Academic literature on the topic 'Émotions chez le nourrisson'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Émotions chez le nourrisson.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Émotions chez le nourrisson"
Léveillé, Élène, Louise Cossette, Martine Gaudreau, and Nancy Côté. "La socialisation des émotions chez le nourrisson: Le rôle du langage maternel." Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement 33, no. 4 (2001): 233–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087145.
Full textLéveillé, Élène, Louise Cossette, Isabelle Blanchette, and Martine Gaudreau. "La socialisation des émotions chez le nourrisson: le rôle des expressions faciales contingentes des mères." International Journal of Psychology 36, no. 4 (August 2001): 260–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590143000027.
Full textDupuy, R. P., and A. Jacquot. "Quels enjeux pour l’examen pédiatrique du nouveau-né en maternité ? « L’examen du nouveau-né revisité »." Périnatalité 13, no. 2 (June 2021): 63–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.3166/rmp-2021-0121.
Full textNeault, Isabelle, Louise Cossette, Kim Houle, and Jean Bégin. "Le développement de la régulation des émotions chez des nourrissons de mères adolescents." Enfance 2015, no. 02 (June 2015): 179–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.4074/s0013754515002025.
Full textKhemakhem, R., T. Chouikh, H. Rahay, S. Ghorbel, F. Nouira, F. Cennoufi, H. Louati, S. Jlidi, I. Bellagha, and B. Chaouachi. "Ictère chez un nourrisson." Feuillets de Radiologie 49, no. 4 (September 2009): 302–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0181-9801(09)72383-8.
Full textMaruani, A., F. Labarthe, T. Dupré, E. Le Bidre, G. Georgesco, and G. Lorette. "Hypercaroténémie chez un nourrisson." Annales de Dermatologie et de Vénéréologie 137, no. 1 (January 2010): 32–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annder.2009.10.177.
Full textAtmani, S., M. Elouardi, A. Bouharrou, and M. Hida. "Zona chez un nourrisson." Archives de Pédiatrie 14, no. 9 (September 2007): 1092–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arcped.2007.06.016.
Full textMalajati, H., and R. Dafiri. "Coma chez un nourrisson." Feuillets de Radiologie 50, no. 6 (December 2010): 350–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.frad.2010.09.001.
Full textMalajati, H., and R. Dafiri. "Coma chez un nourrisson." Feuillets de Radiologie 50, no. 6 (December 2010): 370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.frad.2010.09.002.
Full textMarrekchi, F., and A. Mzid. "Diversification alimentaire chez le nourrisson." Archives de Pédiatrie 6 (January 1999): S555. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0929-693x(99)81695-9.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Émotions chez le nourrisson"
Abramson, Irit. "L' émotion et le symptôme somatique chez l'enfant." Paris 13, 2008. http://www.theses.fr/2008PA131037.
Full textFreud brought to light the psychic causality of hysterical somatic symptoms. But, what is the somatic symptom in early infancy ? Can we define it as an expression of psychic defense or as a substitute physiological defense for an impossible psychic approach of the environment ? The psychodynamic orientation given by the psychoanalytic concept of repression is an essential contribution to the problem of resistance through-out the treatment. One question remains concerning the persistance of non organic fonctional disorders in children when their perception is not yet co-ordinated with a mental représentation but is a condition of psychic development as well as an activation of signal of a biological rythm disorder. This point justifies the investigation of the relation between selective fonctional disorders and relational experiences where the environment reactions amplify the intensity of the baby’s emotional tensions, reinforces the relief of these tensions into muscular activity and finally increases reflex organic mechanisms against pain. Among the factors that provoque lasting fonctional disorders, we can take into consideration the repeated miss-matching of the baby’s physiological need of tension regulation with his mother’s emotional reactions. We can also mention the obstruction effect of non matching interaction to the transition between perceptive experiences and the primary psychic defense that is memory
Lafine, Florence. "Corps, affectivité et jeu de langage : du sensoriel au sens social, l'ontogènese possible de l'habitus et des représentations sociales chez le Bébé." Paris, EHESS, 2010. http://www.theses.fr/2010EHES0166.
Full textThe aim of our project is to make a focus on the concept of habitus (Bourdieu) through the analysis of interactions between the baby and his environment. Our conceptual groundwork consists of three parts: The first one starts with the learning and acquisition of a language game (Wittgenstein) during the intra-uterine life. Throughout the five sense organs, the baby experiences various sensory lines performed in specific grammatical meanings. Based both on customary experiences and strange events, a perceptive sensory system with a proximal closeness is invented, included a social dimension. Thanks to it, the baby can ascribe an intelligibility to his native surrounding conditions, and he can afford to communicate and convey emotions. The second one relates how soon after birth the infant can afford to a more complex system, including a distal dimension. Thanks to the results of PILE, 2004-2008 (a research program on language, based on a qualitative database from video sequencies of babies in interaction with their mother), we can see how specifical conditions or troubles make different the developpement and the efficience of the language game. So that, the baby is differently introduced to self-discovery and to his social environment. The last part tries to describe and study the way of transmission of social meanings on which habitus and language forming are based. This transmission is only possible under the specific charge of affectivity in characteristic activities running social reference patterns. Language game, including a spatio-temporal dimension, will be the always active part of the processes involved with primary habitus and in the infant acquiring language
Bussières, Ève-Line. "Stress maternel prénatal et développement précoce : données de naissance, attention et sécrétion cortisolaire à trois mois : association entre le stress maternel prénatal, l'âge gestationnel et le poids de naissance du bébé : une méta-analyse d'études prospectives : association entre le stress maternel prénatal, l'attention/éveil et la sécrétion cortisolaire de l'enfant à trois mois." Thesis, Université Laval, 2012. http://www.theses.ulaval.ca/2012/29070/29070.pdf.
Full textTremblay, Elsa. "La contribution des émotions de l'enfant et de la sensibilité maternelle dans la relation d'attachement." Master's thesis, Université Laval, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/68418.
Full textPouliot, Sandra. "Structure de la réaction comportementale face à la nouveauté chez les enfants âgés de 18 mois." Master's thesis, Université Laval, 2002. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/46532.
Full textBayet, Laurie. "Le développement de la perception des expressions faciales." Thesis, Université Grenoble Alpes (ComUE), 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015GREAS049/document.
Full textThis thesis addressed the question of how the perception of emotional facial expressions develops, reframing it in the theoretical framework of face perception: the separation of variant (expression, gaze) and invariant (gender, race) streams, the role of experience, and social attention. More specifically, we investigated how in infants and children the perception of angry, smiling, or fearful facial expressions interacts with gender perception (Studies 1-2), gaze perception (Study 3), and face detection (Study 4).In a first study, we found that adults and 5-12 year-old children tend to categorize angry faces as male (Study 1). Comparing human performance with that of several automatic classifiers suggested that this reflects a strategy of using specific features and second-order relationships in the face to categorize gender. The bias was constant over all ages studied and extended to other-race faces, further suggesting that it doesn't require extensive experience. A second set of studies examined whether, in infants, the perception of smiling depends on experience-sensitive, invariant dimensions of the face such as gender and race (Study 2). Indeed, infants are typically most familiar with own-race female faces. The visual preference of 3.5 month-old infants for open-mouth, own-race smiling (versus neutral) faces was restricted to female faces and reversed in male faces. The effect did not replicate with own- or other-race closed-mouth smiles. We attempted to extend these results to an object-referencing task in 3.5-, 9- and 12-month-olds (Study 3). Objects previously referenced by smiling faces attracted similar attention as objects previously cued by neutral faces, regardless of age group and face gender, and despite differences in gaze following. Finally, we used univariate (face side preference) and multivariate (face versus noise side decoding evidence) trial-level measures of face detection, coupled with non-linear mixed modeling of psychometric curves, to reveal the detection advantage of fearful faces (compared to smiling faces) embedded in phase-scrambled noise in 3.5-, 6-, and 12-month-old infants (Study 4). The advantage was as or more evident in the youngest group than in the two older age groups.Taken together, these results provide insights into the early ontogeny and underlying cause of gender-emotion relationships in face perception and the sensitivity to fear
Bat-Pitault, Flora. "Marqueurs sommeil et émotionnels du risque de dépression chez les mères et leurs enfants." Thesis, Aix-Marseille, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015AIXM5063/document.
Full textThe main objective of this work was to search for markers in sleep and emotions level of risk of developing major depression in mothers and their children known as "at risk”. After a clinical description of close links between sleep, depression, emotion and cognition in mothers during pregnancy and the postpartum period and in children and adolescents, we conducted a retrospective study of the first children and adolescents sleep mothers with a personal history of depression. This study highlighted the macro-level architectural features related to depressive identifiable risk adolescents at risk. We then conducted a broader prospective study which involved 302 mother-child dyads followed from birth to 36 months of the child. Initially, our goal was to describe in mothers sleep abnormalities during pregnancy can indicate a risk of postpartum depression and more broadly to induce a number of consequences on the development of the child. Secondly this large cohort allowed us to link early alterations of child sleep with cognitive and emotional peculiarities to 36 months. We have also been able to describe in children 6 months to risk of depression, macro and micro-architectural deterioration of sleep may constitute a subsequent psychopathology risk factor via impaired neuroplasticity early in development; and in these same children 36 months through a negative emotional recognition constitutes a subsequent psychopathology vulnerability factor particularly depressed. The monitoring of this longer-term cohort remains crucial to observe children at risk among those who develop other sleep or emotional anomalies and ultimately a major depressive episode
Ballesta, Sébastien. "Émotions et décisions sociales chez le macaque." Thesis, Lyon 1, 2014. http://www.theses.fr/2014LYO10321/document.
Full textDo macaques are able to take into account others' welfare during social decisions-making ? If so, what capacities allow them to apprehend it and which variables would influence their decisions in such social contexts ? From perception to the production of social behavior, a multitude of complex mental processes allow macaques to live in a dynamic society, structured and cohesive. Although the presence of pro-and antisocial behavior had already been described, their ultimate and proximate causes are not known. Does empathy, defined here as the ability to feel the emotions of others, could explain the expression of pro-social behavior? The answers to such questions might have important implications for science, but also in philosophy and ethics. The originality and relevance of our experimental approach lies in the objective measurement of social behavior using innovative devices and protocols developed and validated during this thesis. Indeed, we aimed to reproduce a social context in a controlled laboratory environment. Thus, by placing two animals face to face, we have developed a unique protocol for social decision allowing a macaque to delivered either an aversive stimulus (an air puff on the face), an appetitive stimulus (drop of fruit juice) to its partner, to an empty space or to itself. The choices are organized in pairs, the actor had to select either the prosocial option or antisocial option or, for non-social controls decision the rational or irrational option. Appropriate analyzes of eye tracking measures (level of mutual gaze and blink frequency) allowed us to establish relationships between emotional responses and pro (or anti) social trends. It revealed processes dependent on past experiences and relationships with the partner, thus homologous to those involved in human emotional empathy
Marineau-Bonnet, Anne-Sophie. "Apport de vitamine D chez le nourrisson." Bordeaux 2, 1999. http://www.theses.fr/1999BOR2P035.
Full textPORRETTI, DOMINIQUE, and SWYSEN CATHERINE PORRETTI. "Premiers soins chez le nourrisson : these filmee." Lille 2, 1989. http://www.theses.fr/1989LIL2M306.
Full textBooks on the topic "Émotions chez le nourrisson"
1896-1934, Vygotskii L. S., ed. Conscience, inconscient, émotions. Paris: Dispute, 2003.
Find full textFirst Feelings: Milestones in the Emotional Development of Your Baby and Child. New York, NY: Penguin Books, 1989.
Find full textThorndike, Greenspan Nancy, ed. First feelings: Milestones in the emotional development of your baby and child. New York, N.Y., U.S.A: Viking, 1985.
Find full textThorndike, Greenspan Nancy, ed. First feelings: Milestones in the emotional development of your baby and child. New York, N.Y., U.S.A: Penguin, 1986.
Find full textVauclair, Jacques. Développement du jeune enfant: Motricité, perception, cognition. Paris: Belin, 2004.
Find full textPicard, Chantal Suzanne. La reconnaissance des émotions chez les enfants d'âge scolaire. Sudbury, Ont: Département de psychologie, Université Laurentienne, 1996.
Find full textMoro, Christiane. L'objet et la construction de son usage chez le bebe: Une approche semiotique du developpement preverbal. Bern: Lang, 2005.
Find full textLes caprices: Comprendre et gérer les émotions de votre enfant. Courbevoie: Soline éd., 2004.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Émotions chez le nourrisson"
Javouhey, E., and R. Pouyau. "Convulsions chez le nourrisson et chez l’enfant." In Réanimation et urgences, 331–34. Paris: Springer Paris, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-2-287-99129-5_18.
Full textLe Coz, P. "Émotions et principes éthiques en néonatologie." In Soins palliatifs chez le nouveau-né, 23–37. Paris: Springer Paris, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-2-8178-0136-0_4.
Full textJavouhey, E., and R. Pouyau. "Diarrhée aiguë et déshydratation chez le nourrisson et l’enfant." In Réanimation et urgences, 335–38. Paris: Springer Paris, 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-2-287-99129-5_19.
Full textLanfranchi, Pierluigi. "L’usage des émotions dans la polémique anti-juive. L’exemple des discours contre les Juifs de Jean Chrysostome." In Judaïsme et christianisme chez les Pères, 237–52. Turnhout: Brepols Publishers, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1484/m.jaoc-eb.5.110707.
Full textJenik, A. G., and N. E. Vain. "Le recours à la tétine chez le nouveau-né et le nourrisson : Quel retentissement ?" In 41es Journées nationales de la Société Française de Médecine Périnatale (Grenoble 12–14 octobre 2011), 251–57. Paris: Springer Paris, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-2-8178-0257-2_25.
Full text"Chez le même éditeur." In La Psychothérapie Centrée sur les émotions, II. Elsevier, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-2-294-76947-4.00010-x.
Full text"Chez le même éditeur." In La psychothérapie centrée sur les émotions, IV. Elsevier, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-2-294-74146-3.00011-1.
Full text"Chez le même éditeur." In Le Développement du Nourrisson de la Naissance à 1 an, II. Elsevier, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-2-294-76484-4.00025-1.
Full textSédat, Jacques. "Satisfaction, plaisir, jouissance chez le nourrisson et l’enfant." In Vous avez dit jouissance ?, 19–35. Érès, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/eres.guily.2019.01.0019.
Full textDelorme, Michèle. "Chez une assistante maternelle." In Professionnels de la petite enfance : au risque des émotions, 147. ERES, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/eres.nain.2009.01.0147.
Full textConference papers on the topic "Émotions chez le nourrisson"
Abraham, Marine. "Jeunesse et plage : approche sociolinguistique des publicités contemporaines." In XXV Coloquio AFUE. Palabras e imaginarios del agua. Valencia: Universitat Politècnica València, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.4995/xxvcoloquioafue.2016.3171.
Full text