To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Evidence assimilation.

Journal articles on the topic 'Evidence assimilation'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Evidence assimilation.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

King, Donald L., Pamela D. Brown, and Hester Hicks. "Assimilation in Visibility: Additional Evidence." Perceptual and Motor Skills 87, no. 3_suppl (December 1998): 1299–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.3f.1299.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Rassin, Eric. "Initial evidence for the assimilation hypothesis." Psychology, Crime & Law 23, no. 10 (September 4, 2017): 1010–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1068316x.2017.1371307.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Carrassi, A., M. Bocquet, A. Hannart, and M. Ghil. "Estimating model evidence using data assimilation." Quarterly Journal of the Royal Meteorological Society 143, no. 703 (January 2017): 866–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/qj.2972.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Burton, Martha W., and Karen E. Robblee. "Perceptual evidence of voicing assimilation in Russian." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 96, no. 5 (November 1994): 3229. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.411167.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Grant, Mary L. "Evidence of New Immigrant Assimilation in Canada." Canadian Journal of Economics / Revue canadienne d'Economique 32, no. 4 (August 1999): 930. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/136411.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Tietsche, S., D. Notz, J. H. Jungclaus, and J. Marotzke. "Assimilation of sea-ice concentration in a global climate model – physical and statistical aspects." Ocean Science 9, no. 1 (January 15, 2013): 19–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/os-9-19-2013.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. We investigate the initialisation of Northern Hemisphere sea ice in the global climate model ECHAM5/MPI-OM by assimilating sea-ice concentration data. The analysis updates for concentration are given by Newtonian relaxation, and we discuss different ways of specifying the analysis updates for mean thickness. Because the conservation of mean ice thickness or actual ice thickness in the analysis updates leads to poor assimilation performance, we introduce a proportional dependence between concentration and mean thickness analysis updates. Assimilation with these proportional mean-thickness analysis updates leads to good assimilation performance for sea-ice concentration and thickness, both in identical-twin experiments and when assimilating sea-ice observations. The simulation of other Arctic surface fields in the coupled model is, however, not significantly improved by the assimilation. To understand the physical aspects of assimilation errors, we construct a simple prognostic model of the sea-ice thermodynamics, and analyse its response to the assimilation. We find that an adjustment of mean ice thickness in the analysis update is essential to arrive at plausible state estimates. To understand the statistical aspects of assimilation errors, we study the model background error covariance between ice concentration and ice thickness. We find that the spatial structure of covariances is best represented by the proportional mean-thickness analysis updates. Both physical and statistical evidence supports the experimental finding that assimilation with proportional mean-thickness updates outperforms the other two methods considered. The method described here is very simple to implement, and gives results that are sufficiently good to be used for initialising sea ice in a global climate model for seasonal to decadal predictions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Magalhaes, Jose R. "Kinetics of15NH4+assimilation in tomato plants: evidence for15NH4+assimilation via GDH in tomato roots1." Journal of Plant Nutrition 14, no. 12 (December 1991): 1341–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01904169109364290.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Shepard, Timothy G., Adam Y. Shavit, Maria G. Veldhuizen, and Lawrence E. Marks. "Contextual Effects in Judgments of Taste Intensity: No Assimilation, Sometimes Contrast." Perception 46, no. 3-4 (December 26, 2016): 268–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006616686099.

Full text
Abstract:
Judgments of taste intensity often show contextual contrast but not assimilation, even though both effects of stimulus context appear in other sense modalities, such as hearing. Four experiments used a paradigm that shifts the stimulus context within a test session in order to seek evidence of assimilation in judgments of the taste intensity of sucrose and, for comparison, the loudness of 500-Hz tones. Experiment 1 found no assimilation in taste using three response scales, magnitude estimation, labeled magnitude, and visual analog, but did find evidence of contrast. Experiments 2 and 3 found no clear evidence of either assimilation or contrast in taste, but found consistent evidence of assimilation in loudness. Experiment 4 found no assimilation in loudness, however, when the intervals between successive stimuli increased from about 6 to 30 s in order to match the interval used with sucrose in Experiments 1 to 3. Taken together, these findings suggest that the assimilation found in intensity judgments in other sensory modalities may not appear in taste perception because of the slower rates presenting of taste stimuli.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Chan, Jeffrey W., and Amy Dolcourt‐McElroy. "Perceptual evidence for anticipatory assimilation of adjacent stops." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 83, S1 (May 1988): S13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2025209.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Villarreal, Andrés, and Christopher R. Tamborini. "Immigrants’ Economic Assimilation: Evidence from Longitudinal Earnings Records." American Sociological Review 83, no. 4 (June 15, 2018): 686–715. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0003122418780366.

Full text
Abstract:
We examine immigrants’ earnings trajectories and measure the extent and speed with which they are able to reduce the earnings gap with natives, using a dataset that links respondents of the Survey of Income and Program Participation (SIPP) to their longitudinal earnings obtained from individual tax records. Our analysis addresses key debates regarding ethnoracial and cohort differences in immigrants’ earnings trajectories. First, we find a racially differentiated pattern of earnings assimilation: black and Hispanic immigrants are less able to catch up with native whites’ earnings compared to white and Asian immigrants, but they are almost able to reach earnings parity with natives of their same race and ethnicity. Second, we find no evidence of a declining “quality” of immigrant cohorts even after controlling for their ethnoracial composition and human capital. Immigrants arriving since 1994 actually experience similar or slightly higher earnings growth compared to immigrants from earlier eras. We identify a pattern of accelerated assimilation in which more educated immigrants experience much of their earnings growth during the first years after arriving.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Tietsche, S., D. Notz, J. H. Jungclaus, and J. Marotzke. "Assimilation of sea-ice concentration in a global climate model – physical and statistical aspects." Ocean Science Discussions 9, no. 4 (July 19, 2012): 2403–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/osd-9-2403-2012.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. We investigate the initialization of Northern-hemisphere sea ice in the global climate model ECHAM5/MPI-OM by assimilating sea-ice concentration data. The analysis updates for concentration are given by Newtonian relaxation, and we discuss different ways of specifying the analysis updates for mean thickness. Because the conservation of mean ice thickness or actual ice thickness in the analysis updates leads to poor assimilation performance, we introduce a proportional dependence between concentration and mean thickness analysis updates. Assimilation with these proportional mean-thickness analysis updates significantly reduces assimilation error both in identical-twin experiments and when assimilating sea-ice observations, reducing the concentration error by a factor of four to six, and the thickness error by a factor of two. To understand the physical aspects of assimilation errors, we construct a simple prognostic model of the sea-ice thermodynamics, and analyse its response to the assimilation. We find that the strong dependence of thermodynamic ice growth on ice concentration necessitates an adjustment of mean ice thickness in the analysis update. To understand the statistical aspects of assimilation errors, we study the model background error covariance between ice concentration and ice thickness. We find that the spatial structure of covariances is best represented by the proportional mean-thickness analysis updates. Both physical and statistical evidence supports the experimental finding that proportional mean-thickness updates are superior to the other two methods considered and enable us to assimilate sea ice in a global climate model using simple Newtonian relaxation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Raeder, K., J. L. Anderson, P. G. Hess, L. K. Emmons, D. P. Edwards, G. G. Pfister, T. L. Campos, and G. W. Sachse. "Evaluating model performance of an ensemble-based chemical data assimilation system during INTEX-B field mission." Atmospheric Chemistry and Physics 7, no. 21 (November 16, 2007): 5695–710. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/acp-7-5695-2007.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. We present a global chemical data assimilation system using a global atmosphere model, the Community Atmosphere Model (CAM3) with simplified chemistry and the Data Assimilation Research Testbed (DART) assimilation package. DART is a community software facility for assimilation studies using the ensemble Kalman filter approach. Here, we apply the assimilation system to constrain global tropospheric carbon monoxide (CO) by assimilating meteorological observations of temperature and horizontal wind velocity and satellite CO retrievals from the Measurement of Pollution in the Troposphere (MOPITT) satellite instrument. We verify the system performance using independent CO observations taken on board the NSF/NCAR C-130 and NASA DC-8 aircrafts during the April 2006 part of the Intercontinental Chemical Transport Experiment (INTEX-B). Our evaluations show that MOPITT data assimilation provides significant improvements in terms of capturing the observed CO variability relative to no MOPITT assimilation (i.e. the correlation improves from 0.62 to 0.71, significant at 99% confidence). The assimilation provides evidence of median CO loading of about 150 ppbv at 700 hPa over the NE Pacific during April 2006. This is marginally higher than the modeled CO with no MOPITT assimilation (~140 ppbv). Our ensemble-based estimates of model uncertainty also show model overprediction over the source region (i.e. China) and underprediction over the NE Pacific, suggesting model errors that cannot be readily explained by emissions alone. These results have important implications for improving regional chemical forecasts and for inverse modeling of CO sources and further demonstrate the utility of the assimilation system in comparing non-coincident measurements, e.g. comparing satellite retrievals of CO with in-situ aircraft measurements.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Raeder, K., J. L. Anderson, P. G. Hess, L. K. Emmons, D. P. Edwards, G. G. Pfister, T. L. Campos, and G. W. Sachse. "Evaluating model performance of an ensemble-based chemical data assimilation system during INTEX-B field mission." Atmospheric Chemistry and Physics Discussions 7, no. 4 (July 5, 2007): 9717–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/acpd-7-9717-2007.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. We present a global chemical data assimilation system using a global atmosphere model, the Community Atmosphere Model (CAM3) with simplified chemistry and the Data Assimilation Research Testbed (DART) assimilation package. DART is a community software facility for assimilation studies using the ensemble Kalman filter approach. Here, we apply the assimilation system to constrain global tropospheric carbon monoxide (CO) by assimilating meteorological observations of temperature and horizontal wind velocity and satellite CO retrievals from the Measurement of Pollution in the Troposphere (MOPITT) satellite instrument. We verify the system performance using independent CO observations taken on board the NSF/NCAR C-130 and NASA DC-8 aircrafts during the April 2006 part of the Intercontinental Chemical Transport Experiment (INTEX-B). Our evaluations show that MOPITT data assimilation provides significant improvements in terms of capturing the observed CO variability relative to no MOPITT assimilation (i.e. the correlation improves from 0.62 to 0.71, significant at 99% confidence). The assimilation provides evidence of median CO loading of about 150 ppbv at 700 hPa over the NE Pacific during April 2006. This is marginally higher than the modeled CO with no MOPITT assimilation (~140 ppbv). Our ensemble-based estimates of model uncertainty also show model overprediction over the source region (i.e. China) and underprediction over the NE Pacific, suggesting model errors that cannot be readily explained by emissions alone. These results have important implications for improving regional chemical forecasts and for inverse modeling of CO sources and further demonstrates the utility of the assimilation system in comparing non-coincident measurements, e.g. comparing satellite retrievals of CO with in-situ aircraft measurements.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Mayer, Jochen, and Regina T. Riphahn. "Fertility assimilation of immigrants: Evidence from count data models." Journal of Population Economics 13, no. 2 (July 6, 2000): 241–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s001480050136.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Wilson, Ben. "The Intergenerational Assimilation of Completed Fertility: Comparing the Convergence of Different Origin Groups." International Migration Review 53, no. 2 (May 31, 2018): 429–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0197918318769047.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent studies have highlighted the need for new research on intergenerational assimilation and how it varies for different origin groups. This article responds by studying the intergenerational assimilation of completed fertility in the United Kingdom. The results provide evidence of assimilation for some origins, in particular for women from Ireland and Jamaica. Yet results also show evidence against assimilation for second-generation Pakistanis and Bangladeshis. The ability of the method used to distinguish between origin groups highlights the importance of a robust statistical approach that takes account of heterogeneity, an approach that can also be used to study outcomes other than fertility.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Alba, Richard, and Victor Nee. "Rethinking Assimilation Theory for a New Era of Immigration." International Migration Review 31, no. 4 (December 1997): 826–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/019791839703100403.

Full text
Abstract:
Assimilation theory has been subject to intensive critique for decades. Yet no other framework has provided the social science community with as deep a corpus of cumulative findings concerning the incorporation of immigrants and their descendants. We argue that assimilation theory has not lost its utility for the study of contemporary immigration to the United States. In making our case, we review critically the canonical account of assimilation provided by Milton Gordon and others; we refer to Shibutani and Kwan's theory of ethnic stratification to suggest some directions to take in reformulating assimilation theory. We also examine some of the arguments frequently made to distinguish between the earlier mass immigration of Europeans and the immigration of the contemporary era and find them to be inconclusive. Finally, we sift through some of the evidence about the socioeconomic and residential assimilation of recent immigrant groups. Though the record is clearly mixed, we find evidence consistent with the view that assimilation is taking place, albeit unevenly.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Rumbaut, Ruben G. "Assimilation and its Discontents: Between Rhetoric and Reality." International Migration Review 31, no. 4 (December 1997): 923–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/019791839703100406.

Full text
Abstract:
The process of immigrant assimilation is typically and uncritically conceived as one of linear progress – becoming similar to the dominant group in the place of destination is presumed to be a good thing. But a compelling body of evidence on the adaptation of immigrants and their children points to a deterioration of outcomes over time and generation in the United States, as well as to nonlinear processes of change. While linguistic assimilation among children of immigrants does proceed rapidly and inexorably as a linear function, other outcomes – in such diverse areas as infant and adolescent health, diet and divorce, delinquency and risk behaviors, educational achievement and aspirations, an ethos of hard work, and the development of an ethnic identity – contradict conventional expectations, expose underlying ethnocentric pretensions, and point instead to assimilation's discontents. By examining such paradoxes of immigrant adaptation that emerge in the conceptual interstices between rhetoric and reality, fruitful reformulations of a seminal sociological concept may be stimulated and advanced.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Carfagna, Simona, Vincenza Vona, Vittoria Di Martino, Sergio Esposito, and Carmelo Rigano. "Nitrogen assimilation and cysteine biosynthesis in barley: Evidence for root sulphur assimilation upon recovery from N deprivation." Environmental and Experimental Botany 71, no. 1 (April 2011): 18–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.envexpbot.2010.10.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Lipani, Lisa. "Word-final velar place assimilation in English." Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 2 (June 12, 2017): 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v2i0.4071.

Full text
Abstract:
In English, word-final alveolar consonants assimilate in place. Additionally, there is recent evidence that assimilation can occur in word-final nasals at all places of articulation (Coleman et al. 2016). Some anecdotal evidence exists that word-final velars can assimilate (Barry 1985), but this has not been substantiated. This study uses the Santa Barbara Corpus of American English (DuBois et al. 2000–2005) to examine word-final velar consonant variation, which was measured by the F2 transitions in the preceding vowel. Given the present data, word-final velars do not seem to undergo categorical assimilation or gradient coarticulation processes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

NICHOLSON, H., M. CONDOMINES, J. G. FITTON, A. E. FALLICK, K. GR NVOLD, and G. ROGERS. "Geochemical and Isotopic Evidence for Crustal Assimilation Beneath Krafla, Iceland." Journal of Petrology 32, no. 5 (October 1, 1991): 1005–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/petrology/32.5.1005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Emilio A. Parrado and S. Philip Morgan. "Intergenerational Fertility Among Hispanic Women: New Evidence of Immigrant Assimilation." Demography 45, no. 3 (2008): 651–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/dem.0.0023.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Ma, Liang. "Diffusion and Assimilation of Government Microblogging: Evidence from Chinese cities." Public Management Review 16, no. 2 (November 19, 2012): 274–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14719037.2012.725763.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Bammens, Bert, Kristin Verbeke, Yves Vanrenterghem, and Pieter Evenepoel. "Evidence for impaired assimilation of protein in chronic renal failure." Kidney International 64, no. 6 (December 2003): 2196–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1755.2003.00314.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Adserà, Alícia, and Ana M. Ferrer. "The Myth of Immigrant Women as Secondary Workers: Evidence from Canada." American Economic Review 104, no. 5 (May 1, 2014): 360–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.104.5.360.

Full text
Abstract:
We use the confidential files of the Canadian Census 1991-2006, combined with information from O*NET on the skill requirements of jobs, to show that the labor market patterns of female immigrants do not fit the profile of secondary workers, but rather conform to the recent experience of married native women with rising participation (and wage assimilation). At best, only relatively uneducated immigrant women in unskilled occupations may fit the profile of secondary workers. Educated immigrant women experience skill assimilation over time: a reduction in physical strength and a gradual increase in analytical skills required in their jobs relative to natives.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Jun, Jongho. "Place assimilation is not the result of gestural overlap: evidence from Korean and English." Phonology 13, no. 3 (December 1996): 377–407. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952675700002682.

Full text
Abstract:
In the theory of articulatory phonology Browman & Goldstein (1986, 1990, 1992) claim that place assimilation is mainly the result of the overlap of gestures and the perception of these overlapping gestures as a single gesture. Ohala (1990) makes a similar claim. The present study provides interesting experimental evidence against this explanation of assimilation as a result of gestural overlap and resulting misperception, and for the importance of gestural reduction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

RHEE, MUNSUNG, and AARON RAE STEPHENS. "INNOVATION-ORIENTATED TECHNOLOGY ASSIMILATION STRATEGY AND KOREAN SMES’ ENHANCING INNOVATION CAPABILITY, COMPETITIVE ADVANTAGE AND FIRM PERFORMANCE." International Journal of Innovation Management 24, no. 06 (January 31, 2020): 2050081. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s1363919620500814.

Full text
Abstract:
This research explores the effects of innovation-orientated technology assimilation strategy on Korean SMEs’ enhancing innovation capability, competitive advantage and firm performance. The results of this paper demonstrate that an innovation-orientated technology assimilation strategy enables SMEs to cultivate innovation capability, a crucial capability for catching-up to advanced firms and developing new products. In addition, innovation-orientated technology assimilation strategy significantly contributes to SMEs’ developing competitive advantages, leading to enhanced market share and sales growth. This study provides empirical evidence of SMEs employing innovation-orientated technology assimilation strategy to enhance innovation capability and to gain competitive advantage, and provides an empirical measurement for the execution of an innovation-orientated technology assimilation strategy.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Callens, Marie-Sophie, Bart Meuleman, and Valentová Marie. "Contact, Perceived Threat, and Attitudes Toward Assimilation and Multiculturalism: Evidence From a Majority and Minority Perspective in Luxembourg." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 50, no. 2 (December 13, 2018): 285–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022118817656.

Full text
Abstract:
In this article, we study how attitudes toward the integration of immigrants (multiculturalism and assimilation) are formed through the interplay between immigration-related threat perceptions, intergroup contacts, and the different migratory backgrounds of residents in a host country. The analysis is conducted using Multiple Group Structural Equation Modeling on data from the 2008 Luxembourg European Values Study. Our findings indicate that stronger perceptions of threat are related to more support for assimilation among all residents and to less support for multiculturalism among native residents and culturally close immigrants. More contact with natives is associated with more support for assimilation among culturally close immigrants and with more threat perceptions among culturally distant immigrants.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Bharati, Pratyush, and Abhijit Chaudhury. "Technology Assimilation Across the Value Chain." Information Resources Management Journal 25, no. 1 (January 2012): 38–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/irmj.2012010103.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper, the authors study technology assimilation, aggregating technologies and assimilation stages for SMEs. The authors employ the twin lenses of organizational innovation and elements of institutional theory. The research validates some institutional actors and most firm characteristics as important determinants. The relative weaknesses of the institutional actors provide evidence of structural isolation in the SME environment that is inhibiting information flow from intermediaries like government support agencies and vendors. The authors recommend a proactive role on the part of technology and enterprise intermediaries to design SME-appropriate solutions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Fürst, J. J., G. Durand, F. Gillet-Chaulet, N. Merino, L. Tavard, J. Mouginot, N. Gourmelen, and O. Gagliardini. "Assimilation of Antarctic velocity observations provides evidence for uncharted pinning points." Cryosphere 9, no. 4 (August 4, 2015): 1427–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/tc-9-1427-2015.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. In ice flow modelling, the use of control methods to assimilate the dynamic and geometric state of an ice body has become common practice. These methods have primarily focussed on inverting for one of the two least known properties in glaciology, namely the basal friction coefficient or the ice viscosity parameter. Here, we present an approach to infer both properties simultaneously for the whole of the Antarctic ice sheet. After the assimilation, the root-mean-square deviation between modelled and observed surface velocities attains 8.7 m a−1 for the entire domain, with a slightly higher value of 14.0 m a−1 for the ice shelves. An exception in terms of the velocity mismatch is the Thwaites Glacier Ice Shelf, where the RMS value is almost 70 m a−1. The reason is that the underlying Bedmap2 geometry ignores the presence of an ice rise, which exerts major control on the dynamics of the eastern part of the ice shelf. On these grounds, we suggest an approach to account for pinning points not included in Bedmap2 by locally allowing an optimisation of basal friction during the inversion. In this way, the velocity mismatch on the ice shelf of Thwaites Glacier is more than halved. A characteristic velocity mismatch pattern emerges for unaccounted pinning points close to the marine shelf front. This pattern is exploited to manually identify seven uncharted features around Antarctica that exert significant resistance to the shelf flow. Potential pinning points are detected on Fimbul, West, Shackleton, Nickerson and Venable ice shelves. As pinning points can provide substantial resistance to shelf flow, with considerable consequences if they became ungrounded in the future, the model community is in need of detailed bathymetry there. Our data assimilation points to some of these dynamically important features not present in Bedmap2 and implicitly quantifies their relevance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Fürst, J. J., G. Durand, F. Gillet-Chaulet, N. Merino, L. Tavard, J. Mouginot, N. Gourmelen, and O. Gagliardini. "Assimilation of Antarctic velocity observations provides evidence for uncharted pinning points." Cryosphere Discussions 9, no. 2 (March 5, 2015): 1461–502. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/tcd-9-1461-2015.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. In ice flow modelling, the use of control methods to assimilate the dynamic and geometric state of an ice body has become common practice. These methods have primarily focussed on inverting for one of the two least known properties in glaciology, namely the basal friction coefficient or the ice viscosity parameter. Here, we present an approach to infer both properties simultaneously for the whole of the Antarctic ice sheet. During the assimilation, the root-mean-square deviation between modelled and observed surface velocities is reduced to 12.3 m a−1, with a value of 16.4 m a−1 for the ice shelves. An exception in terms of the velocity mismatch is the Thwaites Glacier ice shelf, where the RMS value attains almost 80 m a−1. The reason is that the underlying BEDMAP2 geometry ignores the presence of an ice rise, that exerts major control on the dynamics of the eastern part of the ice shelf. On these grounds, we suggest an approach to account for pinning points not included in BEDMAP2 by locally allowing an optimisation of basal friction during the inversion. In this way, the velocity mismatch on the Thwaites ice shelf is more than halved. A characteristic velocity mismatch pattern emerges for unaccounted pinning points close to the marine shelf front. This pattern is exploited to manually identify 7 uncharted features around Antarctica that exert significant resistance to the shelf flow. Potential pinning points are detected on Fimbul, West, Shakelton, Nickerson and Venable ice shelves. As pinning points can provide substantial resistance to shelf flow, with considerable consequences if they became ungrounded in the future, the model community is in need of detailed bathymetry there. Our data assimilation points to some of these dynamically important features, not present in BEDMAP2, and implicitly quantifies their relevance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Mitterer, Holger, and Leo Blomert. "Coping with phonological assimilation in speech perception: Evidence for early compensation." Perception & Psychophysics 65, no. 6 (August 2003): 956–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194826.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Panzano, Phyllis C., Helen Anne Sweeney, Beverly Seffrin, Richard Massatti, and Kraig J. Knudsen. "The Assimilation of Evidence-Based Healthcare Innovations: A Management-Based Perspective." Journal of Behavioral Health Services & Research 39, no. 4 (October 2012): 397–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11414-012-9294-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Heckl, S., C. Reiners, A. K. Buck, A. Schäfer, A. Dick, and M. Scheurlen. "Evidence of impaired carbohydrate assimilation in euthyroid patients with Hashimoto’s thyroiditis." European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 70, no. 2 (October 7, 2015): 222–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2015.167.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Fossati, Flavia, Fabienne Liechti, and Daniel Auer. "Can signaling assimilation mitigate hiring discrimination? Evidence from a survey experiment." Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 65 (February 2020): 100462. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rssm.2019.100462.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Lichter, Daniel T., J. Brian Brown, Zhenchao Qian, and Julie H. Carmalt. "Marital Assimilation Among Hispanics: Evidence of Declining Cultural and Economic Incorporation?" Social Science Quarterly 88, no. 3 (September 2007): 745–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2007.00481.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Portes, Alejandro. "Migration, Development, and Segmented Assimilation: A Conceptual Review of the Evidence." ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 610, no. 1 (March 2007): 73–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0002716206296779.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Xu, Dafeng. "Transportation assimilation revisited: New evidence from repeated cross-sectional survey data." PLOS ONE 13, no. 4 (April 18, 2018): e0194296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0194296.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Inwood, Kris, Chris Minns, and Fraser Summerfield. "Occupational income scores and immigrant assimilation. Evidence from the Canadian census." Explorations in Economic History 72 (April 2019): 114–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eeh.2019.02.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Doi, Hideyuki, Eisuke Kikuchi, Shigeto Takagi, and Shuichi Shikano. "Selective assimilation by deposit feeders: Experimental evidence using stable isotope ratios." Basic and Applied Ecology 7, no. 2 (March 2006): 159–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.baae.2005.04.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Sánchez-Polo, María Teresa, Juan-Gabriel Cegarra-Navarro, Valentina Cillo, and Anthony Wensley. "Overcoming knowledge barriers to health care through continuous learning." Journal of Knowledge Management 23, no. 3 (April 8, 2019): 508–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/jkm-10-2018-0636.

Full text
Abstract:
PurposeThe purpose of this study is to explore the role of continuous learning and the mitigation or elimination of knowledge barriers affecting information technology (IT) assimilation in the health-care sector. Most of the problems with IT assimilations stem from a poor understanding of the nature of suitable information, the lack of trust, cultural differences, the lack of appropriate training and hierarchical bureaucratic structures and procedures. To overcome these barriers, this study provides evidence that a continuous learning process can play a part in overcoming some of the obstacles to the assimilation of IT.Design/methodology/approachThis study investigates how a continuous learning environment can counteract the presence of knowledge barriers, and, along with such an environment, can, in turn, facilitate IT assimilation. The study uses ADANCO 2.0.1 Professional for Windows and involves the collection and analysis of data provided by 210 health-care end users.FindingsThe study provides evidence in support of the proposition that continuous learning may facilitate the assimilation of IT by health-care end users through the mitigation of knowledge barriers (e.g. lack of trust or resistance to change). The mitigation of these barriers requires the gathering and utilization of new knowledge and knowledge structures. The results support the hypothesis that one way in which this can be achieved is through continuous learning (i.e. through assessing the situation, consulting experts, seeking feedback and tracking progress).Research limitations/implicationsA limitation of the study is the relatively simple statistical method that has been used for the analysis. However, the results provided here will serve as a preliminary basis for more sophisticated analysis which is currently underway.Practical implicationsThe study provides useful insights into ways of using continuous learning to facilitate IT assimilation by end users in the health-care domain. This can be of use to hospitals seeking to implement end user IT technologies and, in particular, telemedicine technologies. It can also be used to develop awareness of knowledge barriers and possible approaches to mitigate the effects of such barriers. Such an awareness can assist hospital staff in finding creative solutions for using technology tools. This potentially augments the ability of hospital staff to work with patients and carers, encouraging them to take initiative (make choices and solve problems relevant to them). This, in turn, allows hospitals to avoid negative and thus de-motivating experiences involving themselves and their end users (patients) and improving IT assimilation. This is liable to lead to improved morale and improved assimilation of IT by end users (patients).Social implicationsAs ICT systems and services should entail participation of a wide range of users, developers and stakeholders, including medical doctors, nurses, social workers, patients and programmers and interaction designers, the study provides useful social implication for health management and people well-being.Originality/valueThe paper contributes to a better understanding of the nature and impacts of continuous learning. Although previous studies in the field of knowledge management have shown that knowledge management procedures and routines can provide support to IT assimilation, few studies, if any, have explored the relationship between continuous learning and IT assimilation with particular emphasis on knowledge barriers in the health-care domain.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

SHEVELL, STEVEN K., and DINGCAI CAO. "Chromatic assimilation unaffected by perceived depth of inducing light." Visual Neuroscience 21, no. 3 (May 2004): 373–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952523804213050.

Full text
Abstract:
Chromatic assimilation is a shift toward the color of nearby light. Several studies conclude that a neural process contributes to assimilation but the neural locus remains in question. Some studies posit a peripheral process, such as retinal receptive-field organization, while others claim the neural mechanism follows depth perception, figure/ground segregation, or perceptual grouping. The experiments here tested whether assimilation depends on a neural process that follows stereoscopic depth perception. By introducing binocular disparity, the test field judged in color was made to appear in a different depth plane than the light that induced assimilation. The chromaticity and spatial frequency of the inducing light, and the chromaticity of the test light, were varied. Chromatic assimilation was found with all inducing-light sizes and chromaticities, but the magnitude of assimilation did not depend on the perceived relative depth planes of the test and inducing fields. We found no evidence to support the view that chromatic assimilation depends on a neural process that follows binocular combination of the two eyes' signals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Bain, Wyatt M., Matthew Steele-MacInnis, Fernando Tornos, John M. Hanchar, Emily C. Creaser, and Dorota K. Pietruszka. "Evidence for iron-rich sulfate melt during magnetite(-apatite) mineralization at El Laco, Chile." Geology 49, no. 9 (May 17, 2021): 1044–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1130/g48861.1.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The origins of Kiruna-type magnetite(-apatite) [Mt(-Ap)] deposits are contentious, with existing models ranging from purely hydrothermal to orthomagmatic end members. Here, we evaluate the compositions of fluids that formed the classic yet enigmatic Mt(-Ap) deposit at El Laco, northern Chile. We report evidence that ore-stage minerals crystallized from an Fe-rich (6–17 wt% Fe) sulfate melt. We suggest that a major component of the liquid was derived from assimilation of evaporite-bearing sedimentary rocks during emplacement of andesitic magma at depth. Hence, we argue that assimilation of evaporite-bearing sedimentary strata played a key role in the formation of El Laco and likely Mt(-Ap) deposits elsewhere.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Walworth, Nathan G., Michael D. Lee, Fei-Xue Fu, David A. Hutchins, and Eric A. Webb. "Molecular and physiological evidence of genetic assimilation to high CO2in the marine nitrogen fixerTrichodesmium." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, no. 47 (November 8, 2016): E7367—E7374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1605202113.

Full text
Abstract:
Most investigations of biogeochemically important microbes have focused on plastic (short-term) phenotypic responses in the absence of genetic change, whereas few have investigated adaptive (long-term) responses. However, no studies to date have investigated the molecular progression underlying the transition from plasticity to adaptation under elevated CO2for a marine nitrogen-fixer. To address this gap, we cultured the globally important cyanobacteriumTrichodesmiumat both low and high CO2for 4.5 y, followed by reciprocal transplantation experiments to test for adaptation. Intriguingly, fitness actually increased in all high-CO2adapted cell lines in the ancestral environment upon reciprocal transplantation. By leveraging coordinated phenotypic and transcriptomic profiles, we identified expression changes and pathway enrichments that rapidly responded to elevated CO2and were maintained upon adaptation, providing strong evidence for genetic assimilation. These candidate genes and pathways included those involved in photosystems, transcriptional regulation, cell signaling, carbon/nitrogen storage, and energy metabolism. Conversely, significant changes in specific sigma factor expression were only observed upon adaptation. These data reveal genetic assimilation as a potentially adaptive response ofTrichodesmiumand importantly elucidate underlying metabolic pathways paralleling the fixation of the plastic phenotype upon adaptation, thereby contributing to the few available data demonstrating genetic assimilation in microbial photoautotrophs. These molecular insights are thus critical for identifying pathways under selection as drivers in plasticity and adaptation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Coleman, John, Margaret E. L. Renwick, and Rosalind A. M. Temple. "Probabilistic underspecification in nasal place assimilation." Phonology 33, no. 3 (December 2016): 425–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952675716000208.

Full text
Abstract:
According to many works on English phonology, word-final alveolar consonants – and only alveolar consonants – assimilate to following word-initial consonants, e.g. ran quickly → ra[ŋ] quickly. Some phonologists explain the readiness of alveolar consonants to assimilate (vs. the resistance of velar and labial articulations) by proposing that they have underspecified place of articulation (e.g. Avery & Rice 1989). Labial or dorsal nasals do not undergo assimilation because their place nodes are specified. There are reports that velar and labial consonants sometimes assimilate in English, but these are anecdotal observations, with no available audio and no statistics on their occurrence. We find evidence of assimilation of labial and velar nasals in the Audio British National Corpus, motivating a new, quantitative phonological framework: a statistical model of underspecification and variation which captures typical as well as less common but systematic patterns seen in non-coronal assimilation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Cao, Fan, Ran Tao, Li Liu, Charles A. Perfetti, and James R. Booth. "High Proficiency in a Second Language is Characterized by Greater Involvement of the First Language Network: Evidence from Chinese Learners of English." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, no. 10 (October 2013): 1649–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00414.

Full text
Abstract:
The assimilation hypothesis argues that second language learning recruits the brain network for processing the native language, whereas the accommodation hypothesis argues that learning a second language recruits brain structures not involved in native language processing. This study tested these hypotheses by examining brain activation of a group of native Chinese speakers, who were late bilinguals with varying levels of proficiency in English, when they performed a rhyming judgment to visually presented English word pairs (CE group) during fMRI. Assimilation was examined by comparing the CE group to native Chinese speakers performing the rhyming task in Chinese (CC group), and accommodation was examined by comparing the CE group to native English speakers performing the rhyming task in English (EE group). The CE group was very similar in activation to the CC group, supporting the assimilation hypothesis. Additional support for the assimilation hypothesis was the finding that higher proficiency in the CE group was related to increased activation in the Chinese network (as defined by the CC > EE), including the left middle frontal gyrus, the right inferior parietal lobule, and the right precuneus, and decreased activation in the English network (as defined by the EE > CC), including the left inferior frontal gyrus and the left inferior temporal gyrus. Although most of the results support assimilation, there was some evidence for accommodation as the CE group showed less activation in the Chinese network including the right middle occipital gyrus, which has been argued to be involved in holistic visuospatial processing of Chinese characters.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Rumbaut, Rubén G. "Paradoxes (and Orthodoxies) of Assimilation." Sociological Perspectives 40, no. 3 (September 1997): 483–511. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1389453.

Full text
Abstract:
The concept of assimilation, whether as outcome or process, conflates elements that are both empirical and ideological, ethnographic and ethnocentric. Conventional wisdom on the adaptation of immigrants in America has conceived of “assimilation” prescriptively and not only descriptively, as a linear process of progressive adjustment to American life. This conception is guided by an implicit deficit model: to get ahead immigrants need to learn how to “become American” and overcome their deficits with respect to the new language and culture, the new economy and society. As they shed the old and acquire the new over time, they surmount those obstacles and make their way more successfully—a homogenizing process more or less completed by the second or third generation. Recent research findings, however, especially in the areas of immigrant health, mental health, ethnic self-identity and education, debunk such ethnocentric assumptions, often running precisely in the opposite direction of what is expected from traditional perspectives. Some empirical examples are highlighted, focusing on paradoxes—on evidence that contradicts orthodox expectations—in order to identify areas that need conceptual, analytical, and theoretical refinement, including the need to spell out precisely and systematically what it is that is being “assimilated,” by whom, under what circumstances, and in reference to what sector of American society. The diversity of contemporary immigrants to the United States, in terms of class, culture, color, and the contexts within which they are received, and their segmented modes of incorporation, raise new questions about assimilation from what? to what? and for what?
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Brewster, Billy E. "How a Systems Perspective Improves Knowledge Acquisition and Performance in Analytical Procedures." Accounting Review 86, no. 3 (May 1, 2011): 915–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.2308/accr.00000040.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACT: Auditors are required to understand dynamic business environments as part of the performance of analytical procedures. Prior evidence, though, suggests that auditors have difficulty understanding such environments. This study reports an experimental investigation of techniques that help auditors to identify incorrect management representations by developing expectations that are both accurate and adaptive to changing business conditions. My predictions suggest that analyzing a dynamic client environment through a systems perspective enhances information-processing ability, which then improves both evidence discrimination and the assimilation of new audit evidence. Results reveal that participants taking a holistic systems-based view of a client environment develop more coherently organized mental models that increase their likelihood of identifying management representations that are inconsistent with industry evidence. Furthermore, these participants more efficiently use their information-processing ability, thereby improving assimilation of newly learned evidence to understand how changing business conditions affect their initial expectations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Barak, Phillip, J. A. E. Molina, Aviva Hadas, and C. E. Clapp. "Mineralization of Amino Acids and Evidence of Direct Assimilation of Organic Nitrogen." Soil Science Society of America Journal 54, no. 3 (May 1990): 769–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.2136/sssaj1990.03615995005400030024x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

O’Connell-Cooper, Catherine D., Alan P. Dickin, and John G. Spray. "The Manicouagan impact melt sheet: Evidence for isotopic homogenization with limited assimilation." Earth and Planetary Science Letters 335-336 (June 2012): 48–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.epsl.2012.04.033.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Lemos, Sara. "Immigrant economic assimilation: Evidence from UK longitudinal data between 1978 and 2006." Labour Economics 24 (October 2013): 339–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2013.09.009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography