To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG).

Journal articles on the topic 'Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 39 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Müller, Stefan. "Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, Sign-Based Construction Grammar, and Fluid Construction Grammar." Constructions and Frames 9, no. 1 (2017): 139–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/cf.9.1.05mul.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Van Trijp (2013, 2014) claims that Sign-Based Construction Grammar (SBCG) and Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) are fundamentally different from Fluid Construction Grammar (FCG). He claims that the former approaches are generative ones while the latter is a cognitive-functional one. I argue that it is not legitimate to draw these distinctions on the basis of what is done in FCG. Van Trijp claims that there are differences in the scientific model, the linguistic approach, formalization, the way constructions are seen, and in terms of processing. This paper discusses all these
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Paggio, Patrizia. "The information structure of Danish grammar constructions." Nordic Journal of Linguistics 32, no. 1 (2009): 137–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0332586509002066.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper addresses the issue of how information structure can be accounted for in a formal grammar of Danish. Three information structure features – topic, focus and background – are discussed, and it is shown how they are instantiated in a number of different grammatical constructions from a corpus of spoken Danish. Prosodic, syntactic and information structure constraints characterising the various constructions are represented as typed feature structures following Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG), and the constructions themselves are ordered in a type hierarchy. The proposed ap
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

HERZIG SHEINFUX, LIVNAT, NURIT MELNIK, and SHULY WINTNER. "Representing argument structure." Journal of Linguistics 53, no. 04 (2016): 701–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226716000189.

Full text
Abstract:
Existing approaches to the representation of argument structure in grammar tend to focus either on semantics or on syntax. Our goal in this paper is to strike the right balance between the two levels by proposing an analysis that maintains the independence of the syntactic and semantic aspects of argument structure, and, at the same time, captures the interplay between the two levels. Our proposal is set in the context of the development of a large-scale grammar of Modern Hebrew within the framework of Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG). Consequently, an additional challenge it faces
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Getahun, Amare. "The structure of Argobba nominal phrase." Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 39, no. 2 (2018): 127–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/jall-2018-0011.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This paper analyzes the internal structure of Argobba nominal phrase in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) formalism. Argobba is a seriously endangered Semitic language in Ethiopia. Unlike its sister languages in the Ethio-Semitic subfamily, Argobba nouns qualified by a demonstrative, possessive pronoun and genitive NP bear a definite article. It is argued in this paper that the definite article is not an independent syntactic element, but an affix, which is attached to indefinite nouns lexically. It is argued that the derivation of Argobba definite common nouns is captured b
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Borsley, Robert D. "An HPSG approach to Welsh." Journal of Linguistics 25, no. 2 (1989): 333–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226700014134.

Full text
Abstract:
Welsh differs from English in a number of ways. The most obvious point is that it is a VSO language, but it also has distinctive agreement phenomena and clitics. For this reason, it is natural to ask of any theory of syntax that has been developed primarily on the basis of English: how can it handle Welsh? Welsh has had fairly extensive attention within the Government-Binding theory (see, for example, Harlow, 1981; Sproat, 1985; Sadler, 1988, and Hendrick, 1988). It has also had some attention within Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar (GPSG) (see Harlow, 1983; Borsley, 1983; 1988a). In this
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Bjerre, Anne. "Danish non-specific free relatives." Nordic Journal of Linguistics 37, no. 1 (2014): 5–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0332586514000043.

Full text
Abstract:
Within the Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) community, one part of the Base Hypothesis concerning free relatives proposed by Bresnan & Grimshaw (1978) has gained wide support, namely that free relatives are headed by the wh-phrase. The second part of the hypothesis is that the wh-phrase is base-generated, and this has not gained support. In this paper, we will consider a subset of free relative constructions, i.e. non-specific free relatives, and provide support for this second part, restated in HPSG terms as a claim that there is no filler–gap relation between a free relative p
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

McFetridge, Paul, Fred Popowich, and Dan Fass. "An analysis of compounds in HPSG (Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar) for database queries." Data & Knowledge Engineering 20, no. 2 (1996): 195–209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0169-023x(96)00033-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

VAN EYNDE, FRANK. "Sign-Based Construction Grammar: A guided tour." Journal of Linguistics 52, no. 1 (2015): 194–217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226715000341.

Full text
Abstract:
Sign-Based Construction Grammar (sbcg) is, on the one hand, a formalized version of Berkeley Construction Grammar (bcg), and, on the other hand, a further development of constructionist Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (hpsg). The volume edited by Hans Boas and Ivan Sag is the first book length presentation of the framework. Its centerpiece is a 130-page synopsis of the theory by Ivan Sag. The other contributions to the volume provide background, justification, case studies, an extension to diachronic syntax and a presentation of the FrameNet Constructicon. This review gives a guided tour
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Meurers, W. Detmar. "On Expressing Lexical Generalizations in HPSG." Nordic Journal of Linguistics 24, no. 2 (2001): 161–217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/033258601753358605.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper investigates the status of the lexicon and the possibilities for expressing lexical generalizations in the paradigm of Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG). We illustrate that the architecture readily supports the use of implicational principles to express generalizations over a class of word objects. A second kind of lexical generalizations expressing relations between classes of words is often expressed in terms of lexical rules. We show how lexical rules can be integrated into the formal setup for HPSG developed by King (1989, 1994), investigate a lexical rule specificatio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

PAOLILLO, JOHN C. "Formalizing formality: an analysis of register variation in Sinhala." Journal of Linguistics 36, no. 2 (2000): 215–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226700008148.

Full text
Abstract:
Variation in language on the basis of formality (register variation) is often neglected both in grammatical descriptions and in sociolinguistic analyses. I demonstrate here that in Sinhala, and perhaps in other diglossic languages, register variation in syntax cannot be ignored. In a Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) analysis based on a corpus of naturally occurring Sinhala texts, I propose an analysis of register variation in which the syntax of all observed registers is accounted for within a single grammar. I further explain how the approach to register variation developed here ca
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

KOENIG, JEAN-PIERRE. "Any questions left? Review of Ginzburg & Sag's Interrogative investigations." Journal of Linguistics 40, no. 1 (2004): 131–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226703002354.

Full text
Abstract:
For better or worse, linguistics is rife with frameworks, each with its own ethos. Two important aspects of Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar's are: (1) the search for an explicit and exhaustive account of the intricate syntactic and semantic facts that constitute one's grammar and (2) the hypothesis that general/universal and specific aspects of one's grammar are not qualitatively distinct. This book stands as a superb example of this ethos. It illustrates the fecundity of Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (henceforth, HPSG) as a framework and advances our knowledge of the syntax and se
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Markantonatou, Stella, and Bjarne Ørsnes. "Group adjectives." Journal of Greek Linguistics 3, no. 1 (2002): 139–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/jgl.3.07ors.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis paper attempts a comparative syntactic study of group adjectives in Danish, English and Modern Greek. The central problem with group adjectives is to account for the contrast between their seemingly argumental behaviour and their inability to introduce new referents into the discourse. We propose a novel analysis whereby group adjectives form a weakly lexical structure with their head noun and modify the first argument of the argument structure of the head noun while the argument itself remains unexpressed. If this analysis is right, it lends support to the representation of argum
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Faber, Andie, Luiz Amaral, and Marcus Maia. "Pronominal Feature Re-assembly: L1 and L2 Pronoun Resolution of Spanish Epicene and Common Gender Antecedents." Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 14, no. 2 (2021): 281–320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/shll-2021-2046.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In this paper, we propose the implementation of a full-fledged feature-based lexicalist syntactic theory as a way to represent the possible configurations of features in the learner’s interlanguage and formalize a theory of acquisition based in feature reassembly. We describe gender agreement pronominal coindexation in Spanish using Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) and use it to analyze the results of a self-paced reading test with L1 and L2 speakers. We find that the specification of the gender feature value at the syntactic level in epicene antecedents facilitates pronomi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Mellow, J. Dean. "Connectionism, HPSG signs and SLA representations: specifying principles of mapping between form and function." Second Language Research 20, no. 2 (2004): 131–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1191/0267658304sr234oa.

Full text
Abstract:
A current limitation of the connectionist approach to second language acquisition (SLA) research is that it does not, to my knowledge, include complex linguistic representations. This article proposes a partial solution to this limitation by motivating and illustrating specific analyses that utilize the sign-based representations developed within Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG). To motivate the proposed representations, the article applies them to an analysis of four types of mappings between form and function: one-to-one, primed redundancy, nonprimed redundancy and polyfunctional.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

MELNIK, NURIT. "Raising, inversion and agreement in modern Hebrew." Journal of Linguistics 53, no. 1 (2015): 147–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226715000444.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper focuses on the interaction between raising, subject–verb inversion and agreement in Modern Hebrew. It identifies, alongside ‘standard’ (i.e., English-like) subject-to-subject raising, two additional patterns where the embedded subject appears post-verbally. In one, the raising predicate exhibits long-distance agreement with the embedded subject, while in the other, a colloquial variant, it is marked with impersonal (3sm) agreement. The choice between the three raising constructions in the language is shown to be solely dependent on properties of the embedded clause. The data are dis
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

SAMVELIAN, POLLET. "A (phrasal) affix analysis of the Persian Ezafe." Journal of Linguistics 43, no. 3 (2007): 605–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226707004781.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper discusses the status of the Ezafe particle -(y)e in Persian and provides an affixal analysis of the Ezafe, formalized within Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG). The Ezafe, a feature of certain Western Iranian languages, is realized as an enclitic and links the head noun to its modifiers and to the possessor NP. The latter follow the head and are linked to one another by the Ezafe. On the basis of crucial empirical facts that have never been discussed in previous studies, I argue that the Ezafe is best regarded as an affix attaching to nominal heads (nouns, adjectives and so
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

HRISTOV, BOZHIL P. "The atoms of language." Journal of Linguistics 51, no. 3 (2015): 644–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226715000225.

Full text
Abstract:
The first part of this review article summarises and evaluates the contents of the book, attempting to do justice to the wealth of perspectives it offers. The book considers phonological, morphological, syntactic and semantic features from the vantage points of approaches as diverse as typology, computational linguistics and formal theories like Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) and Minimalism. The second part of the article discusses several of the unifying threads that run through the volume, including the internal and cross-linguistic validity and correspondence of features, as we
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Gunji, Takao, Carl Pollard, and Ivan A. Sag. "Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar." Language 72, no. 2 (1996): 417. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/416665.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Culy, Christopher, John Nerbonne, Klaus Netter, and Carl Pollard. "German in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar." Language 73, no. 2 (1997): 401. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/416033.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Weininger, Markus J. "Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar: Eine Einführung." Informationen Deutsch als Fremdsprache 36, no. 2-3 (2009): 263–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/infodaf-2009-2-361.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Sciarini-Gourianova, Natalie. "Slavic in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (review)." Language 78, no. 2 (2002): 348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/lan.2002.0131.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Kurtz, Gunde. "Deutsche Syntax deklarativ. Head- Driven Phrase Structure Grammar für das Deutsche." Informationen Deutsch als Fremdsprache 28, no. 2-3 (2001): 285–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/infodaf-2001-2-371.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Венкова [Venkova], Цветомира [TSvetomira]. "На кръстопътя на езиковите теории: глаголната комбинаторика". Slavia Meridionalis 15 (25 вересня 2015): 122–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.11649/sm.2015.011.

Full text
Abstract:
At the crossroads of linguistic theories: Verb combinatoricsThis paper discusses the limitations of syntactic research conducted within a single theoretical framework. The basic claim is that theories have both distinctive and common features, which can be taken into consideration and some interesting results and ideas can be encoded in terms of the original theory. The discussion of the theory interactions is focused around a particular linguistic issue – the head element of the simple verb phrase. Three basic syntactic models are analyzed in regard to their treatment of the head element in t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

KIM, HYEREE. "Subcategorization inheritance in Old English P-V compounds." Journal of Linguistics 33, no. 1 (1997): 39–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226796006299.

Full text
Abstract:
This article deals with the relation between subcategorization and the notion head in OE [P-V] compounds. Many morphosyntactic properties such as category and morphological class percolate from the head to the mother. Subcategorization, however, can percolate to the mother from a nonhead as well as from the head. This is evident from the comparison of their case government properties. This paper shows that the process of ARGUMENT ATTRACTION developed within Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar permits a straightforward account of this phenomenon.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

VAN EYNDE, FRANK. "Regularity and idiosyncracy in the formation of nominals." Journal of Linguistics 54, no. 4 (2018): 823–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226718000129.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper explores the interaction of regularity and idiosyncracy in the formation of nominals. It treats both nominals whose formation is highly regular, such as red box, and nominals whose formation is rather idiosyncratic, such as the Big Mess Construction (bmc; so good a bargain) and the Binominal Noun Phrase Construction (bnpc; her nitwit of a husband). Both the bmc and the bnpc conform to productive patterns, but the proper place of those patterns in the grammar as a whole is not easy to identify. To rise to the challenge, we build on recent developments in Head-driven Phrase Structure
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

POLLARD, CARL J., and EUN JUNG YOO. "A unified theory of scope for quantifiers and wh-phrases." Journal of Linguistics 34, no. 2 (1998): 415–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226798007099.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper presents an analysis of quantifier and wh-operator scope in terms of a lexicalized theory of quantifier storage, within the framework of Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar. The proposed analysis provides an alternative to the derivational approach wherein quantifier scope is determined at a separate level of representation via movement, and shows how scope of quantifiers and wh-phrases (fronted or in situ) can be handled in a unified way in a constraint-based grammar. Lexicalization of quantifier storage offers an account of scope facts in raising and unbounded dependency construc
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

TROUSDALE, GRAEME, and DAVID ADGER. "Special issue on English dialect syntax." English Language and Linguistics 11, no. 2 (2007): 257–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1360674307002249.

Full text
Abstract:
This special volume is concerned with the syntax of nonstandard varieties of (mainly British) English, and how such syntactic variation is accounted for within a range of theoretical models. There has been a growing interest in the modelling of dialect syntax (a) in a number of languages and (b) in a number of syntactic theories (see, for instance, the research on syntactic microvariation in some Germanic languages in Barbiers, Cornips & van der Kelij, 2002, or the construction-based approach to variation in Leino & Östman, 2005). We have brought together five articles written in diffe
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Wechsler, Stephen. "Prepositional Phrases from the Twilight Zone." Nordic Journal of Linguistics 20, no. 2 (1997): 127–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s033258650000408x.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper proposes a treatment of optional prepositional phrases with a status intermediate between adjuncts and complements. The essential idea is simply that such PPs are semantically but not syntactically selected. It is argued that prepositions often assign an external theta role to a complement of the governing verb, subject to the heretofore unnoticed syntactic condition that such external arguments must be direct rather than oblique. This explains certain binding opacity effects and also greatly simplifies the subcategorization frames of verbs. The analysis is formulated in the framewo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Van Eynde, Frank. "Clustering and stranding in Dutch." Linguistics 57, no. 5 (2019): 1025–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling-2019-0023.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This paper has both a descriptive and a theoretical aim. The descriptive one is to demonstrate that the phenomenon of clustering is not limited to verbs, but that it also affects adpositions. The theoretical one is to develop a formal analysis that captures the common properties of verb clustering and adposition clustering. For that purpose we employ the framework of Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar. Both the descriptive and the theoretical part are backed up with quantitative data about the use of adposition clusters in a sample consisting of one million words of spoken Dutch and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

LESOURD, PHILIP S. "Raising and long-distance agreement in Passamaquoddy: A unified analysis." Journal of Linguistics 55, no. 2 (2018): 357–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226718000415.

Full text
Abstract:
This article presents an analysis of two constructions in the Eastern Algonquian language Passamaquoddy in which the position of the object of a verb of cognition (‘know’, ‘believe’, ‘remember’, ‘wonder about’, ‘suspect’) is linked, either by apparent raising or by apparent long-distance agreement, to a position within a clausal complement to the verb. The latter position may be arbitrarily deeply embedded. The analysis developed here, formulated in the framework of Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar, demonstrates that the two constructions in fact represent alternative realizations of ident
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Hudson, Richard A. "Zwicky on heads." Journal of Linguistics 23, no. 1 (1987): 109–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226700011051.

Full text
Abstract:
An interesting development in the last decade or so has been the increasing use that theoretical linguists have made of the notion ‘head’ – or rather, in order not to beg the question, of notions to which they have given the name ‘head’. The term has been around for a long time in linguistics, of course – for example Bloomfield uses it in relation to endocentric constructions (1933: 195), where the head is the daughter constituent which has the same distribution as the mother. Before that, Sweet had used ‘head-word’ to refer to any word to which another is subordinate (1891: 16, quoted in Matt
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Yoo, Eun-Jung. "A Lexical Approach to English Floating Quantifiers." Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 28, no. 1 (2002): 351. http://dx.doi.org/10.3765/bls.v28i1.3850.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper investigates how the syntactic and semantic characteristics of floating quantifiers (FQ) in English can be explained within the framework of the Head Driven Phrase Structure Grammar. Previous analyses treat FQs either basically as NP quantifiers (Postal 1974, Maling 1976, Sportiche 1988, Mccawley 1998), or as VP quantifiers that are syntactically and semantically distinct from NP quantifiers (Dowty & Brodie 1984). In contrast, this paper proposes an analysis in which FQs are base-generated VP modifiers as in Dowty & Brodie, but their logical (or semantic) contributions are m
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

MAKINO, SHOZO, AKINORI ITO, MITSURU ENDO, and KEN’ITI KIDO. "A CONTINUOUS SPEECH RECOGNITION SYSTEM USING A MODIFIED LVQ2 METHOD AND A DEPENDENCY GRAMMAR WITH SEMANTIC CONSTRAINTS." International Journal of Pattern Recognition and Artificial Intelligence 08, no. 01 (1994): 197–213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0218001494000097.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper describes an overview of a continuous speech recognition system composed of an acoustic processor and a linguistic processor. The system deals with 843 conceptual words and 431 functional words. We have constructed an acoustic processor using a modified learning vector quantization method (MLVQ2) for phoneme recognition. The phoneme recognition score was 85.5% for 226 sentences uttered by two male speakers. The linguistic processor is composed of a processor for spotting bunsetsu units (i.e. units similar to a “phrase” in English) and a syntactic processor. The structure of the buns
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Van Eynde, Frank. "What participles are a mixture of." Linguistics 59, no. 4 (2021): 817–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ling-2021-0070.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract It is commonly assumed that participles show a mixture of verbal and adjectival properties, but the issue of how this mixed nature can best be captured is anything but settled. Analyses range from the purely adjectival to the purely verbal with various shades in between. This lack of consensus is at least partly due to the fact that participles are used in a variety of ways and that an analysis which fits one of them is not necessarily equally plausible for the other. In an effort to overcome the resulting fragmentation this paper proposes an analysis that covers all uses of the parti
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

ACKERMAN, FARRELL, ROBERT MALOUF, and JOHN MOORE. "Symmetrical objects in Moro: Challenges and solutions." Journal of Linguistics 53, no. 1 (2015): 3–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022226715000353.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper examines the syntactic and semantic behavior of object arguments in Moro, a Kordofanian language spoken in central Sudan. In particular, we focus on multiple object constructions (ditransitives, applicatives, and causatives) and show that these objects exhibit symmetrical syntactic behavior; e.g., any object can passivize or be realized as an object marker, and all can do so simultaneously. Moreover, we demonstrate that each object can bear any of the non-agentive roles in a verb’s semantic role inventory and that the resulting ambiguities are an entailment of symmetrical object con
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

WINTNER, SHULY. "John Nerbonne, Klaus Netter and Carl Pollard (editors), German in Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar. Stanford, CA: CSLI, 1994. ISBN 1 881 52630 5, £16.95/US$21.95 (paperback). £40.00/US$49.95 (hardback)." Natural Language Engineering 2, no. 1 (1996): 81–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1351324996221205.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

BORSLEY, ROBERT D. "On so-called transitive expletives in Belfast English." English Language and Linguistics 13, no. 3 (2009): 409–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1360674309990177.

Full text
Abstract:
Henry & Cottell (2007) argue that Belfast English (BE) sentences such asThere shouldn't anybody say thatandThere've lots of people passed the testare transitive expletive constructions (TECs) similar to those that are a feature of Icelandic. They propose that the difference between BE and Standard English (SE) is that whereas expletivethereis introduced in Spec vP in the latter it is introduced in Spec TP in the former. On the assumption that transitive subjects originate in Spec vP, this entails that expletivetherecannot co-occur with a transitive verb. While it is clear that BE is differ
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Aregbesola, MK, RA Ganiyu, SO Olabiyisi, and EO Omidiora. "A Comparative Study of Deep and Shallow Parsing Approaches to Automated Grammaticality Evaluation." Journal of Computer Science and Its Application 27, no. 1 (2020). http://dx.doi.org/10.4314/jcsia.v27i1.2.

Full text
Abstract:
The concept of automated grammar evaluation of natural language texts is one that has attracted significant interests in the natural language processing community. It is the examination of natural language text for grammatical accuracy using computer software. The current work is a comparative study of different deep and shallow parsing techniques that have been applied to lexical analysis and grammaticality evaluation of natural language texts. The comparative analysis was based on data gathered from numerous related works. Shallow parsing using induced grammars was first examined along with
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Nerbonne, John. "Deutsche Syntax deklarativ: Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar für das Deutsche. By Stefan Müller. (Linguistische Arbeiten, 394.) Tübingen: Niemeyer, 1999. Pp. xiii, 486. Paper. DM 186.00." Journal of Germanic Linguistics 13, no. 01 (2001). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1470542701003154.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!