To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Famous faces.

Journal articles on the topic 'Famous faces'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Famous faces.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Castillo, M. "Recognizing Famous Faces." American Journal of Neuroradiology 34, no. 2 (2012): 257–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.a3100.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Swinbanks, David. "Famous foreign faces." Nature 345, no. 6274 (1990): 379. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/345379a0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

MILDERS, M. "Naming Famous Faces and Buildings." Cortex 36, no. 1 (2000): 139–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70842-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Alsufyani, Abdulmajeed, Omid Hajilou, Alexia Zoumpoulaki, et al. "Breakthrough percepts of famous faces." Psychophysiology 56, no. 1 (2018): e13279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13279.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Fisher, Rachel. "Book Review: Nostalgia, Famous Faces." British Journal of Occupational Therapy 49, no. 5 (1986): 171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/030802268604900518.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

O'Grady, Cathleen. "Famous psychologist faces posthumous reckoning." Science 369, no. 6501 (2020): 233–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.369.6501.233.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Carbon, Claus-Christian. "Famous Faces as Icons. The Illusion of Being an Expert in the Recognition of Famous Faces." Perception 37, no. 5 (2008): 801–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5789.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Greene, John D. W., and John R. Hodges. "Identification of famous faces and famous names in early Alzheimer's disease." Brain 119, no. 1 (1996): 111–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.1.111.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hicks, Richard E., Victoria Alexander, and Mark Bahr. "Facial Recognition and Visual Processing as We Age: Using the Thatcher Illusion with Famous and Non-Famous Faces." International Journal of Psychological Studies 9, no. 2 (2017): 26. http://dx.doi.org/10.5539/ijps.v9n2p26.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper reports a study examining preferred visual processes in recognition of facial features in older vs younger age groups, using Thatcherised images of famous and non-famous people in the one study. The aims were to determine whether decline in visual system processing occurs increasingly as we grow older, and whether there is less decline in recognition of famous (or familiar) faces. Three groups (younger, middle-old and older) made up the sample of 73 people (aged 19-82 years). Visual decline in face recognition across the age groups was assessed based on the Thatcher illusion—using four famous and four non-famous faces either with normal features or with distorted features. The faces were presented one at a time on computer screen, and participants were asked to judge whether the face was distorted (eyes and/or mouth not aligned in relation to the face); in addition, time taken to decision (latency) was also measured. Decline was found in visual processing such that older individuals gave limited attention to facial details (processing faces holistically, with detail errors) and they took longer to decide. Whether the faces were famous or not did not have significant effects on the decisions and there was no interaction with age, though famous faces were given longer attention. Our visual system processes decline as we age in that we give less attention to details and more to holistic processing and so make more errors in recognition. Implications for treatment or amelioration of the effects are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Stone, Anna, and Tim Valentine. "Orientation of attention to nonconsciously recognised famous faces." Cognition & Emotion 19, no. 4 (2005): 537–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930441000409.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Valentine, Tim, and Stephen Darling. "Competitor effects in naming objects and famous faces." European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 18, no. 5 (2006): 686–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440500299131.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Schweinberger, Stefan R., A. Mike Burton, and Stephen W. Kelly. "Priming the access to names of famous faces." British Journal of Psychology 92, no. 2 (2001): 303–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712601162202.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Langlois, Roxane, Francine Fontaine, Caroline Hamel, and Sven Joubert. "Manque du nom propre et effet de la modalité sur la capacité à reconnaître des personnes connues au cours du vieillissement normal." Canadian Journal on Aging / La Revue canadienne du vieillissement 28, no. 4 (2009): 337–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0714980809990183.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThe aim of this study was to investigate the impact of aging on the ability a) to name famous faces and b) to access biographical knowledge about famous people from different modalities of presentation (faces and names). Healthy elderly subjects (n = 117) divided into three different age groups were assessed using a semantic memory test of famous people. Results of this study indicate a decline in naming performance between oldest and youngest groups. In contrast, no difference between groups was found in terms of the ability to access semantic knowledge about famous people. Finally, a main effect of modality of presentation (faces vs. names) was found, revealing greater ability to identify famous people in the verbal (names) than in the visual modality (faces). Taken together, results of this study may contribute to developing new cognitive intervention programs in older adults presenting with proper-name anomia.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Snowden, J. S. "Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia." Brain 127, no. 4 (2004): 860–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awh099.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Jenkins, Rob, Nilli Lavie, and Jon Driver. "Ignoring famous faces: Category-specific dilution of distractor interference." Perception & Psychophysics 65, no. 2 (2003): 298–309. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03194801.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Bar, M., E. Aminoff, and A. Ishai. "Famous Faces Activate Contextual Associations in the Parahippocampal Cortex." Cerebral Cortex 18, no. 6 (2007): 1233–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm170.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Glosser, G., A. E. Salvucci, and N. D. Chiaravalloti. "Naming and recognizing famous faces in temporal lobe epilepsy." Neurology 61, no. 1 (2003): 81–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000073621.18013.e1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Lander, Karen, and Vicki Bruce. "Recognizing Famous Faces: Exploring the Benefits of Facial Motion." Ecological Psychology 12, no. 4 (2000): 259–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1204_01.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Machado, Liana, Hayley Guiney, and Andrew Mitchell. "Famous Faces Demand Attention Due to Reduced Inhibitory Processing." PLoS ONE 6, no. 5 (2011): e20544. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020544.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Konstantinou, Ira, and John Gardiner. "Conscious control and memory awareness when recognising famous faces." Memory 13, no. 5 (2005): 449–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000016.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Gallegos, Diana R., and Daniel Tranel. "Positive facial affect facilitates the identification of famous faces." Brain and Language 93, no. 3 (2005): 338–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2004.11.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Martens, Ulla, Stefan R. Schweinberger, Markus Kiefer, and A. Mike Burton. "Masked and unmasked electrophysiological repetition effects of famous faces." Brain Research 1109, no. 1 (2006): 146–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.066.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Ryan, Jennifer D., Sandra N. Moses, Melanie L. Ostreicher, et al. "Seeing Sounds and Hearing Sights: The Influence of Prior Learning on Current Perception." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, no. 6 (2008): 1030–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20075.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known that previous perceptual experiences alter subsequent perception, but the details of the neural underpinnings of this general phenomenon are still sketchy. Here, we ask whether previous experiences with an item (such as seeing a person's face) leads to the alteration of the neural correlates related to processing of the item as such, or whether it creates additional associative connections between such substrates and those activated during prior experience. To address this question, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to identify neural changes accompanying subjects' viewing of unfamiliar versus famous faces and hearing the names of unfamiliar versus famous names. We were interested in the nature of the involvement of auditory brain regions in the viewing of faces, and in the involvement of visual regions in the hearing of names. Evoked responses from MEG recordings for the names and faces conditions were localized to auditory and visual cortices, respectively. Unsurprisingly, peak activation strength of evoked responses was larger for famous versus nonfamous names within the superior temporal gyrus (STG), and was similar for famous and nonfamous faces in the occipital cortex. More relevant to the issue of experience on perception, peak activation strength in the STG was larger for viewed famous versus nonfamous faces, and peak activation within the occipital cortex was larger for heard famous versus nonfamous names. Critically, these experience-related responses were present within 150–250 msec of stimulus onset. These findings support the hypothesis that prior experiences may influence processing of faces and names such that perception encompasses more than what is imparted on the senses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Johnston, Robert A., Christopher Barry, and Catherine Williams. "Incomplete Faces Don't Show the Whole Picture: Repetition Priming from Jumbled Faces." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 49, no. 3 (1996): 596–615. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755637.

Full text
Abstract:
Four experiments examined repetition priming of familiarity decisions to faces of famous people by the prior exposure of intact or jumbled faces. In Experiments 1 and 2 the primes were either the identical picture of the target face or a picture of the face with the internal features jumbled up. (In Experiment 2 the external features were also removed from all faces.) Compared with response times to previously unseen faces, familiarity decisions were made more rapidly if the subject had seen and identified the famous face in the pre-training stage; this was independent of whether they saw an intact or jumbled face. Priming was not shown if the face was not recognized earlier. Experiment 3 demonstrated that, if faces were not recognized spontaneously in the pre-training stage, being prompted as to their identity by the experimenter still did not yield priming at test—a result that replicated a previous study using incomplete faces (Brunas-Wagstaff, Young, & Ellis, 1992). Experiment 4 showed that it was the situation in which the information was given that was critical in determining whether priming occurred. The findings of this study are related to mechanisms for repetition priming of faces and used to discuss the necessity of modifications to the Bruce and Young (1986) model such as those offered by Burton, Bruce, and Johnston (1990).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Lander, Karen, Fiona Christie, and Vicki Bruce. "The role of movement in the recognition of famous faces." Memory & Cognition 27, no. 6 (1999): 974–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03201228.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Damjanovic, Ljubica, and J. Richard Hanley. "Recalling episodic and semantic information about famous faces and voices." Memory & Cognition 35, no. 6 (2007): 1205–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193594.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Baird, Lyndsay M., and A. Mike Burton. "The bilateral advantage for famous faces: Interhemispheric communication or competition?" Neuropsychologia 46, no. 5 (2008): 1581–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Axelrod, Vadim, and Galit Yovel. "Successful Decoding of Famous Faces in the Fusiform Face Area." PLOS ONE 10, no. 2 (2015): e0117126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117126.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Boehm, Stephan G., and Werner Sommer. "Neural correlates of intentional and incidental recognition of famous faces." Cognitive Brain Research 23, no. 2-3 (2005): 153–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.10.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Claunch, Joshua D., Adam D. Falchook, John B. Williamson, et al. "Famous faces but not remembered spaces influence vertical line bisections." Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 34, no. 9 (2012): 919–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2012.698600.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Yang, Pei-Ling, and Diane M. Beck. "Does familiarity influence discrimination? Famous and Inverted Faces and Logos." Journal of Vision 21, no. 9 (2021): 2001. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.21.9.2001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Lee, Kieran J., and David Perrett. "Presentation-Time Measures of the Effects of Manipulations in Colour Space on Discrimination of Famous Faces." Perception 26, no. 6 (1997): 733–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p260733.

Full text
Abstract:
Caricaturing the distinctive shape of famous faces can produce an advantage in reactiontime paradigms but the role of distinctive colour and intensity information in recognition of facial identity has not previously been explored. A presentation-time paradigm was developed by which stimuli could be presented for a range of brief display periods. Subjects were required to identify photo-realistic colour representations of famous faces which either were veridical, were caricatured in colour space, or had enhanced colour saturation and intensity contrast (as contrast controls). Recognition accuracy was greater when viewing the caricatured stimuli than either the veridical images or the contrast controls. The removal of colour information to produce grey-scale images also decreased accuracy of face recognition. Both results indicate that colour information aids differentiation of a class of natural stimuli with similar configurations. Thus it is demonstrated that caricaturing faces can be extended to the colour domain and, as with shape caricaturing, enhancement of distinctive information can produce a recognition advantage for famous faces.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Richards, Rachel M., and Andrew W. Ellis. "Mechanisms of Identity and Gender Decisions to Faces: Who Rocked in 1986?" Perception 37, no. 11 (2008): 1700–1719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p6023.

Full text
Abstract:
Two experiments are reported in which participants made familiarity decisions (Is this face familiar or not?) or gender decisions (Is this face male or female?) to the same sets of faces presented as whole faces or as internal features only. The experimental items on which the analysis was performed were famous and unfamiliar male faces that differed on rated masculinity. The famous faces also differed on age of acquisition (AoA). The experimental male faces were combined with an equal number of famous and unfamiliar female faces for presentation to participants. In experiment 1 we found faster and more accurate familiarity decisions to whole male faces than to internal features; also an interaction between AoA and masculinity such that familiarity decisions were both faster and more accurate to high-than to low-masculinity faces when those faces were late-acquired, but not when they were early-acquired. In experiment 2 we found the same benefit for whole faces over internal features and the same interaction between AoA and masculinity in gender decisions. The similarity between the effects of AoA and masculinity in familiarity and gender decisions is more readily accounted for by models of face processing which posit a close relationship between gender and identity processing than by models which maintain that those aspects of face perception are dealt with by quite separate processing streams. We propose that gender decisions, like familiarity decisions, are semantic judgments (rather than judgments based on the analysis of the surface features of faces), and that the shared basis of the two forms of decision explains why they show similar influences of AoA and masculinity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Bortolon, Catherine, Siméon Lorieux, and Stéphane Raffard. "Self- or familiar-face recognition advantage? New insight using ambient images." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 71, no. 6 (2018): 1396–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2017.1327982.

Full text
Abstract:
Self-face recognition has been widely explored in the past few years. Nevertheless, the current literature relies on the use of standardized photographs which do not represent daily-life face recognition. Therefore, we aim for the first time to evaluate self-face processing in healthy individuals using natural/ambient images which contain variations in the environment and in the face itself. In total, 40 undergraduate and graduate students performed a forced delayed-matching task, including images of one’s own face, friend, famous and unknown individuals. For both reaction time and accuracy, results showed that participants were faster and more accurate when matching different images of their own face compared to both famous and unfamiliar faces. Nevertheless, no significant differences were found between self-face and friend-face and between friend-face and famous-face. They were also faster and more accurate when matching friend and famous faces compared to unfamiliar faces. Our results suggest that faster and more accurate responses to self-face might be better explained by a familiarity effect – that is, (1) the result of frequent exposition to one’s own image through mirror and photos, (2) a more robust mental representation of one’s own face and (3) strong face recognition units as for other familiar faces.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Neumann, M. F., S. R. Schweinberger, and A. M. Burton. "Seeing sets for famous faces: power and limits of summary representations." Journal of Vision 12, no. 9 (2012): 503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.9.503.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Brace, N. A., G. E. Pike, P. Allen, and R. I. Kemp. "Identifying composites of famous faces: Investigating memory, language and system issues." Psychology, Crime & Law 12, no. 4 (2006): 351–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10683160500151159.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Nie, Aiqing, Minye Li, and Jingheng Ye. "Lag-length effect on repetition priming of famous and unfamiliar faces." NeuroReport 27, no. 10 (2016): 755–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000000605.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Neumann, Markus F., Stefan R. Schweinberger, and A. Mike Burton. "Viewers extract mean and individual identity from sets of famous faces." Cognition 128, no. 1 (2013): 56–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.006.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Bindemann, Markus, Janice Attard, Robert A. Johnston, and Peter Walla. "Perceived ability and actual recognition accuracy for unfamiliar and famous faces." Cogent Psychology 1, no. 1 (2014): 986903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2014.986903.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Damian, Markus F., and Rasha Abdel Rahman. "Semantic priming in the name retrieval of objects and famous faces." British Journal of Psychology 94, no. 4 (2003): 517–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/000712603322503079.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Tay, Sze Yan, Christopher Gabriel, Way Inn Koay, Kinjal Doshi, Simon Kang Seng Ting, and Shahul Hameed. "P3-259: DEVELOPMENT AND VALIDATION OF THE SINGAPORE FAMOUS FACES TEST." Alzheimer's & Dementia 10 (July 2014): P726—P727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2014.05.1351.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Rhodes, G., and R. Wooding. "Laterality effects in identification of caricatures and photographs of famous faces." Brain and Cognition 9, no. 2 (1989): 201–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(89)90030-4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Benson, Philip J., and David I. Perrett. "Visual Processing of Facial Distinctiveness." Perception 23, no. 1 (1994): 75–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p230075.

Full text
Abstract:
Three experiments are reported in which the role of facial distinctiveness in line-drawing representations of famous faces has been examined. In the first subjects were asked to select their impressions of best likeness interactively from a continuous range of drawings generated in real-time. On average, 42% of caricatures were considered to be the best likeness of famous individuals. The distinctiveness of the target faces related by independent observers correlated with the degree of exaggeration faces received to produce an optimal likeness. More distinctive faces required less caricaturing. In the second experiment caricature levels chosen as best likenesses were investigated. Caricatures of famous faces were recognised more quickly (by 36% of subjects) and more accurately (by 28%) than the true veridical drawings. Thus caricature enhancement can improve recognition in terms of both speed and accuracy. In the third experiment the role of distinctiveness and its interaction with external and internal facial features was investigated. The external (outer) hairline present in the previous experiment was found to provide a powerful frame of reference for the perceptual assessment of individuals' appearances; estimations of best likeness without external features present were, on average, veridical. It is concluded that accurate line drawings of even highly familiar faces are poor at conveying identity. Distinctiveness enhancement improves their likeness and their recognisability. Such caricatured drawings provide improved access to memories of famous faces, which lends strong support to models of human-face memory and processing based on norm-based coding. When required to decide upon the veracity of line drawings based only upon internal features subjects were able to make accurate judgments of veridicality. As well as validating the interaction approach to best-likeness judgments, this result further indicates the difficulty in making judgments of identification based on piecemeal and sparse configural information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Stone, Anna, and Tim Valentine. "Angry and happy faces perceived without awareness: A comparison with the affective impact of masked famous faces." European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19, no. 2 (2007): 161–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09541440600616390.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Zion-Golumbic, Elana, Marta Kutas, and Shlomo Bentin. "Neural Dynamics Associated with Semantic and Episodic Memory for Faces: Evidence from Multiple Frequency Bands." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 2 (2010): 263–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21251.

Full text
Abstract:
Prior semantic knowledge facilitates episodic recognition memory for faces. To examine the neural manifestation of the interplay between semantic and episodic memory, we investigated neuroelectric dynamics during the creation (study) and the retrieval (test) of episodic memories for famous and nonfamous faces. Episodic memory effects were evident in several EEG frequency bands: theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (9–13 Hz), and gamma (40–100 Hz). Activity in these bands was differentially modulated by preexisting semantic knowledge and by episodic memory, implicating their different functional roles in memory. More specifically, theta activity and alpha suppression were larger for old compared to new faces at test regardless of fame, but were both larger for famous faces during study. This pattern of selective semantic effects suggests that the theta and alpha responses, which are primarily associated with episodic memory, reflect utilization of semantic information only when it is beneficial for task performance. In contrast, gamma activity decreased between the first (study) and second (test) presentation of a face, but overall was larger for famous than nonfamous faces. Hence, the gamma rhythm seems to be primarily related to activation of preexisting neural representations that may contribute to the formation of new episodic traces. Taken together, these data provide new insights into the complex interaction between semantic and episodic memory for faces and the neural dynamics associated with mnemonic processes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Jemel, Boutheina, Anne-Marie Schuller, and Valérie Goffaux. "Characterizing the Spatio-temporal Dynamics of the Neural Events Occurring prior to and up to Overt Recognition of Famous Faces." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 10 (2010): 2289–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21320.

Full text
Abstract:
Although it is generally acknowledged that familiar face recognition is fast, mandatory, and proceeds outside conscious control, it is still unclear whether processes leading to familiar face recognition occur in a linear (i.e., gradual) or a nonlinear (i.e., all-or-none) manner. To test these two alternative accounts, we recorded scalp ERPs while participants indicated whether they recognize as familiar the faces of famous and unfamiliar persons gradually revealed in a descending sequence of frames, from the noisier to the least noisy. This presentation procedure allowed us to characterize the changes in scalp ERP responses occurring prior to and up to overt recognition. Our main finding is that gradual and all-or-none processes are possibly involved during overt recognition of familiar faces. Although the N170 and the N250 face-sensitive responses displayed an abrupt activity change at the moment of overt recognition of famous faces, later ERPs encompassing the N400 and late positive component exhibited an incremental increase in amplitude as the point of recognition approached. In addition, famous faces that were not overtly recognized at one trial before recognition elicited larger ERP potentials than unfamiliar faces, probably reflecting a covert recognition process. Overall, these findings present evidence that recognition of familiar faces implicates spatio-temporally complex neural processes exhibiting differential pattern activity changes as a function of recognition state.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Toth, Jeffrey P., and Karen A. Daniels. "The Influence of Prior Knowledge on Memory and Metamemory for Famous Names and Faces." Zeitschrift für Psychologie 229, no. 2 (2021): 131–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000443.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Two experiments examined the effect of prior knowledge on memory and metamemory for names and faces using famous 1960s and 2000s actors as the manipulation of prior knowledge. In Experiment 1, 66 participants studied the names of famous actors, half presented with their faces, with instructions to remember only the names. In Experiment 2, 56 participants studied the faces of these actors, half presented with their names, with instructions to remember only the faces. In both experiments, participants made immediate Judgments of Learning (JOLs) for each to-be-remembered stimulus followed by a test of recognition that used a Recollect/Familiar/No-Memory judgment. We found higher JOLs, recognition memory, and JOL accuracy for 2000s actors. Adding a name to a face or a face to a name increased JOLs while paradoxically decreasing memory. Back-sorting and binning analyses converged on the conclusion that immediate JOLs predicted memories accompanied by recollection but not familiarity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Campbell, Ruth, and Michelle Tuck. "Recognition of Parts of Famous-Face Photographs by Children: An Experimental Note." Perception 24, no. 4 (1995): 451–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p240451.

Full text
Abstract:
Children aged between 5 and 10 years were shown photographs of familiar TV personalities, presenters, and celebrities for immediate recognition (naming). The pictures were of whole faces, outer features only (hair, chin, ears), or inner features (eyes, nose, mouth). Only the oldest group (9–10 years) resembled adults in recognising these familiar faces more efficiently from internal than external parts. In the youngest children (5–6 years) there were some indications that the outer face features were more salient, particularly when cartoon characters were included in the array.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Thoma, Volker, and Jan W. De Fockert. "Three-Quarter Views of Depth-Rotated Faces Induce Face-Specific Capacity Limits in Visual Search." Experimental Psychology 65, no. 6 (2018): 360–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000421.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Participants made speeded categorization decisions regarding a famous person (politician or film star) accompanied by a peripheral distracter face (either the same or from the opposite category). The first experiment found that processing a peripheral distracter face is independent of load when the search set contains name strings. The search set in the second experiment consisted of faces. Interference effects between the target and distracter face (both shown in frontal views) were found when no additional non-target faces were present (low load), but not when two non-famous faces (high load) accompanied the target face, even when the latter were shown in three-quarter views. These results indicate that face-specific capacity limitations are independent of changes in view (up to 45°) and gaze direction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Ameller, Aurely, Laetitia Delbos, Anne-Laure Cote, Pierre Thomas, and Delphine Pins. "Poster #66 COVERT RECOGNITION OF FAMILIAR, FAMOUS AND UNFAMILIAR FACES IN SCHIZOPHRENIA." Schizophrenia Research 136 (April 2012): S115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(12)70381-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography