Academic literature on the topic 'English word stress'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'English word stress.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "English word stress"
Ladd, D. Robert, and Erik Fudge. "English Word-Stress." Language 62, no. 1 (March 1986): 170. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/415608.
Full textKreidler, Charles W., Ivan Poldauf, and W. R. Lee. "English Word Stress: A Theory of Word-Stress Patterns in English." Language 63, no. 1 (March 1987): 155. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/415394.
Full textATOYE, RAPHAEL O. "Word stress in Nigerian English." World Englishes 10, no. 1 (March 1991): 1–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-971x.1991.tb00132.x.
Full textTaylor, David. "Compound word stress." ELT Journal 45, no. 1 (January 1, 1991): 67–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/elt/45.1.67.
Full textTaylor, David S. "Demystifying word stress." English Today 12, no. 4 (October 1996): 46–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0266078400009305.
Full textBell, Melanie J., and Ingo Plag. "Informativity and analogy in English compound stress." Word Structure 6, no. 2 (October 2013): 129–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/word.2013.0042.
Full textIngram, John. "Vietnamese Acquisition of English Word Stress." TESOL Quarterly 39, no. 2 (June 1, 2005): 309. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3588314.
Full textLadd, D. Robert. "English word-stress By Erik Fudge." Language 62, no. 1 (1986): 170–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.1986.0025.
Full textZhou, Qian, Yonghong Li, and Lei Guo. "An Acoustic Study of English Word Stress of Amdo English Learners." Theory and Practice in Language Studies 10, no. 1 (December 24, 2019): 117. http://dx.doi.org/10.17507/tpls.1001.17.
Full textSHEVCHENKO, TATIANA. "ENGLISH WORD STRESS IN LONG-TERM LANGUAGE CONTACT." Theoretical and Applied Linguistics, no. 2 (2021): 160–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.22250/2410-7190_2021_7_2_160_168.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "English word stress"
Okobi, Anthony O. (Anthony Obiesie) 1976. "Acoustic correlates of word stress in American English." Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/37963.
Full textIncludes bibliographical references (p. 123-126).
Acoustic parameters that differentiate between primary stress and non-primary full vowels were determined using two-syllable real and novel words and specially constructed novel words with identical syllable compositions. The location of the high focal pitch accent within a declarative carrier phrase was varied using an innovative object naming task that allowed for a natural and spontaneous manipulation of phrase-level accentuation. Results from male native speakers of American English show that when the high focal pitch accent was on the novel word, vowel differences in pitch, intensity prominence, and amplitude of the first harmonic, H1 * (corrected for the effect of the vocal tract filter), accurately distinguished full vowel syllables carrying primary stress vs. non-primary stress. Acoustic parameters that correlated to word stress under all conditions tested were syllable duration, HI*-A3*, as a measurement of spectral tilt, and noise at high frequencies, determined by band-pass filtering the F3 region of the spectrum. Furthermore, the results indicate that word stress cues are augmented when the high focal pitch accent is on the target word.
(cont.) This became apparent after a formula was devised to correct for the masking effect of phrase-level accentuation on the spectral tilt measurement, Hi *-A3*. Perceptual experiments also show that male native speakers of American English utilized differences in syllable duration and spectral tilt, as controlled by the KLSYN88 parameters DU and TL, to assign prominence status to the syllables of a novel word embedded in a carrier phrase. Results from this study suggest that some correlates to word stress are produced in the laryngeal region and are due to vocal fold configuration. The model of word stress that emerges from this study has aspects that differ from other widely accepted models of prosody at the word level. The model can also be applied to improve the prosody of synthesized speech, as well as to improve machine recognition of speech.
by Anthony O. Okobi.
Ph.D.
Chan, Ka-wai Ricky, and 陳嘉威. "Implicit learning of L2 word stress rules." Thesis, The University of Hong Kong (Pokfulam, Hong Kong), 2012. http://hub.hku.hk/bib/B4961793X.
Full textpublished_or_final_version
English
Master
Master of Philosophy
Ruiz, García María Isabel. "Word Stress Patterns in the English of Spanish Speakers: A Perceptual Analysis." Doctoral thesis, Universidad de Alicante, 2018. http://hdl.handle.net/10045/114906.
Full textOu, Shu-chen. "Factors and mechanisms in L2 word stress acquisition : evidence from Chinese-English interlanguage." Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/25047.
Full textRibeiro, Thais Sada. "Acoustic correlates of word stress production in the connected speech of american english and brazilian portuguese speakers." Florianópolis, SC, 2006. http://repositorio.ufsc.br/xmlui/handle/123456789/89540.
Full textMade available in DSpace on 2012-10-22T22:48:42Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 241386.pdf: 235440 bytes, checksum: 8e0526a24435203c9e6a01d3971d1684 (MD5)
Este estudo teve como objetivo investigar os correlatos acústicos do acento tônico na fala conectada de três brasileiras e três americanas. Após os sujeitos gravarem um texto informativo, dois falantes bilíngües delimitaram os acentos tônicos produzidos. Vogais tônicas, pré-tônicas e pós-tônicas foram então medidas quanto a sua duração, pico de F0 e pico de intensidade. Unidades acústicas foram a seguir transformadas em unidades perceptuais, a fim de que diferenças estatísticas significativas pudessem ser classificadas como perceptíveis ou imperceptíveis. Resultados significativos e perceptíveis foram classificados como ocasionais, recorrentes ou consistentes, conforme sua coincidência com os resultados de um, dois ou três falantes por grupo. Apenas resultados consistentes e recorrentes foram considerados como resultados representativos de cada grupo. De acordo com estes critérios, a duração foi o único correlato acústico significativo e perceptível do acento tônico para ambos os grupos de falantes. Enquanto as vogais tônicas se mostraram mais longas do que as pré-tônicas para o grupo de americanas, para o grupo de brasileiras as vogais tônicas se mostraram mais longas do que as pós-tônicas. Nenhuma diferença significativa e perceptível entre os dois grupos foi encontrada quanto à duração das vogais tônicas, pré-tônicas ou pós-tônicas produzidas.
Shin, Jeonghwa. "Prosodic Effects on Spoken Word Recognition in Second Language:Processing of Lexical Stress by Korean-speaking Learners of English." The Ohio State University, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=osu1338388308.
Full textLepage, Andrée. "The contribution of word stress and vowel reduction to the intelligibility of the speech of Canadian French second language learners of English." Doctoral thesis, Université Laval, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/26124.
Full textThis thesis studies the perception of French accented English by native English speakers. It presents the results of a mixed methods study aimed at exploring (1) whether both incorrect word stress and incorrect vowel reduction have an impact on intelligibility, (2) if so, whether they interfere equally, (3) whether the omission of vowel reduction has a greater or lesser impact on intelligibility than misplacement, (4) whether rightward misplacement of word stress has a greater impact on intelligibility than leftward misplacement, and (5) whether L2 speech that typically misproduces word stress and vowel reduction is more intelligible to listeners who are familiar with the accent. Sixty native Canadian English listeners performed a close-shadowing task whereby they evaluated 80 Canadian French (CF) accented two-, three- and four-syllable words categorized according to the naturally occurring prosodic errors they contain. Thirty native English judges had had little or no exposure to CF L2 speech (i.e., are non-accent-tolerant) and 30 use, or are in daily contact with French and/or French-accented English (i.e., are accent-tolerant). The judges’ responses were analysed qualitatively to determine which prosodic error contribute to loss of intelligibility, and quantitatively to determine which errors slow word identification. Results show that both incorrect stress and vowel reduction interfere with an L2 speaker’s intelligibility (Research Question 1) but they do so unequally. Incorrect vowel reduction is more detrimental (Research Question 2). Results also show that omitting vowel reduction is less detrimental to intelligibility than misplacing it (Research Question 3). As for stress, intelligibility is more impaired by rightward than leftward misplacement (Research Question 4). As for accent familiarity, the accent tolerant group accurately identified more words than the non-accent-tolerant group, suggesting that exposure to a particular L2 does benefit a listener (Research Question 5), though the response times for both listener groups were statistically similar. A possible explanation for these mixed results is that the accent-tolerant listeners, because they speak French, activate more lexical candidates during lexical access, thus slowing down their reaction times.
Kebboua, Chaker Nadia. "Integrating Information Technology in theTeaching/Learning of English Pronunciation in the Classroom: Designing and Implementing an Online Course to Teach Word and Sentence Stress to Tertiary Level Students." Doctoral thesis, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, 2019. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/668789.
Full textBanzina, Elina. "The Role of Secondary-stressed and Unstressed-unreduced Syllables in Word Recognition: Acoustic and Perceptual Studies with Russian Learners of English." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1340114580.
Full textSilveira, Amanda Post da. "ESTRATÉGIAS DE REPARO NA ATRIBUIÇÃO DO ACENTO PRIMÁRIO DO INGLÊS POR FALANTES NATIVOS DE PB." Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, 2010. http://repositorio.ufsm.br/handle/1/9826.
Full textIn this study, we investigated the repair strategies applied to English word primary stress in the process of acquisition by native speakers of Brazilian Portuguese (BP). For this purpose, we used Connectionist Optimality Theory (henceforth, COT) by Bonilha (2004) as its theoretical basis. COT, beyond previewing constraint interaction, the interaction of different phonological levels (such as segment, syllable and stress) and constraints reranking in an L2 acquisition process, it also claims that constraints which are specific from the L2 may be acquired. Such restructuring process may be explained by an implemented gradual learning algorithm (BOERSMA & HAYES 2001) which constitutes a great advantage to acquisition data analysis. In the present study, we analyzed English stress acquisition in suffixed words, especially, the production of words whose suffixes carry the primary stress. In this last group, we observed the following suffixes: -oon, -eer, -ee, -ette, -esque, - ese, -ique, -et, -aire, -euse and -eur. Taking into account the process of constraint reranking, we intended to characterize interlanguage hierarchy evidenced by learners from the repair strategies they applied to deviant productions. For this aim, we used oral language data from sixteen informants, academics of Languages - English major from a Brazilian Southern university. Data collection consisted of two recordings of an instrument which contained 135 suffixed and non-suffixed words and 135 carrier-sentences. Afterwards, data were transcribed by auditory method and reviewed by two evaluators. The transcriptions were based on IPA. Data were statistically treated by Chi-Square test via Statistica 7.0 software. We could observe that, among the suffixes that present different behaviors concerning the stress of the primitive words, the ones which receive the primary stress were the least correctly produced, presenting around 50% of errors. Data also confirmed the trends indicated by studies on L2 acquisition that interlanguage systems show the militancy of L1 hierarchy of constraints. This is due to the predominance of the trochaic pattern as a repair strategy - around 50% of deviant productions, and, as argued by Bisol (1992), it is the stress pattern of BP. Concerning the suffixes that carry the primary stress, it seems that L2 vowel length, that may be the main element of stress attribution in words such as refugee and mountaineer, is not easily perceived/produced by BP native speakers, according to the findings by Nobre-Oliveira (2007). Taking into account the results, we understand that some specific constraints to vowel length were not ranked in learners interlanguage hierarchy yet. Also, we relate the phenomenon of the random production of such stress pattern to its low frequency in English lexicon. Thus, frequency seems to be one important factor for learners oral productions. In sum, we believe that such factors as a whole may be conspiring to the fact that informants productions are still deviant from the pattern of the target language. Finally, we argue that COT seems to be an important tool in order to describe and analyze L2 language acquisition data.
Neste estudo, investigamos as estratégias de reparo aplicadas ao padrão de acento primário do Inglês, no processo de aquisição, por falantes nativos de português brasileiro (PB). Para esse propósito, utilizamos a Teoria da Otimidade Conexionista (COT), a partir dos estudos de Bonilha (2004) como sua base teórica. A COT, além de prever a interação de restrições, a interação de diferentes níveis fonológicos (como segmental, silábico e acentual) e o reranqueamento no processo de aquisição de uma L2, também propõe que restrições específicas da L2 devam ser adquiridas. No presente estudo, analisamos a aquisição do acento do inglês em palavras sufixadas, especialmente, na produção de palavras cujo sufixo carrega o acento, como -oon, -eer, -ee, -ette, -esque, -ese, -ique, -et, - aire, -euse e -eur. Levando-se em consideração o processo de reranqueamento, intentamos caracterizar a hierarquia de interlíngua evidenciada pelas produções desviantes dos aprendizes a tal padrão. Com este objetivo, usamos dados de linguagem oral de dezesseis sujeitos, acadêmicos de Letras - habilitação Língua Inglesa de uma universidade do Sul do Brasil. Os dados consistiram de duas coletas, realizadas por meio de um instrumento que contém 135 palavras (entre sufixadas e nãosufixadas) e 135 frases-veículo. Em seguida, houve a transcrição fonética pelo método de oitiva e a revisão por dois avaliadores. As transcrições tiveram por base o Alfabeto Fonético Internacional (IPA). Os dados foram tratados estatisticamente com o teste Qui-quadrado via programa Statistica 7.0. Pudemos observar que, dentre os sufixos que apresentam comportamentos distintos, no que concerne ao acento da palavra primitiva, os sufixos que atraem o acento primário foram os que apresentaram o menor percentual de acertos, aproximadamente 50%. Os dados também confirmam as tendências, apontadas por outros estudos em aquisição de L2, de que os sistemas de interlíngua apresentam a militância da hierarquia de restrições da L1. Evidenciamos isso pelo predomínio do padrão acentual trocaico como estratégia de reparo, que responde por cerca de 50% dos tokens desviantes e, como argumenta Bisol (1992), o padrão trocaico é o predominante do PB. Com relação aos sufixos que atraem o acento primário, há evidências de que o prolongamento da vogal, que é o principal elemento considerado na atribuição de peso da sílaba e na atração do acento em palavras como refugee e mountaineer, não está sendo adquirido pelos falantes nativos de PB, conforme já aponta o estudo de Nobre-Oliveira (2007). Visto os dados de que dispomos nesta pesquisa, compreendemos que algumas restrições ligadas ao prolongamento e fidelidade da vogal acentuada, bem como algumas restrições de marcação, precisam ainda ser ranqueadas na hierarquia de interlíngua dos aprendizes. Também, relacionamos o fenômeno de atribuição flutuante do padrão oxítono sufixado a sua baixa frequência no léxico do Inglês. Assim, observamos que a frequência também parece desempenhar um importante papel nas produções orais dos sujeitos. Em suma, acreditamos que tais fatores devem conspirar para a emergência de produções desviantes ao padrão da língua alvo. Por fim, com este estudo, pudemos comprovar a validade dos presupostos da COT e tomá-la como um modelo linguístico pertinente à descrição e análise de dados de aquisição de L2.
Books on the topic "English word stress"
Speyer, Augustin. Topicalization and stress clash avoidance in the history of English. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 2010.
Find full textSpeyer, Augustin. Topicalization and stress clash avoidance in the history of English. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, 2010.
Find full textTopicalization and stress clash avoidance in the history of English. De Gruyter Mouton: Berlin ; New York, 2010.
Find full textThe thematic structure of the sentence in English and Polish: Sentence stress and word order. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, 2011.
Find full textWenszky, Nóra. Secondary stress in English words. Budapest: Akadémiai Kiadó, 2004.
Find full textill, Christman Annaliese, and Olsen Lillian translator, eds. World trigger. San Francisco, CA: Viz Media, 2015.
Find full texttranslator, Tanaka Yamato, Blackman Abigail, and Starr Paul Tuttle editor, eds. Cells at work! New York: Kodansha America, Incorporated, 2016.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "English word stress"
Smakman, Dick. "Word stress." In Clear English Pronunciation, 43–46. New York : Taylor and Francis, 2020.: Routledge, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780429347382-9.
Full textBeňuš, Štefan. "Word Stress." In Investigating Spoken English, 155–73. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-54349-5_9.
Full textTurcsan, Gabor, and Sophie Herment. "Making Sense of Nonce Word Stress in English." In Investigating English Pronunciation, 23–46. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/9781137509437_2.
Full textPhillips, Betty S. "Word frequency and lexical diffusion in English stress shifts." In Historical Linguistics 1995, 223. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/cilt.162.17phi.
Full textWaniek-Klimczak, Ewa. "Factors Affecting Word Stress Recognition by Advanced Polish Learners of English." In Second Language Learning and Teaching, 189–204. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-11092-9_11.
Full textBoswell, Eileen. "“Revenge of the Schwa”: A Metaphor for Teaching English Word Stress in Academic Vocabulary." In Asian Research on English for Specific Purposes, 69–83. Singapore: Springer Singapore, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-1037-3_5.
Full textShevchenko, Tatiana, and Daria Pozdeeva. "Canadian English Word Stress: A Corpora-Based Study of National Identity in a Multilingual Community." In Speech and Computer, 221–32. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-66429-3_21.
Full textRojczyk, Arkadiusz. "Vowel Quality and Duration as a Cue to Word Stress for Non-native Listeners: Polish Listeners’ Perception of Stress in English." In Second Language Learning and Teaching, 59–71. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24019-5_5.
Full textPozdeeva, Daria, Tatiana Shevchenko, and Alexey Abyzov. "New Perspectives on Canadian English Digital Identity Based on Word Stress Patterns in Lexicon and Spoken Corpus." In Speech and Computer, 401–13. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26061-3_41.
Full textJarmulowicz, Linda. "Chapter 8. Stress Production in Derived English Words as a Developmental Window." In Linguistic Rhythm and Literacy, 163–86. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/tilar.17.08jar.
Full textConference papers on the topic "English word stress"
Chang, Wei, Wei-Chieh Fang, Yu-Lun Lin, and Nian-Shing Chen. "Gesture-Facilitated Learning of English Word Stress Patterns." In 2014 International Conference of Educational Innovation through Technology (EITT). IEEE, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/eitt.2014.14.
Full textIshikawa, Keiichi, and Jun Nomura. "Word stress placement by native speakers and Japanese learners of English." In Interspeech 2008. ISCA: ISCA, 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2008-516.
Full textEriksson, Anders, and Mattias Heldner. "The acoustics of word stress in English as a function of stress level and speaking style." In Interspeech 2015. ISCA: ISCA, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2015-9.
Full textPate, John K., and Mark Johnson. "Syllable weight encodes mostly the same information for English word segmentation as dictionary stress." In Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP). Stroudsburg, PA, USA: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/v1/d14-1091.
Full textGuo, Xing-Rong, Xiao-Xiang Chen, and Yi-Ming Guo. "Prosodic Transfer in the Beginning-and Advanced-Level Chinese Learners' Production of English Word Stress." In 2017 2nd International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2017). Paris, France: Atlantis Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.2991/hss-17.2017.4.
Full textTseng, Chiu Yu, and Chao Yu Su. "Binary Contrast and Categorical Differentiation Prosodic Characteristics of English Word Stress in Broad and Narrow Focus Positions." In 7th International Conference on Speech Prosody 2014. ISCA: ISCA, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/speechprosody.2014-30.
Full textZhang, Yonghuai, and Qi Wang. "Design and development of an objective English accent recognition system based on RankNet's word stress recognition method." In CIPAE 2020: 2020 International Conference on Computers, Information Processing and Advanced Education. New York, NY, USA: ACM, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3419635.3419728.
Full textSze Lai, Wience Wing, and Manwa Lawrence Ng. "Different acoustic cues for emphasis in teaching English word stress to Hong Kong Cantonese ESL learners of different proficiencies." In 7th Tutorial and Research Workshop on Experimental Linguistics. ExLing Society, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.36505/exling-2016/07/0019/000278.
Full textMartin, Philippe. "Automatic detection of accent phrases in French." In 11th International Conference of Experimental Linguistics. ExLing Society, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.36505/exling-2020/11/0030/000445.
Full textKebboua, Nadia, and Joaquín Romero. "Integrating information technology in the teaching of English pronunciation: Designing and implementing an online course to teach word and sentence stress to tertiary level students." In ISAPh 2018 International Symposium on Applied Phonetics. ISCA: ISCA, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/isaph.2018-13.
Full text