 1986. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/3389.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of the present study was the integration of the father-absence research to determine the effects of father absence on children's cognitive development as assessed by standardized intelligence, aptitude, and achievement tests and school grades. The study used the quantitative integrative review methodology of meta-analysis through which the findings from individual studies were integrated and relations between the study findings and characteristics were explored. The meta-analytic approach involved transforming the findings of individual studies to a common metric (i.e., effect size), describing and coding the characteristics of the studies, and then using the analysis of variance and multiple regression analysis to determine whether there were overall effects, subsample effects, and relations among the characteristics of the studies and the study findings. Extensive manual and computer searches uncovered 137 father-absence studies representing 9,955,118 father-absent and father-present subjects from preschool to college age. Analysis of the study findings at the highest level of aggregation yielded a mean effect size of -.26 reflecting a .26 standard deviation superiority of the father-present subjects over the father-absent subjects. Mean effect sizes were found to differ significantly as a function of age of the child at onset of the father absence, age of the subjects at time of study, sample size, sample geographic distribution, and number of matched/controlled factors in each study. Five significant correlations between study characteristics and study effect sizes were obtained: (1) larger effect sizes were associated with father-absence onset during 7-12 years of age; (2) larger effect sizes were identified with younger study subjects; (3) larger effect sizes were associated with smaller study sample sizes; (4) larger effect sizes were related to narrow geographic distributions of study samples; and (5) larger effect sizes were associated with a greater number of matched or controlled factors in the study. Only 14% of the total variance in the study effect sizes was accounted for by the composite set of predictors (i.e., study characteristics).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Mojardin, Heraldez Ambrocio 1963. "Age differences in forgetting false memories." Thesis, The University of Arizona, 1997. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/291922.

Full text
Abstract:
This study replicated and extended the results of some recent studies concerned with the effects of repeated testing in false-memory creation (e.g. Brainerd and Reyna, 1996), and recent studies concerned with the persistence of false memories over time (Brainerd and Reyna, 1996; McDermott, 1996; Payne et al., 1996). One hundred and twenty children of ages 6, 9 and 12 listened to a series of sentences and took three recognition tests (Immediate, One-week, One-month). Participants made recognition decisions about four items: (1) targets, (2) distractors with the same meaning as targets but different words, (3) distractors with different meaning than targets, but the same words, and (4) distractors with different meaning than targets and different words. Analysis of variance of hits and false alarms showed effects of repeated testing on both. Stochastic dependency analyses showed greater long-term persistence for false alarms than for hits. The effects of testing repetition in creating false memories and the persistence of false memories increased with age. Results are discussed using Fuzzy-Trace Theory as a theoretical framework.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Bowman, Shennen. "Correlation of Cognitive Load with the Physical Learning Environment for Undergraduate College Students in an Introduction to Psychology Class." Thesis, Grand Canyon University, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10937852.

Full text
Abstract:

A gap in the knowledge of literature was found in that no research had been performed examining the effect different physical learning environments have on cognitive load levels. This is important because high cognitive load levels are known to affect learning. The purpose of this quantitative correlational study is to examine the relationship between the overall, intrinsic, and extraneous cognitive load and the physical learning environment (online and traditional classrooms) of undergraduate college students in an Introduction to Psychology class, at a medium-sized liberal arts college. Cognitive load theory provides a framework that has been used extensively to promote learning. Cognitive load refers to the total amount of mental activity imposed onto the learner. Research noted what has not been examined is how different physical learning environments may affect cognitive load. For this study, the physical learning environment is the place where learning takes place. Three research questions sought a correlation between cognitive load levels and the physical learning environment, online or traditional classroom. The Leppink scale was used to measure cognitive load. A survey was sent, one week in December, until the minimum sample size was determined. Data were determined using a Spearman correlation. The findings indicated no significant relationship exists between Overall Cognitive Load and the physical learning environment (r s = –0.011, p > 0.05), Intrinsic Cognitive Load and the physical learning environment (rs = –0.082, p > 0.05), and Extraneous Cognitive Load and the physical learning environment (rs = 0.086, p > 0.05). Recommendations for future research include looking at gender differences and testing at different times during the semester.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Kalra, Priya. "Implicit Learning: Development, Individual Differences, and Educational Implications." Thesis, Harvard University, 2015. http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:16460206.

Full text
Abstract:
This dissertation attempts to link models from cognitive neuroscience with problems and models from education research as well as to advance our understanding of implicit learning. In addition to a review of the current understanding of implicit learning from psychology and neuroscience, an essay on the potential applications of implicit learning to education and two empirical studies comprise this document. The first study compares implicit learning in adults and children to address the question of developmental invariance in implicit learning. One novel aspect of this study is the use of a battery of implicit learning tasks, as well as comparison explicit learning tasks. Although gross differences were not found between adults and children in the implicit learning tasks, nevertheless first-level item analysis revealed that children and adults may differentially exploit stimulus frequency information to perform the tasks. The second study uses parallel forms of multiple implicit learning tasks to determine the reliability of implicit learning tasks for adult participants. Contrary to the prevailing view of implicit learning, stable individual differences were found. Correlations between individual implicit learning and certain non-cognitive traits (such as conscientiousness) were found, but IQ was not correlated with implicit learning. Finally, the implications of these findings for basic research as well as for the possibility of applying implicit learning to K-12 instruction are discussed.
Human Development and Education
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Berkowitz, Megan. "Understanding the Relevance of Cognitive Psychology to Composition: Taking a Closer Look at How Cognitive Psychology has Influenced Ideas about Reading, Writing, and the Teaching Process." Oxford, Ohio : Miami University, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=miami1208900950.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Denton, Stephen E. "Exploring active learning in a Bayesian framework." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2009. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3380073.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Indiana University, Dept. of Psychological and Brain Sciences the Dept. of Cognitive Science, 2009.
Title from PDF t.p. (viewed on Jul 19, 2010). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 70-12, Section: B, page: 7870. Advisers: John K. Kruschke; Jerome R. Busemeyer.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Sims-Cutler, Kristin. "The General Abilities Index as a Third Method of Diagnosing Specific Learning Disabilities." Thesis, Walden University, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3687595.

Full text
Abstract:

Many studies have investigated problems with the ability achievement discrepancy (AAD) method of diagnosing specific learning disabilities (SLDs). The definition of an SLD includes the presence of a deficit in one or more cognitive processing systems. Researchers in other studies found that the AAD method overdiagnoses English language learners and students from low socioeconomic backgrounds, and underdiagnoses students with cognitive processing deficits. Although SLD diagnostic methods have been widely researched, much less information is available regarding SLD diagnostic methods that predict important student outcomes, such as high school completion. The General Abilities Index (GAI) is an SLD diagnostic method that can identify cognitive processing deficits. This study examined the relationships between cognitive processing deficits and the GAI method, high school completion status, performance on state standards assessments, and SLD eligibility. Using a multivariate, nonexperimental design, this study analyzed 149 datasets from records of students tested for an SLD between 1996 to 2013. A GLM analysis found that several types of cognitive processing deficits predicted math and writing performance on the state standards assessment and predicted not being diagnosed with an SLD, while the GAI method failed to predict any relationship with the dependent variables. Positive social changes from this study may include improved SLD diagnostic practices and improved educational interventions that target the cognitive processing deficits. Improved educational outcomes for SLD persons may reduce the high rates of unemployment, substance abuse, and incarceration experienced by the adult SLD population.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Beasley, Vista. "Self-presentational concern as an antecedent of athletic injury." Thesis, The Florida State University, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=1552803.

Full text
Abstract:

This study was an initial investigation of the relationship between self-presentational concern and chronic injury. An experiment with two manipulations was conducted to determine how situational self-presentational concerns affected expressions of toughness that may be linked to health-damaging behaviors that cause chronic injury. Covariates of self-presentational concerns and mental toughness were measured and determined to be equivalent across groups so that effects could be attributed to the manipulations. For a manipulation of situational self-presentational concerns, collegiate middle-distance and distance runners assigned to experimental groups read a passage. The passage was based on the components of a self-presentation model, impression motivation and impression construction. It indicated the need for runners to score high on toughness questionnaires to be evaluated favorably by coaches and governing bodies of the sport in order to achieve goals. All participants completed two questionnaires pertaining to mental toughness and tough attitude toward training through pain and injury. Expressions of mental toughness related to confidence and control were higher for participants exposed to heightened levels of situational self-presentational concerns than participants lacking the exposure. For a second manipulation, participants in one of the experimental groups were additionally exposed to a prototype describing the performance of the most successful runners on the mental toughness questionnaire. Expression of mental toughness of these participants did not differ significantly from other participants, failing to provide evidence of the prototype-matching process. Participants' expression of tough attitude toward training through pain and injury did not differ in relation to exposure to either manipulation. The results support the integration of self-presentational concerns related to mental toughness as a psychological antecedent in a model linking stressful, athletic situations and chronic injury.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Rappoccio, Paul. "Proprioception And Literacy In the Digital Realm." Thesis, State University of New York at Albany, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=1571694.

Full text
Abstract:

Drawing on research in the fields of neuroscience, reading cognition, and the history of writing, the author explores the condition of reading today. Rather than accepting the apocalyptic pronouncements that the Internet is "dumbing down" current readers, the author argues for a more nuanced understanding of the effects of digital media. He argues that the literacies needed for the new digital realm are not new, but are literacies developed over thousands of years. The author argues for the need of more education and instruction in the use of digital media, and that the digital realm requires new proprioceptive (spatial awareness) abilities to navigate.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Dow, Gayle T. "An investigation of the influence of media modality (text, radio, and television) on creativity, problem solving, and recall an information processing perspective /." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2008. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3315915.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Indiana University, Dept. of Counseling and Educational Psychology, 2008.
Title from PDF t.p. (viewed on May 7, 2009). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 69-07, Section: A, page: 2601. Adviser: Jonathan A. Plucker.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Scott, Brianna Michelle. "Exploring the effects of student perceptions of metacognition across academic domains." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2008. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3331280.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Indiana University, Dept. of Counseling and Educational Psychology, 2008.
Title from PDF t.p. (viewed on Jul 23, 2009). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 69-11, Section: A, page: 4247. Adviser: Joyce Alexander.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Arici, Anne Dickson. "Meeting kids at their own game a comparison of learning and engagement in traditional and three-dimensional MUVE educational-gaming contexts /." [Bloomington, Ind.] : Indiana University, 2008. http://gateway.proquest.com/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:3342204.

Full text
Abstract:
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Indiana University, School of Education and the Dept. of Psychological and Brain Sciences, 2008.
Title from PDF t.p. (viewed on Oct. 5, 2009). Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 70-02, Section: A, page: 0469. Adviser: Joyce Alexander.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Harb, Lauren. "The use of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy to address shame in Binge Eating Disorder." Thesis, Pepperdine University, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3630236.

Full text
Abstract:

To date, research on Binge Eating Disorder is limited compared to studies on other eating disorders, including Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Given that Binge Eating Disorder recently became an independent diagnosis in the DSM-5, has significant medical implications, and commonly involves psychiatric comorbidity, it is worthwhile to explore contributing factors and evidence-based treatment for the disorder. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy is an evidence-based treatment for Binge Eating Disorder, and most experts agree that while it yields positive treatment results, there is room for improvement in treatment. Shame is an important contributing factor in the development and maintenance of Binge Eating Disorder. The purpose of this review of the literature was to examine shame literature in order to explore potential methods for improving evidence-based Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Binge Eating Disorder. The importance of researching Binge Eating Disorder is reviewed, and then shame is explored from a cognitive behavioral standpoint. Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Binge Eating Disorder is outlined, and limited techniques that address shame in treatment are identified. Recommendations for addressing shame more directly in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Binge Eating Disorder are then made prior to suggestions for future research.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Le, Marie Xuan. "The Just Distribution of Educational Resources| Children's Judgments about Differential Treatment by Teachers." Thesis, University of California, Berkeley, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3640527.

Full text
Abstract:

The aim of this study was to examine children's reasoning about the fairness of differential treatment by teachers and to determine if there were circumstances under which children may consider differential treatment to be acceptable. Participants (n = 76), ages 6 to 11 years, evaluated hypothetical stories entailing unequal distributions of different educational goods: quantity of work, turns to read aloud, and individual attention from the teacher. The teacher's rationale for the differential treatment was systematically varied in order to determine whether it altered participants' evaluations. In addition to a condition in which no rationale was given, four different rationales were proposed to underlie the differential treatment: differentiating instruction for high achieving students, differentiating instruction for struggling students, preferential treatment for favored students, and preferential treatment for boys.

The findings showed that differential treatment in the form of individual teacher attention was most acceptable to students, followed by differential assigned work, and lastly differential turns to read aloud. Children drew a distinction between the condition in which no rationale for the differential treatment was provided and the conditions in which they were. When a rationale was not presented to explain the teacher's behavior, the majority of participants rejected the differential treatment. When rationales were introduced, participants were significantly more likely to endorse differential treatment for struggling and high achieving students than for favorite students or boys. Participants were also significantly more likely to endorse differential treatment for struggling students than for high-achieving students. Analysis of justifications supporting children's evaluations of differential treatment indicated that children drew a distinction between differentiated instruction and preferential treatment.

The presence of age differences in children's judgments of differential treatment depended upon the particular educational good at stake and the teacher's rationale. When no rationale for the differential treatment was provided, 6-7 year old participants were significantly less likely to endorse differential treatment than either 8-9 year old or 10-11 year old participants. When a rationale for differential treatment was presented, there were only two conditions (out of twelve) in which significant age differences emerged. Although the majority of participants favored modified work for high achieving students, 6-7 year old participants were significantly less likely to favor it for high achievers than 10-11 year old participants. Further, 10-11 year old participants were significantly less likely to favor extra turns for struggling readers than 8-9 year old participants.

Previous research had not yet examined justice reasoning as applied to the distribution of educational opportunities in the form of differential treatment. The present study found evidence that children as young as 6 years did not equate fairness with simple equality of treatment, but recognized that special needs may warrant an unequal distribution of the teacher's individual attention, extra opportunities to read aloud, or a modification of assignments. The findings add complexity to the view that the justice concept of equality emerges earlier in ontogeny than the justice concept of merit or need. In this study, participants across ages 6 to 11 years considered claims to equality, merit, and need (i.e., the acceptability of differential treatment for high achievers and struggling students). The results present a more nuanced picture of children's justice conceptions than has been discussed in earlier work.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Mojardin-Heraldez, Ambrocio 1963. "The underlying memory processes of adults' spontaneous and implanted false memories." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/288855.

Full text
Abstract:
False memories are an important problem in many spheres of life. It is necessary to identify what kinds of memory processes cause them in order to prevent their negative consequences. This study confirmed most of fuzzy-trace theory's assumptions about the type of memory processes that underlie spontaneous and implanted false memories (Brainerd and Reyna, in press). Following the MISINFORM model's procedures, 131 university Mexican students listened to a list of words and took two recognition tests (immediate and one-week delayed). Testing lists included four types of targets and four types of distractors. Targets were: (1) control, (2) repeated/nonmisinformed, (3) nonrepeated/nonmisinformed, and (4) repeated/misinformed. Distractors were: (1) control-related distractors, (2) misinforming-related distractors that supplanted targets during misinformation-RD1, (3) misinforming related distractors presented with their instantiating targets during misinformation- RD2, and (4) unrelated distractors. Analysis of variance of hits and false alarms showed the misinformation and mere-memory testing effects. Stochastic dependency analyses found neither persistence of true nor of false memories. MISINFORM analyses showed that true memories are due to identity judgments about targets, spontaneous false memories are due to false identity and similarity judgements about related distractors, and implanted false memories are due to false identity judgments about misinforming related distractors and nonidentity judgments about misinformed targets. MISINFORM also showed that targets cue the retrieval of verbatim memories, related distractors cue the retrieval of gist memories about targets, and misinforming distractors cue verbatim memories of misinformation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Chavira, Maria Romo 1968. "Cultural differences in reasoning and memory: A follow-up." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1996. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/289477.

Full text
Abstract:
The focus of this study was to manipulate factors to determine whether mental representation of logical problems differed by culture. The cultural differences hypothesis suggests that Anglo students would be more likely to have a linear representation (e.g., arranging objects that differ in a "line" mentally) whereas Hispanic students would have a nonlinear (pivot) representation. The results indicated that Hispanic children solved questions better if they appeared in a pivotal format, whereas Anglo children performed better if the stimuli were presented in a linear method. With grade level children improved upon the linear format. This suggests that there may be differences in mental representations of objects for Hispanic children that is influenced by acculturation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Zelmanow, Ari. "The Learning Pathways of Ironman Triathletes| Case Studies of Age-Group Ironman Triathletes." Thesis, Lindenwood University, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3666916.

Full text
Abstract:

The Ironman Triathlon is an epic endurance event consisting of a 2.4-mile swim, a 112-mile bike ride, and a 26.2-mile run. There is paucity in the literature relating to how athletes learn how to negotiate this event. This qualitative study was conducted over a 9 month period, to align with the 2013 Ironman training and racing season. Seven athletes were selected for participation in the study. Utilizing a case study approach, the Ironman athletes' learning pathways were examined through in-depth interviews and audio and video content personally captured by the participants.

The learning pathways revealed the athletes initially learned through cognitive means, i.e. social interaction, reading, Internet sources, and the observation of others. As athletes traversed the learning pathway, they subsequently operationalized the knowledge they learned and constructively made it meaningful to their respective personal training and racing situations. At the terminal end of the learning pathway, the athletes operationalized the learned content in an experiential learning cycle. During the entire learning pathway, the athletes practiced the learned content, which is best characterized as behavioral learning. The audio and video content provided by the athletes empirically validated the interviews.

The interviews with the athletes were coded. Some unifying themes emerged from the data independent of cognitive, constructivist, behavioral, or experiential learning theories; e.g. the importance of mental toughness, the understanding of pain during the training and racing process, how success is measured, the importance of training with a power meter, and motivating factors.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Rodriguez, Vanessa. "Exploring Social-Emotional Cognition and Psychophysiologic Synchrony During Teaching Interactions." Thesis, Harvard University, 2016. http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:27112694.

Full text
Abstract:
Conceptions of learning have evolved from behaviorist and mechanistic models to models that are dynamic, complex, and process-oriented. Educators’ and researchers’ understanding of teaching can benefit from a similar evolution, one that embodies teaching as a dynamic, cognitive skill. Because teaching is an inherently interactive phenomenon, it can be characterized as a social-emotional cognitive skill. Using a dynamic systems and social-emotional cognitive lens, in this dissertation, I describe two studies that explored teaching from these perspectives. The first, presented in Chapter 2, used structured cognitive interviews to elicit expert teachers’ reflections on their teaching processes, as well as to illuminate the interactive nature of these processes and the factors that influence the teachers’ capabilities. In the second study, presented in Chapter 3, I borrowed methods from interactive social-emotional cognitive studies to identify periods of psychophysiologic synchrony between the teachers and students to determine if these correlated with their relational experiences. Five themes, or awarenesses, emerged from the interview data: (a) awareness of learner (AoL), (b) awareness of teaching practice (AoTP), (c) awareness of context (AoC), (d) awareness of self as a teacher (AoST), and (e) awareness of interaction (AoI). Within each theme, I identified several categories that characterized the teachers’ reflections on the individual social-emotional cognitive processes employed during teaching. These data show that expert teaching leverages a complex, social-emotional cognitive framework to achieve learning goals. In the second study, I observed significant increases of psychophysiologic synchrony in the teacher–student dyads that were engaged in a supported teaching task. This elevated synchrony was correlated with multiple domains of two established measures of individual social-emotional cognition. Moreover, after dividing the data by the median of achieved synchrony into an upper and lower group, strong but unique correlation patterns were observed between the teacher–student synchrony and the social-emotional cognitive survey measures. In particular, several student measures of the teachers’ perspective taking demonstrated inverse associations between the lower and upper 50th percentiles of synchrony. These data indicated that the ability to create synchrony during supported interactions was connected to the teacher’s distinct social-emotional cognitive capacity. These results also support the potential neurobiologic and psychophysiologic bases of teachers’ social-emotional cognitive processing. Together, these two studies represent an initial step along a larger trajectory of future research that could advance the conception of teaching as a social-emotional cognitive skill that develops as a complex system.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Cheng, San Chye. "Effects of Socio-Cognitive Conflicts on Group Cognition and Group Performance." Thesis, Harvard University, 2014. http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:13383546.

Full text
Abstract:
Socio-cognitive conflict is a mechanism that drives cognitive development/learning in collaborative learning. Such conflicts occur when individuals have different perspectives on the same problem. To adequately solve problems, groups face the challenge of integrating different perspectives, which when successful can result in an increase in shared knowledge (i.e., knowledge convergence), an intermediate process-related collaborative learning outcome. Knowledge convergence plays an important role in explaining the quality of group performance, an ultimate collaborative learning outcome. However, students do not always learn from one another in groups, with studies revealing variability in collaborative learning outcomes. Among other factors, their communication can be unproductive or productive. This mixed evidence could be because: (1) interactions have not been analyzed using instruments developed with theoretical and empirical underpinnings within a socio-cognitive conflict paradigm to measure a comprehensive range of socio-cognitive processes; and (2) it is insufficient to assign group tasks without providing any scaffolding. My study acknowledges these issues and uses a randomized experimental design that aims to: (1) Test out a script that strives to scaffold interactions to generate collaborative socio-cognitive processes. To analyze interactions, my study uses an instrument capable of identifying a comprehensive range of socio-cognitive processes; and (2) Examine the effect of socio-cognitive processes on knowledge convergence and consequently on the quality of group performance. Findings suggest that the script offers a promising way to facilitate the type of productive communication to make group-work beneficial. It generated interactions with collaborative socio-cognitive processes. Additionally, the frequency of collaborative socio-cognitive processes is positively related to the increase in shared knowledge in terms of the number of similar elements and statements members had in common after dyads’ interactions. Also, the increase in the number of similar elements is positively related to the quality of dyads’ performance, whereas there is no corresponding effect for statements. Implications for designing collaborative learning activities include requiring the duration of students’ interactions to be long enough to have sufficient collaborative socio-cognitive processes so as to have substantial knowledge convergence and higher quality of group performance. Future studies include addressing issues regarding measurement accuracy in analyzing chat-logs and knowledge convergence.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Rauch, Thyra. "Effects of Short and Long Study Times on Learning by Maps Versus Navigation." W&M ScholarWorks, 1987. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539625423.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Ryle, Mary Katherine. "Exploring the Prevalence of Learning Styles in Educational Psychology and Introduction to Education Textbooks: A Content Analysis." TopSCHOLAR®, 2017. https://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/2045.

Full text
Abstract:
The implementation of learning styles models in the classroom remains a heavily debated topic in education. Notable problems with utilization of learning styles in the classroom include a lack of empirical research support and potential maladaptive effects on student learning and motivation. The primary research questions focused on the presence and quantity of learning styles discussion in the text, which definitions, models, and recommendations were presented, and which of the cited references were based on empirical data. The answers to these questions were compared between educational psychology and introduction to education textbooks. A content analysis of introduction to education (n = 10) and educational psychology (n = 10) textbooks was conducted. Eighty percent of the textbooks included a discussion of learning styles. Half of the textbooks defined learning style as a preference or approach and the other half as an individual process or style. One-fourth of the textbooks recommended matching instructional methods to learning styles. One comparison of text types, the number of empirical references cited in the text, was statistically significant. Given that most textbooks do not recommend matching instructional methods to learning styles, future research should examine the source of teachers’ beliefs that student learning is improved with the matching of learning styles to teaching approach.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Alrshed, Afnan Mohammed. "The Effect of Chewing Frankincense (Boswellia Sacra) Gum on Recall and Recognition of Stories Presented in Auditory Forms." University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1492446169320554.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Ellis, Carl Richard. "The utility of a computerized assessment battery to evaluate cognitive functioning and attention." W&M ScholarWorks, 1991. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539618883.

Full text
Abstract:
In recent years much attention has been given to the application of computer technology to psychometric methods, but researchers have concentrated on adapting traditional methods of psychological testing to the new technology instead of utilizing it to develop innovative methods of assessment. The purpose of this study was to determine whether a computerized assessment battery designed to evaluate cognitive functioning and attention could demonstrate reliability and validity. The Computerized Cognitive Assessment Battery (CCAB) was developed according to the PASS Model of Cognitive Functioning and administered via a Macintosh computer and test results included response style variables (mouse movement and response time). Children having attention problems (N = 25) in grades three through five were compared to a random group of children (N = 29). On the newly developed CCAB, the majority of the sectional variables displayed significant intercorrelations (p {dollar}<{dollar}.01) indicating internal consistency of this measure. The reliability of the Sequential component of the CCPT was found to be.90 for Scale 1 and.83 for Scale 2. No relationship was found between the covert measures on the CCAB and Conners' parent and teacher rating scales. Evidence indicated that the covert measures are related to the Planning Factor. The attention measure of the CCPT was able to discriminate between the two groups as well as the Gordon Diagnostic System. The total CCAB was able to predict group membership with one hundred percent accuracy using the classification results of the discriminant function analysis. Consistent with the prediction of the PASS model, the Attention component (CCPT) was the only area in which the scores of the two groups differed. The present study demonstrated the feasibility and practicality of a fully-computerized cognitive assessment battery to aid in the assessment process. The results of this research indicate that the potential exists to evaluate cognitive functioning by a computer-based assessment system. Not only could such a test provide an index of intellectual ability based on a well researched and extensively used IQ test (Raven), it could also yield a great deal of information related to meta-cognitive skills, self-regulatory behavior, processing styles and compensatory mechanisms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Bickart, John. "The possible role of intuition in the child's epistemic beliefs in the Piagetian data set." Thesis, The University of North Carolina at Charlotte, 2013. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3589794.

Full text
Abstract:

U.S. schools teach predominately to the analytical, left-brain, which has foundations in behaviorism, and uses a mechanistic paradigm that influences epistemic beliefs of how learning takes place. This result is that learning is impeded. Using discourse analysis of a set of Piagetian children, this study re-analyzed Piaget's work. This study found that, although the participating children answered from both an intuitive and an analytical perspective, Piaget's analysis of the interviews ignored the value in the intuitive, right-brain answers; Piaget essentially stated that the children were only doing valuable thinking when they were analytical and logical. Using other comparable re-analysis as the yardstick, this study extended Piaget's original interpretations. Implications for teaching and learning are also described. This study also extends a call for research into a pedagogical balance between analytic and intuitive teaching.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Liao, Shih-Chieh. "How logical reasoning ability and empirical knowledge interact in the process of solving problems about light and vision among Taiwanese secondary school students." Diss., The University of Arizona, 2002. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/280194.

Full text
Abstract:
Piagetian scholars argue that the effect of problem content, e.g., empirical knowledge, should decrease with age. Indeed, they believe that the empirical knowledge cannot affect human problem-solving after individuals approach the formal operation stage. In arguing this point, this study uses an A-AR model to address how empirical knowledge affects the problem-solving process among Taiwanese secondary students. The A-AR model is borrowed from mathematics and the symbols, A, A, and R, represent Assumption, Answering, and Reasoning, respectively. Similar to solving mathematics problems, the A-AR model problems require participants to use the given assumptions by logical reasoning in order to respond to the problems. In this situation, the effect of empirical knowledge on problem-solving is easy to detect. There are three results about human problem-solving found in this study. First, the empirical knowledge still affects human problem-solving at the formal operation stage. Not like the Piagetian scholars' assumption: the effect of empirical knowledge is decreasing with age, this study finds that the effect of empirical knowledge is S-shape. The S-shape is a result of academic training. Second, the academic training, major, shapes human problem-solving strategies. For instance, the 12th grade science students' problem-solving strategy is based on logical reasoning ability by the given assumptions and the same grade social science students' strategy is according of their empirical knowledge. Third, the interference of logical reasoning ability and empirical knowledge is a predictor of the empirical knowledge effect on human problem-solving. The relation between the empirical knowledge and interference can be characterized as: the more negative interference the participants have, the more of the empirical knowledge effect they will have in the next year. This study does not agree with the Piagetian theory about human problem-solving: the effect of empirical knowledge should decrease with age. Indeed, this study argues that the problem content still affects human problem-solving after individuals move into the formal operation stage. The different kinds of academic training---science and social science major---shape human problem-solving strategies into either a logical reasoning base or an empirical knowledge stand, respectively.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Rinholm, Joanne. "The mediating role of task orientation in the relation between parenting practices and children's cognitive performance." Thesis, University of Ottawa (Canada), 1992. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/7927.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study was designed to clarify the relations between parenting practices and cognitive performance in school-aged children. Particular attention was directed toward the identification of gender differences in the pattern of relations among variables and toward investigating a model of how parenting practices may influence children's cognitive performance. The model postulated that parenting practices affect children's task orientation (i.e., mastery behaviors and impulse control), which in turn affects their cognitive performance. Using path analysis, the model was assessed in a sample of 63 female and 62 male Grade 6 children. The measure of cognitive performance was academic grades, and the possible confounding influence of cognitive ability upon academic grades was controlled. As expected, maternal encouragement of independence predicted both boys' and girls' academic performance, with mastery behaviors mediating the relation in the female sample. Paternal nurturance also predicted girls' academic performance, with impulse control mediating this association. The remaining findings of the study provided support for individual components of the model. With respect to the relations between parenting practices and task orientation, maternal nurturance predicted both higher mastery behaviors and impulse control in the female sample. For girls, paternal restrictive control predicted lower impulse control, whereas maternal restrictive control predicted higher impulse control. For boys, both maternal and paternal nurturance predicted higher impulse control. In both the male and female samples, the expected associations between academic performance and both mastery behaviors and impulse control were obtained. The implications of the results and suggestions for future research were discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Porter, Kristen M. "An Exploratory Study of the Need for Cognition in Children and Adolescents." University of Toledo / OhioLINK, 2010. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=toledo1290134272.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Vekiari, Konstantina. "The structure of social and cognitive development in Native American children." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/288931.

Full text
Abstract:
An important question in the area of child development is the nature of the relationship between social and cognitive development. Does cognitive development affect social competence, are there reciprocal effects between the two areas, or does social competence affect cognitive development? The present study investigated the direction of effect between social development and cognitive development in Native American children during early childhood. The study involved the secondary analysis of existing data from the Navajo Nation Transition Project. Data for one hundred and fifty-one children attending Kindergarten were used in the study. The children were graduates of Head Start, a program that offered services to promote the development of low-income children and their families. Cognitive and social developmental level in this study was measured by a measurement and planning system (MAPS) devised for the assessment of young children's developing ability through a path-referenced approach. A nonrecursive linear structural equation model was used to examine if there was a reciprocal relationship between social and cognitive development. The study revealed a direction of effect from cognitive development to social development. No reciprocal relationship was found between the two areas of development. Future research directions and implications were also addressed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Chan, Mei Ling. "Web-Based Usability Evaluation of Text-Resizing Methods and Users' Visual Fatigue on Online Reading Tasks." Thesis, California State University, Long Beach, 2017. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10638681.

Full text
Abstract:

A web-based usability evaluation was conducted to examine the effects of two text-resizing methods on normal and low vision Internet users’ online reading experience. Normal or corrected to normal vision (N/CTN; N = 50) and low vision (N = 5) participants completed two blocks of four mock Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) reading tests on the experiment website using fixed width (FW) and text reflow (TR) text-resizing methods. Subjective visual fatigue scores, modified System Usability Scale (SUS) score, time on task, and accuracy were collected as dependent measures for user experience evaluation. Results showed that when FW method was presented in the second block, N/CtN participants’ visual fatigue continued to increase, along with a significant decrease in time spent on reading tasks. Such effects were not observed in TR method. Results of low vision participants were different from those obtained in lab settings, suggesting that testing environment may have played a role in low vision participants’ performance. Implications of the current research can be used to guide future web accessibility research in regards to online experiment design and recruitment of participants with visual impairments.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Jahner, Erik Erwin. "Resting as Knowing| A Lagged Structure Analysis of Resting State fMRI with Application to Mind Wandering during Oral Reading." Thesis, University of California, Riverside, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10680604.

Full text
Abstract:

The human brain is an ongoing dynamic system not activated by experience but nudged from intrinsic activity into new network configurations during perception and learning. Ongoing neural activity during rest is assumed to reflect these intrinsic dynamics in a relatively closed system state. Traditionally, inter-regional connectivity in this system is measured by obtaining time-locked correlations in BOLD activity using fMRI. It is well documented, however, that neural activity unfolds across time and is not isolatent to some reference point.

This exploratory study is a theoretical analysis of how a lagged analysis of resting state dynamics in fMRI could represent persistent representations of knowledge in the neocortex. A novel procedure using both surface based maps and independent component analysis (ICA) is applied to a small group of 54 adolescents. The ICA methods appear to reveal lagged structures with different information than traditional resting state analysis. The group level results are symmetrical between hemispheres and may represent high level perceptual systems. 

The components obtained from this exploration are then used to attempt understand how these knowledge systems in neocortex frame mind-wandering frequency when reading aloud in a subset of 38 individuals. The results did not correlate with any known neural systems related to mind wandering, but the methods here are unique. One of the identified components shows significant difference in the lag structure of the occipital cortex as a function of mind wandering frequency during oral reading. This demonstrates that it may be worth exploring the timing in visual system to understand why individuals mind wander when reading aloud. Reverse inference is used to interpret results and suggest future approaches.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Clark, Chadwick W. "Estimates of association between cognitive complexity levels and creativity levels of field grade military officers : an exploratory study of the relationship." Diss., Manhattan, Kan. : Kansas State University, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/2097/858.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Miller, Emma. "The development of cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) for children and young people within an educational psychology service." Thesis, University of Newcastle Upon Tyne, 2010. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.512172.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Bursuk, Lois Ilene. "The effects of a school-based cognitive-behavioral intervention program on the depression scores of sixth-grade students: A comparison outcome study." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282610.

Full text
Abstract:
The effects of a school-based cognitive-behavioral intervention approach on the depression scores of sixth grade students were examined in the study. Two hundred and one sixth grade students served as participants in one of four experimental groups: treatment group, attention-placebo group, delayed treatment group, and no treatment control group. All groups, except the control group, participated in the school-based program called "learned optimism." The learned optimism program is an eight-week curriculum-based program designed to assist adolescents in developing a more optimistic self-explanatory style that contributes to resiliency and positive mental health. All participants completed the Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) on three occasions: before the learned optimism program began (pretest), immediately after the first eight-week program was terminated (posttest 1) and eight weeks later, after the second eight-week program was terminated (posttest 2/follow-up). The results were unexpected. They showed only a significant difference on CDI total scores between the treatment group and delayed treatment group immediately following both groups' participation in the learned optimism program. No significant differences on CDI total scores were found at any other time between or within any of the four groups. Some significant differences were found on CDI subscales between the treatment and delayed treatment groups, but not in the expected direction. Results from an informal questionnaire completed by participants showed that most liked the learned optimism program and it made them feel happier. Plausible explanations for the findings were discussed along with limitations of the study and recommendations for future research in this area.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Boudreault, Patrick. "Grammatical processing in American Sign Language, effects of age of acquisition and syntactic complexity." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1999. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk1/tape3/PQDD_0032/MQ64322.pdf.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Gibbs, Benjamin Guild. "Gender and Cognitive Skills throughout Childhood." The Ohio State University, 2009. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1249496662.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Gunther, Steven. "Preschools, Pencils, Promise| Cognitive Functioning, Academic Achievement, and Behavior Problems as Correlates of Inhibitory Control Among Head Start Participants." Thesis, Alliant International University, 2017. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10281603.

Full text
Abstract:

The Pencil-tapping Task is an assessment of key neuropsychological components such as cognitive inhibitory control (Smith-Donald, Raver, Hayes, & Richardson, 2007), attention, and working memory (Xue, Atkins-Burnett, Caronongan, & Moiduddin, 2011). These constructs are parts of executive function, which governs the brain’s ability to analyze and respond to its own processes. Given that executive function has been linked to number knowledge, expressive and receptive vocabulary, and classroom engagement (Fitzpatrick & Pagani, 2012), we may expect tests of executive function and its development to correlate with tests of cognitive functioning, academic achievement, and classroom behavior problems. This study, an analysis of archival data from the Head Start Family and Child Experiences Survey of 2009, seeks to find connections between inhibitory control, cognitive function, academic achievement, and behavior in one of the largest studies ever to feature the relatively new Pencil-tapping Task.

The central variable in this study, the Pencil-tapping Task, is used as an operationalization of inhibitory control development. Research questions seek to identify relationships between this task, a measure of receptive vocabulary development called the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (Dunn & Dunn, 2007), a measure of expressive vocabulary development called the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (Brownell, 2000), measures of language and mathematics skill development contained within the Woodcock-Johnson-III battery (McGrew & Woodcock, 2001), and measures of behavioral control observed by Head Start classroom teachers (West, Tarullo, Aikens, Malone, & Carlson, 2011).

Multi-level, simple regressions demonstrated that a measure of inhibitory control significantly predicted variance on measures of functioning, achievement, and classroom behavior problems. Large effect sizes were observed for the relationship between inhibitory control and cognitive functioning, as well as the relationship between race/ethnicity and cognitive functioning. These results are similar to those obtained by other researchers of preschool executive function development, and provide support for a general model of executive function predicting both cognitive functioning, academic achievement, and classroom behavior.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Ippolito, Karen Odle. "Cognitive development and the attainment of critical thinking skills in associate degree nursing students." Scholarly Commons, 2011. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/114.

Full text
Abstract:
Piaget's final stage of cognitive development, formal operations, involves reasoning skills that appear to relate to the traits of critical thinking. The current study was designed to assess whether a relationship exists between the level of cognitive development (as measured by the Lawson Classroom Test of Scientific Reasoning [Lawson CTSR]) and critical thinking (as measured by Assessment Technology Institute Critical Thinking Assessment-Entrance test [ATI-CTA-Ent]). This study explored the possible relationship between the level of cognitive development and the level of critical thinking exhibited by first semester nursing students seeking an Associate Degree (ADN). It also explored the relationship between specific subsets of cognitive development and critical thinking as a whole as well as between cognitive development and the distinct traits that characterize critical thinking. Furthermore, this project asked whether the level of general knowledge held by nursing students (as measured by Assessment Technology Institute Test of Essential Academic Skills [ATI-TEAS] is correlated with the level of cognitive development. All statistical analyses controlled for gender, age, and prior schooling. A sample of 190 first semester nursing students were administered the Lawson CSTR as a test of cognitive development, the ATI-CTA-Ent as a test of critical thinking and the ATI-TEAS as a test of general knowledge. In a hierarchical multiple regression analysis it was found that cognitive development accounted for 19.3% of the variance in critical thinking scores after controlling for gender, age and prior schooling. With multiple subscales on both the predictor (Lawson CTSR) and the criterion (ATI CIA) side, regression models noted ATI-Inference with the largest explained variance (21.15%) and the smallest for ATI Evaluation (9.36%). The covariates explained 9.6% and cognitive development explained an additional 33.6% of the variance in general knowledge. These findings suggest that measures of cognitive development are associated with measures of critical thinking ability.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Beck, Corbatto Deborah. "Trust as a Precursor of Flow| A Social Cognitive View of Flow in Elite Coach/Athlete Dyads." Thesis, George Mason University, 2018. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=10792355.

Full text
Abstract:

The purpose of the present study was to explore the influence of trust, help seeking/help giving, and relation-inferred self-efficacy on the onset of flow experience in a dyadic relationship between an elite athlete and their coach. The social cognitive theory of triadic reciprocal determinism was used to examine the relationship of the elite athlete and their trusted coach in a high-pressure athletic environment as it related to the ability of the elite athlete to achieve a flow state. Using a multiple case study approach, semi-structured interviews were conducted with five coach-athlete dyads. Data were analyzed using thematic network analysis (i.e., looking for thematic ties to established theory, as well as emerging themes). Prior research has focused on flow as a personally experienced phenomenon arising due to constructs that are largely self-controlled (e.g., loss of self-consciousness, merging of action and awareness, autotelic nature, centering of attention, feeling in control). Findings of this study, based on the triadic reciprocal determinism model, showed support for a more expansive model for flow in elite sport dyads, including behavioral, personal, and environmental influences, particularly in the area of trust. Based on findings of this study, recommendations are made for further research, including the necessity for sports flow research to move to a more applied focus using social cognitive theory. Implications of this line of research include uncovering the method by which an elite coach might create an environment in which flow experiences and improved performance outcomes might ideally occur for the athlete.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Emmett, Daniel Warren. "Individual Ability to Learn a Parallel Processing Technique and Musical Aptitude." ScholarWorks, 2018. https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations/5325.

Full text
Abstract:
Correlations between music training and psychosocial skills, sensory abilities, and aspects of intelligence, are sorted into primary or secondary effects. Correlations between these areas of human development and music training lack support pertaining to the underlying cognitive networks that these processes rely on. Thus, this study was based on the work of Baddeley and Hitch's model of working memory, and implemented a test of parallel processing (Articulatory Suppression Task, AST), which measures proficiency of working memory systems. Individual differences therein, were compared with music aptitude. Participants were gathered throughout urban and rural regions of the state of Oregon. Half the participants received specific training on how to excel on AST, the other half received no training. The training was based on research showing musicians to be more proficient in rhythm, the phonological loop, and mental imagery. Group AST pretest/posttest scores and the Drake Musical Aptitude test scores were analyzed using 2-tailed t test and regression models for within-group and between-group variation. No significant difference between musical aptitude and participant ability to increase proficiency with parallel processing was found, however, the results indicated that music training influences proficiency with parallel processing in general, and there were indicators that a ceiling effect may have confounded the pretest-posttest range in scores. This supports findings of previous research that musical training has beneficial influences on mathematics, socio-emotional awareness, motor skills, language, and general intelligence, highlighting that positive social change may result if music were a core class in K-12 education.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Ford, Lauren. "The use of experiential acceptance in psychotherapy with emerging adults." Thesis, Pepperdine University, 2015. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3731118.

Full text
Abstract:

Emerging adulthood is recognized as a growing developmental stage that varies within and across cultures. Existing research generally characterizes this period as one of identity exploration, instability, self-reflection, and optimism. For many in this cohort, life events that were once organized into a stable sequence such as entering the workforce, marriage, and having children are increasingly a highly individualized and somewhat unstructured trajectory. This lack of structure provides opportunities and potential challenges to those transitioning from adolescence to adulthood. To this end, experiential acceptance may be an important target skill for intervention in guiding emerging adults through this tumultuous period.

Experiential acceptance is multiply defined in the literature, but is generally understood to be a present-focused approach that encourages a willingness to engage with one’s moment-to-moment experience, nonjudgment of moment-to-moment experiencing, and nonattachment to thoughts or feelings. This focus may be useful for both therapists to use as an intervention tool in helping clients to form an integrated sense of self; a developmental task that is predictive of mental health in young adults. Despite the apparent fit between experiential acceptance and the emerging adult age range, no studies to date have explored experiential acceptance as an intervention with this population.

Accordingly, the purpose of the current study was to qualitatively explore how therapists facilitate experiential acceptance with emerging adult clients. A sample of 5 client-therapist pairs from community counseling centers was selected, and two videotaped therapy sessions for each participant pair were analyzed. Inductive content analysis was employed, using open coding and abstraction methodology to create a hierarchy of themes. Results indicated that experiential acceptance, overall, was rarely employed by trainee therapists in psychotherapy sessions with emerging adults. The one parent theme that emerged across participants was termed Increasing Awareness. Comments aimed at increasing flexibility in thinking were also observed, but not across participants. It is hoped that this study will provide foundational information on experiential acceptance use in psychotherapy with emerging adults, which could be used to promote more attention to skill and theory integration in clinical training and spur future research on experiential acceptance use in therapy-as-usual.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Mintzer, Maureen Ryan. "An Exploration of the Cognitive and Affective Components of an Empathy Assessment to Inform Intervention." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2015. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/322011.

Full text
Abstract:
School Psychology
Ph.D.
While empathy is widely understood as a multifaceted construct and an important component of prosocial behavior, its role is less certain with regard to aggressive and bullying behavior in schools. In an effort to further the bully-prevention-and-intervention initiative, the validity of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI)--a self-report assessment that aims to evaluate both cognitive and affective components of empathy--was examined to determine its potential effectiveness as a screener to inform intervention for school-age youth. It is hypothesized that the IRI may insufficiently assess students' true empathic capacity due to the social desirability bias inherent in self-report scales, particularly with a youth population. The present study uses a correlational design to separately examine the strengths of the relationships between individuals' self-reports of cognitive and affective empathy on the IRI and respective criterion measures, social cognitive processing tasks and physiological responses to emotion-eliciting stimuli. Reliability analyses were also conducted to determine whether the IRI measures cognitive and affective empathy as separate constructs with a school-age population. It was hypothesized that individuals' self-reports of cognitive empathy would be strongly, positively related to performance on social cognitive processing tasks, and that students' self-report of affective empathy would yield weaker correlations with physiological responses to emotion-eliciting stimuli due to the social desirability bias inherent to the scale. Children in grades three though eight (n= 37) participated in the current study. Youth were recruited from an afterschool program and a summer camp from a parochial elementary school in a city in Pennsylvania. Students were asked to complete the IRI self-report scale. Two social cognitive processing tasks from the NEPSY-II were administered as a criterion measure for self-report of cognitive empathy. Change in fingertip temperature was measured during the viewing of two video vignettes to observe physiological response to emotion-eliciting stimuli as a criterion measure for affective empathy. While some strong, positive correlations were observed between male students' responses within the cognitive and affective empathy subscales of the IRI and performance on criterion measures, no positive correlations were observed between female students' self-reports of empathy and performance on criterion measures. Reliability analyses yielded no sufficient distinction between self-reports of affective and cognitive empathy.
Temple University--Theses
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Rittenmyer, George John. "The relationship between early maladaptive schemas and job burnout among public school teachers." Scholarly Commons, 1997. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/2755.

Full text
Abstract:
Early Maladaptive Schemas, as defined by Young (1990), are relatively permanent psychological constructs which influence human information processing. They consist of self-perceptions, attitudes and beliefs which are primarily the result of early childhood experiences. These schemas produce faulty thinking about an individual's self and environment, producing cognitive distortions. They are also the source of dysfunctional behavior as well as a wide range of mental conditions including depression, phobias and anxiety. Burnout is a significant problem among today's teachers which adversely effects the operation of schools (Maslach & Jackson, 1986). It is the result of teachers having to solve complicated problems, on a day-to-day basis, in their classrooms. Solutions to these problems are not always easily reached. This leads to frustration among teachers, culminating in cynical attitudes toward work, hostility toward students, absenteeism and job-leaving. The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between Early Maladaptive Schemas and teacher burnout. The Schema Questionnaire and the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI) were administered to fifty teachers employed by the Los Banos Unified School District, a middle-sized, suburban school district in central California. Subjects were asked to complete both self-report scales and given reimbursement for doing so. Forty-seven sets of questionnaires were collected. It was found that two of the higher order factors on the Schema Questionnaire, Overconnection (OVRC) and Exaggerated Standards (EXST), correlated strongly with the Emotional Exhaustion (EE) scale of the MBI. Weaker, but still significant, correlations were found between OVRC and the other two MBI factors, Depersonalization (DP) and Personal Accomplishment (PA). These relationships were all in the expected directions. Additionally, correlational data analyses suggested differences in gender, age and years of teaching on some of the Schema Questionnaire and MBI factors. Subsequent statistical analysis of mean differences indicated women scored significantly higher on OVRC and EE than did men. Analysis of mean differences, however, did not substantiate significant differences in scores on the Schema Questionnaire factors and the three MBI factors according to age and years of teaching experience.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Williams, Nina Lynne. "Identification of giftedness in preschoolers: Are some environmental factors related to cognitive assessments?" Diss., The University of Arizona, 1999. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/284027.

Full text
Abstract:
This study has the objective of examining parental perceptions, some home environmental factors, and SES on a child's cognitive development. Two groups were used to collect data, a high SES group and a low SES group. Twenty-six preschool children, 13 females and 13 males (24 Caucasian, one African American, and one Asian) were referred for giftedness. Parents responded to two surveys, one questionnaire, and an interview. The children were administered two cognitive skills tests: (a) The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983), KABC and (b) the Qualitative Use of English and Spanish Tasks, QUEST, (Gonzalez, 1991). Qualitative analyses was used to: (a) describe parental perceptions about the meaning of giftedness; and (b) describe parental perceptions about the development of giftedness. A total of seven patterns were found. Quantitative analyses was used to: (a) explore any relationship between parental perceptions and their own child's cognitive abilities; (b) explore any relationship between some environmental factors and the child's cognitive abilities; and (c) explore any relationship between the parents socioeconomic status and their own child's cognitive abilities. Quantitative results suggest that some home environmental factors do influence a child's cognitive abilities. Quantitative results also suggest that the parents' SES influences the child's performance on cognitive skills tests.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Georgiou, Andrea M. "The effects of different levels of high fidelity simulation on teamwork in senior-level undergraduate aviation students." Thesis, Capella University, 2014. http://pqdtopen.proquest.com/#viewpdf?dispub=3619937.

Full text
Abstract:

Industries in which effective teamwork is critical for safe operations are quickly discovering the benefits of simulation-based training (SBT). Research has shown the aviation industry, military operations, and medical field utilize impressive simulations allowing people to refine their technical and nontechnical skills in a "no consequence" environment. Grounded in team cognitive and group dynamic research, researchers are focusing their efforts on how to effectively use simulation as a training tool. The focus of this study was to determine how the degree of simulation difficulty affects teamwork. With a unique high fidelity simulation lab, the participants completed 3 hour work shifts to a run a simulated regional airline. Teamwork was based on the following five variables: contributing to teamwork, interacting with the team, keeping the team on track, expecting quality, and having the necessary knowledge, skills, and abilities (KSA's). The experimental design consisted of three teams randomly assigned to either a minimal, moderate, or maximum level of difficulty. After completion of the simulation, the participants completed an online survey for evaluations of their performance. The findings suggest some components of teamwork are affected by the design of the simulation, while others are not as susceptible to its effects. It was discovered the level of difficulty significantly affected individual and group performance in their expectation of quality and having relevant knowledge, skills, and abilities (KSA's). As simulation difficulty only affected two out of the five teamwork variables, this leads to the conclusion that generally a team will perform based on their level of team cognition and efficient group behaviors, not necessarily based on the degree of difficulty presented during a simulation.

APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Markey, John Brian. "Efficacy of a cognitive-behavioral treatment approach in improving academic performance of low-achieving elementary-age children." W&M ScholarWorks, 1986. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539618539.

Full text
Abstract:
Low-achieving students tend to be "externals," lack mastery orientation, and often do not "take responsibility" for learning. Through the cognitive behavioral approach in this study, the goal is to help the low achiever become better at self-monitoring and encoding information.;Low-achieving students from two elementary schools were pre- and posttested using the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test (to measure reading achievement), Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory (to measure self-esteem), and the Children's Academic Intrinsic Motivation Inventory (to measure intrinsic motivation). The teachers completed, as both a pre and post measure, the Devereux Elementary School Behavior Rating Scale II to measure student classroom behavior. Thirty low-achieving students who also received remedial reading instruction in each school, were randomly assigned to a cognitive-behavioral, study skills, and control group. The treatment groups participated in an eleven-week program of either a cognitive-behavioral or study skills intervention. The cognitive-behavioral approach used Meichenbaum's self-instructional phases in the context of developing better reading comprehension, study and problem-solving skills in the classroom. The study skills group received training on developing better study habits, and the control group received no treatment. The treatment groups were led by school psychologists. The course of the treatments followed a detailed outline.;It was predicted that all three groups would show significant gains in reading. It was also hypothesized that the cognitive-behavioral group would show significantly higher posttest score gains in achievement, student classroom behavior, intrinsic motivation, and self-esteem than the study skills or control groups. In addition, it was predicted that the study skills group would show significantly higher posttest score gains in academic achievement than the control group.;The MANOVA design was used to compare all groups to determine how they differed on each dependent variable. All groups, when analyzed together, showed a significant F ratio regarding gains in reading. There were no significant differences between any of the groups. It was thus concluded that no individual treatment was more effective than the other.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Hall, David Alvin. "The effectiveness of cognitive restructuring and paradoxical directives counseling interventions of adolescent self-esteem : a comparative study." W&M ScholarWorks, 1986. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539618763.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigated the relative merits of the paradoxical directives (PD) counseling techniques to the more didactic cognitive restructuring (CR) intervention. It was hypothesized that subjects in the PD groups would show an improvement in self-esteem, a reduction in depression and anxiety, and a change reflective of a more internal locus-of-control orientation to a degree significantly greater than the CR and the control groups.;The sample consisted of sixty eighth grade volunteers, twenty from each of three middle schools in a local area school district. Six groups were formed of ten subjects each, two groups for each treatment and two groups receiving no treatment. Participants were randomly assigned by treatment and counselor within each school. Eight group meetings were held, two a week for four consecutive weeks.;The research design used was the pretest/posttest control group design. Instruments used were the Coopersmith Self Esteem Inventory, the Children's Depression Inventory, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (trait anxiety portion only), and the Nowicki-Strickland Locus-of-Control Scale for Children. An analysis of covariance was used to discern differences in posttest group means by treatment taking into consideration pre-treatment group differences. Statistical analysis resulted in the acceptance of null on hypotheses 1, 3, and 4 as reviewed in Chapter 4 and below. The null hypothesis was rejected at the .95 level of confidence for the second hypothesis. Posttest treatment means did not significantly differ with regard to self-esteem, anxiety, and locus-of-control. On the other hand, posttest treatment group means differed significantly at the 0.05 level on the measure of depression.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Hemmy, Laura Sue. "Educational attainment and rate of cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease." [College Station, Tex. : Texas A&M University, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/1969.1/ETD-TAMU-1189.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Messenger, Carla Lynn. "Anxiety Sensitivity and Panic among College Students: Cognition, Emotion, and Somatic Symptoms." W&M ScholarWorks, 1997. https://scholarworks.wm.edu/etd/1539626146.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography