To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Homographie.

Journal articles on the topic 'Homographie'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Homographie.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Laxén, J., J. M. Lavaur, and X. Aparicio. "Reconnaissance en traduction et homographie interlangue." Psychologie Française 56, no. 3 (2011): 161–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2011.07.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Borel, Jean-Pierre. "Image par homographie de mots de Christoffel." Bulletin of the Belgian Mathematical Society - Simon Stevin 8, no. 2 (2001): 241–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.36045/bbms/1102714171.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Alghamdi, Latifa. "The Challenges of Homographs among EFL Learners of the College of Foreign Languages." Journal of English Language Teaching and Applied Linguistics 3, no. 12 (2021): 01–08. http://dx.doi.org/10.32996/jeltal.2021.3.12.1.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to investigate the erroneous use (misuse) of homographs among EFL learners of the College of Foreign Languages to determine the misuse of homographs among EFL learners, find out the ability of EFL learners to distinguish homographs, and enrich learners with different usage of some words included in the homographs. The sample of the study consisted of 30 participants, 15 each of male and female students, randomly selected from 7th – 8th levels of the College of Foreign Languages in Albaha University, Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. The participants were assigned to (2) di
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Nurhayati, I. Nyoman Sudika, and Ratna Yulida Ashriany. "Homografi dalam Bahasa Sasak di Kelurahan Tanjung Kabupaten Lombok Timur." Jurnal Bastrindo 1, no. 1 (2020): 67–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.29303/jb.v1i1.11.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstrak: Permasalahan dalam penelitian ini, yaitu (1) Bagaimanakah wujud homografi dalam bahasa Sasak di Kelurahan Tanjung Kabupaten Lombok Timur? (2) Bagaimanakah distribusi homografi dalam pembentukan kalimat ? (3) Bagaimanakah makna leksem homografi ? Tujuan penelitian ini adalah untuk (1) mengidentifikasi wujud homografi dalam bahasa Sasak di Kelurahan Tanjung Kabupaten Lombok Timur, (2) mengidentifikasi distribusi homografi dalam pembentukan kalimat, (3) mendeskripsikan makna leksem homografi. Dalam pengumpulan data digunakan beberapa metode, yaitu: 1) metode introspeksi, 2) metode cakap,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Kaliszan, Jerzy. "Словарные глагольные омографы в русском языке". Studia Rossica Posnaniensia, № 42 (19 червня 2018): 149–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.14746/strp.2017.42.13.

Full text
Abstract:
The author analyzes the homographic potency of Russian verbs stressed differently in their infinitive forms, such as úкать — икáть, трýсить — трусúть, срéзать — срезáть etc. In contemporary Russian there are about 350 of these pairs. They are able to create over 3,200 pairs of predicative homographs and almost 27,000 pairs of attributive (participial) homographs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Rajnik, Eugeniusz. "Rzeczowniki homograficzne w języku duńskim − analiza morfologiczna i semantyczna." Studia Rossica Posnaniensia, no. 43 (November 26, 2018): 269–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.14746/strp.2018.43.20.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is a study of nouns with the same spelling but different grammatical gender (graphic homonymy, i.e. homography) in Modern Danish. The aim of the article is to analyze the grammatical and lexical homography of both neuter and non-neuter Danish nouns, which have been divided into two groups. The nouns in the first group occur in both grammatical genders with no difference in meaning (grammatical homographs), while the second group is comprised of nouns that do have different meanings based on their grammatical gender (lexical homographs). The author has also identified differences ba
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Ge, Dong Yuan, Xi Fan Yao, Wen Jiang Xiang, and Yuan Liu. "Solving Intrinsic Parameters of Camera Calibrated from Controlled Motion Sequences and Homography." Applied Mechanics and Materials 121-126 (October 2011): 4716–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.121-126.4716.

Full text
Abstract:
A camera calibration based on controlled motion sequences and homography is adopted. First condition of unique solution for constrained equation set is proofed. Then two sets three orthogonal motions are carried out for camera in manipulator, thus 6 homographic matrixes of plane are obtained at 7 view points, and thus the intrinsic parameters of camera are achieved. The experiments on real images validate our technology based on homography.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Ondrašovič, Milan, and Peter Tarábek. "Homography Ranking Based on Multiple Groups of Point Correspondences." Sensors 21, no. 17 (2021): 5752. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s21175752.

Full text
Abstract:
Homography mapping is often exploited to remove perspective distortion in images and can be estimated using point correspondences of a known object (marker). We focus on scenarios with multiple markers placed on the same plane if their relative positions in the world are unknown, causing an indeterminate point correspondence. Existing approaches may only estimate an isolated homography for each marker and cannot determine which homography achieves the best reprojection over the entire image. We thus propose a method to rank isolated homographies obtained from multiple distinct markers to selec
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

JOURAVLEV, OLESSIA, and DEBRA JARED. "Reading Russian–English homographs in sentence contexts: Evidence from ERPs." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17, no. 1 (2013): 153–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728913000205.

Full text
Abstract:
The current study investigated whether Russian–English bilinguals activate knowledge of Russian when reading English sentences. Russian and English share only a few letters, but there are some interlingual homographs (e.g., POT, which means “mouth” in Russian). Critical sentences were written such that the Russian meaning of the homographs fit the context. Sentences presented to participants contained either the English translation of the Russian meaning of a homograph, an interlingual homograph, or a control word (e.g., TO SEE TOM'S THROAT, THE DOCTOR ASKED TOM TO OPEN HIS MOUTH/POT/NET WIDEL
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Tuinman, Annelie. "Visuele Woordherkenning in Tweetaligen." Toegepaste Taalwetenschap in Artikelen 71 (January 1, 2004): 93–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/ttwia.71.09tui.

Full text
Abstract:
The bilingual visual word-recognition experiment described in the article focused on the recognition of cognates, noncognates, and interlingual homographs in more and less proficient bilinguals and was designed to test the following hypotheses: 1. There is a difference in reaction time to cognate, noncognate, and interlingual homograph words compared with control words. 2. There is a difference in reaction time to cognate, noncognate and interlingual homograph words, compared with control words, between bilinguals with different levels of proficiency in their second language. Both proficiency
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Kaliszan, Jerzy. "Rosyjska homografia gramatyczna." Folia Scandinavica Posnaniensia 20, no. 1 (2016): 133–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/fsp-2016-0031.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The article is devoted to the study of Russian grammatical homography (i.e. the relationship between paradigmatic forms with the same spelling but different pronunciation caused by different word stress) among nouns, adjectives and verbs. This type of homography may be illustrated by such pairs of inflectional forms of the same lexeme as руки - руки, большую - большую, смотрите - смотрите etc. The author’s aim is an attempt to describe all kinds of grammatical homographs existing in contemporary Russian.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

GREENBERG, SETH N., and JEAN SAINT-AUBIN. "Inter-lingual homograph letter detection in mixed language text: Persistent missing-letter effects and the effect of language switching." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11, no. 1 (2008): 111–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728907003240.

Full text
Abstract:
Heretofore, we learned that bilinguals better detected letters in inter-lingual homographs when the context language ascribed a content role to the homograph as compared to a function role. In previous work the target homographs appeared in passages that were of a single language. The present work investigated whether this letter detection pattern would hold if both languages were activated by intermixing languages in a passage. Results suggested that despite intermixing of languages that would excite competing function and content meanings, local sentence context was sufficient to engender a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Jared, Debra, and Carrie Szucs. "Phonological activation in bilinguals: Evidence from interlingual homograph naming." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5, no. 3 (2002): 225–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728902003024.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigated whether bilinguals simultaneously activate phonological representations from both of their languages when reading words in just one. The critical stimuli were interlingual homographs (e.g., PAIN) that were low in frequency in the target language of the study (English) and high in frequency in the nontarget language (French). Both English-French and French-English bilinguals were tested. In each experiment, participants named a block of English experimental words, a block of French filler words, and then a second block of English experimental words. In the first block of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

SMITS, ERICA, HEIKE MARTENSEN, TON DIJKSTRA, and DOMINIEK SANDRA. "Naming interlingual homographs: Variable competition and the role of the decision system." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 9, no. 3 (2006): 281–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s136672890600263x.

Full text
Abstract:
To investigate decision level processes involved in bilingual word recognition tasks, Dutch–English participants had to name Dutch–English homographs in English. In a stimulus list containing items from both languages, interlingual homographs yielded longer naming latencies, more Dutch responses, and more other errors in both response languages if they had a high-frequency Dutch reading. Dutch naming latencies were slower than or equally slow as English naming latencies. In a stimulus list containing only English words and homographs, there was no homograph effect in naming latencies, although
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Banga, Arina, Esther Hanssen, Robert Schreuder, and Anneke Neijt. "How subtle differences in orthography influence conceptual interpretation." Units of Language – Units of Writing 15, no. 2 (2012): 185–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/wll.15.2.04ban.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study investigates linguistic relativity. The units of writing investigated are e and en, which are used to represent units of language in Dutch, Frisian, and Afrikaans. Dutch has homographic forms in the plural suffix -en and the linking element of noun-noun compounds en. Frisian does not have homography of this kind, while Afrikaans has a different homography. This raises the question whether second language learners of Dutch consistently interpret the linking en in Dutch noun-noun compounds as plural in the way that native speakers do. Plurality ratings for Dutch modifiers obtai
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

HOVERSTEN, LIV J., and MATTHEW J. TRAXLER. "A time course analysis of interlingual homograph processing: Evidence from eye movements." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 19, no. 2 (2015): 347–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728915000115.

Full text
Abstract:
We recorded eye movements during natural reading to explore the influence of sentence context on bilingual word recognition. English monolinguals and Spanish–English bilinguals read sentences in English that biased either the English or the Spanish meaning of interlingual homographs. Shortly after encountering the homograph, the groups showed equivalent implausibility effects when its English meaning was incongruent with the preceding sentence context. No evidence for immediate homograph interference emerged during this period in the bilingual group. Only in later processing measures did group
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

GREENBERG, SETH N., and JEAN SAINT-AUBIN. "Letter detection for homographs with different meanings in different language texts." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7, no. 3 (2004): 241–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728904001622.

Full text
Abstract:
Tests of inter-lingual homographs that have different meanings across two languages support models postulating initial non-selective access to competing language representations, e.g. Bilingual Interactive Activation (BIA) model. Most such research assessed inter-lingual homographs in the absence of connected text. Here a letter detection paradigm was used that required subjects to detect letters in words in connected text. Prior work with this paradigm suggested that readers respond to only one interpretation of an intra-lingual homograph when detecting letters. Three experiments described he
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

DIJKSTRA, TON, HENK VAN JAARSVELD, and SJOERD TEN BRINKE. "Interlingual homograph recognition: Effects of task demands and language intermixing." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1, no. 1 (1998): 51–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728998000121.

Full text
Abstract:
A series of three lexical decision experiments showed that interlingual homographs may be recognized faster than, slower than, or as fast as monolingual control words depending on task requirements and language intermixing. In Experiment 1, Dutch bilingual participants performed an English lexical decision task including English/Dutch homographs, cognates, and purely English control words. Reaction times to interlingual homographs were unaffected by the frequency of the Dutch reading and did not differ from monolingual controls. In contrast, cognates were recognized faster than controls. In Ex
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Kaliszan, Jerzy. "Омографические триады в русском языке". Studia Rossica Posnaniensia, № 41 (20 червня 2018): 315–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.14746/strp.2016.41.28.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is devoted to the study of Russian homographic rows consisting of three components, such as ворона (gen. and acc. sg. of ворон ‘raven’) — ворона (nom. sg.) ‘crow’ — ворона (short form of вороной ‘black’), вертела (gen. sg. of вертел ‘spit for roasting’) — вертела (nom. and acc. pl. of вертел) — вертела (past of вертеть ‘to turn’), здорово (adv.) ‘greatly, superbly; hard, strongly’ — здорово (short neut. form of здоровый I ‘healthy’) — здорово (short neut. form of здоровый II ‘tremendous, hefty’). The author has analyzed and classified 70 such three-component groups of homographs re
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Poort, Eva D., and Jennifer M. Rodd. "Towards a distributed connectionist account of cognates and interlingual homographs: evidence from semantic relatedness tasks." PeerJ 7 (May 16, 2019): e6725. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6725.

Full text
Abstract:
Background Current models of how bilinguals process cognates (e.g., “wolf”, which has the same meaning in Dutch and English) and interlingual homographs (e.g., “angel”, meaning “insect’s sting” in Dutch) are based primarily on data from lexical decision tasks. A major drawback of such tasks is that it is difficult—if not impossible—to separate processes that occur during decision making (e.g., response competition) from processes that take place in the lexicon (e.g., lateral inhibition). Instead, we conducted two English semantic relatedness judgement experiments. Methods In Experiment 1, high
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Carvalho, Paula. "Grammaires de levée d’ambiguïtés entre noms et adjectifs." Ambiguity 24, no. 1 (2001): 127–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/li.24.1.07car.

Full text
Abstract:
Summary Most of lexical ambiguity caused by homography can only be resolved by the syntactic analysis of the structures where the homographs appear. Thus, in order to resolve ambiguity caused by the homography between nouns and adjectives, we constructed for Portuguese formal grammars that describe noun phrase structures with adjectives. The formal properties of about 2,000 adjectives have been studied and, according to their behavior, they have been integrated in different classes and sub-classes. The syntactic and distributional properties of those adjectives were coded and associated to the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Hermena, Ehab W., Sana Bouamama, Simon P. Liversedge, and Denis Drieghe. "Does diacritics‐based lexical disambiguation modulate word frequency, length, and predictability effects? An eye‐movements investigation of processing Arabic diacritics." PLOS ONE 16, no. 11 (2021): e0259987. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0259987.

Full text
Abstract:
In Arabic, a predominantly consonantal script that features a high incidence of lexical ambiguity (heterophonic homographs), glyph-like marks called diacritics supply vowel information that clarifies how each consonant should be pronounced, and thereby disambiguate the pronunciation of consonantal strings. Diacritics are typically omitted from print except in situations where a particular homograph is not sufficiently disambiguated by the surrounding context. In three experiments we investigated whether the presence of disambiguating diacritics on target homographs modulates word frequency, le
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Wojan, Katarzyna. "Homonimika rosyjska jako przedmiot zainteresowania polskich leksykologów i leksykografów. Na marginesie najnowszych prac Jerzego Kaliszana." Studia Rossica Posnaniensia, no. 43 (November 26, 2018): 327–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.14746/strp.2018.43.26.

Full text
Abstract:
The text is dedicated to Professor Jerzy Kaliszan, an outstanding Polish researcher of homonymy in the Russian language, the author of significant monographs and the first Polish dictionary of Russian homographs. The article is a review of Polish academic literature on Russian homonymy including both intra- and interlinguistic aspects (relations between Polish and Russian). In the first part, the author outlines the most important theoretical and lexicographical studies of Polish scholars. The second part is devoted to a description of Jerzy Kaliszan’s achievements in the field of lexical and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Nievas, Francisco, Fernando Justicia, José J. Cañas, and M. Teresa Bajo. "Lexical Processing of Ambiguous Words: Dominance or Associative Strength?" Spanish Journal of Psychology 8, no. 2 (2005): 157–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1138741600005059.

Full text
Abstract:
Four experiments examined the role of meaning frequency (dominance) and associative strength (measured by associative norms) in the processing of ambiguous words in isolation. Participants made lexical decisions to targets words that were associates of the more frequent (dominant) or less frequent (subordinate) meaning of a homograph prime. The first two experiments investigated the role of associative strength at long SOAs (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony) (750 ms.), showing that meaning is facilitated by the targets' associative strength and not by their dominance. The last two experiments traced
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Ivanova, Natalya, and Rimma Kuzmina. "Homographic Proper Names: On Peculiarities of Pronunciation Registration." Vestnik Volgogradskogo gosudarstvennogo universiteta. Serija 2. Jazykoznanije, no. 2 (May 2020): 98–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.15688/jvolsu2.2020.2.9.

Full text
Abstract:
The article considers phonetic variants of English proper names that comprise homographic groups, and some impairments during their registration in pronouncing dictionaries. The authors present the results of lexicographic and comparative analysis of units extracted from two pronouncing dictionaries: "Cambridge English Pronouncing Dictionary" by D. Jones and "Longman Pronunciation Dictionary" by J. Wells using the continuous sampling method. Particular attention is focused on the methods of registration and completeness of phonetic notation of homographs pronunciation. A number of peculiaritie
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Alqraini, Faisl M. "Using context clues to teach homographs to d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing students in Saudi Arabia." South African Journal of Education 41, Supplement 2 (2021): S1—S10. http://dx.doi.org/10.15700/saje.v41ns2a2024.

Full text
Abstract:
Teaching a homograph by using context clues is more effective than just teaching vocabulary separately. The goal of the study reported on here was to teach 12 homographs to d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing (d/Dhh) students in the sixth grade by applying metacognitive skills to understand the meanings and contexts in sentences. A single case design (multiple probe design across subjects) was employed to achieve the goal of this study with 2 profoundly deaf students in the sixth grade. From baseline to follow-up, the study was completed in 4 weeks. The results show that d/Dhh students encountered chal
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Mohammed, H. M., and N. El-Sheimy. "FEATURE MATCHING ENHANCEMENT OF UAV IMAGES USING GEOMETRIC CONSTRAINTS." ISPRS - International Archives of the Photogrammetry, Remote Sensing and Spatial Information Sciences XLII-1 (September 26, 2018): 307–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/isprs-archives-xlii-1-307-2018.

Full text
Abstract:
<p><strong>Abstract.</strong> Preliminary matching of image features is based on the distance between their descriptors. Matches are further filtered using RANSAC, or a similar method that fits the matches to a model; usually the fundamental matrix and rejects matches not belonging to that model. There are a few issues with this scheme. First, mismatches are no longer considered after RANSAC rejection. Second, RANSAC might fail to detect an accurate model if the number of outliers is significant. Third, a fundamental matrix model could be degenerate even if the matches are al
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Hanssen, Esther, Arjen Versloot, Eric Hoekstra, Arina Banga, Anneke Neijt, and Robert Schreuder. "Morphological variation in the speech of Frisian-Dutch bilinguals." Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5, no. 3 (2015): 356–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/lab.5.3.03han.

Full text
Abstract:
In standard Dutch, the plural suffix -en is homographic and homophonic with the linking suffix -en (boek+en “books”, boek+en+kast “bookcase”), both being pronounced as schwa. In Frisian, there is neither homography nor homophony (boek+en “books”, pronounced with syllabic nasal; boek+e+kast “bookcase”, pronounced with a linking schwa). Seeing that many areas of Frisian grammar are subject to interference from Dutch, we investigated whether Frisian-Dutch bilinguals exhibit interference from Dutch with respect to the linking suffix during Frisian speech production. Two types of Frisian-Dutch bili
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Woud, Marcella L., Elske Salemink, Thomas E. Gladwin, et al. "Alcohol Homograph Priming in Alcohol-Dependent Inpatients." SUCHT 62, no. 4 (2016): 217–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/0939-5911/a000432.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Aim: Alcohol dependency is characterized by alcohol-related interpretation biases (IBs): Individuals with high levels of alcohol consumption generate more alcohol-related than alcohol-unrelated interpretations in response to ambiguous alcohol-related cues. However, a response bias could be an alternative account, meaning that individuals with high levels of alcohol consumption generate more alcohol-related IBs because of a greater baseline tendency to endorse alcohol-related responses. Methods: To test this alternative explanation, the present study employed a homograph-priming task,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

CASTNER, JOANNA E., DAVID A. COPLAND, PETER A. SILBURN, TERRY J. COYNE, FELICITY SINCLAIR, and HELEN J. CHENERY. "Subthalamic stimulation affects homophone meaning generation in Parkinson's disease." Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 14, no. 5 (2008): 890–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617708081046.

Full text
Abstract:
Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in individuals with Parkinson's disease (PD) has often been associated with reduced verbal fluency performance. This study aimed to directly assess semantic switching as a function of STN stimulation in PD participants with the Homophone Meaning Generation Test (HMGT). Seventeen participants with PD who had received STN DBS completed the HMGT in on and off stimulation conditions. Twenty-one non-neurologically impaired participants acted as controls. PD participants (in both on and off stimulation conditions) generated significantly
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Azmi, Aqil M., Rehab M. Alnefaie, and Hatim A. Aboalsamh. "Light Diacritic Restoration to Disambiguate Homographs in Modern Arabic Texts." ACM Transactions on Asian and Low-Resource Language Information Processing 21, no. 3 (2022): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3486675.

Full text
Abstract:
Diacritic restoration (also known as diacritization or vowelization) is the process of inserting the correct diacritical markings into a text. Modern Arabic is typically written without diacritics, e.g., newspapers. This lack of diacritical markings often causes ambiguity, and though natives are adept at resolving, there are times they may fail. Diacritic restoration is a classical problem in computer science. Still, as most of the works tackle the full (heavy) diacritization of text, we, however, are interested in diacritizing the text using a fewer number of diacritics. Studies have shown th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Bar-On, Amalia, Elitzur Dattner, and Oriya Braun-Peretz. "Resolving homography: The role of post-homograph context in reading aloud ambiguous sentences in Hebrew." Applied Psycholinguistics 40, no. 6 (2019): 1405–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716419000316.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis study examined whether the context immediately succeeding a heterophonic-homographic word (ht-homographic) plays a role in ambiguity resolution during voiced reading of Hebrew. A pretest was designed to find the preferred alternatives of 12 ht-homographic words: 20 adult subjects completed truncated sentences, each ending with a homographic word, preceded by a context allowing for both of its alternatives to be read. Following the pretest, each word was embedded in four research conditions determined by post-homographic context (keeping preceding context constant): two adjacent re
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Rubin, Jacques. "Applications of a Particular Four-Dimensional Projective Geometry to Galactic Dynamics." Galaxies 6, no. 3 (2018): 83. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/galaxies6030083.

Full text
Abstract:
Relativistic localizing systems that extend relativistic positioning systems show that pseudo-Riemannian space-time geometry is somehow encompassed in a particular four-dimensional projective geometry. The resulting geometric structure is then that of a generalized Cartan space (also called Cartan connection space) with projective connection. The result is that locally non-linear actions of projective groups via homographies systematically induce the existence of a particular space-time foliation independent of any space-time dynamics or solutions of Einstein’s equations for example. In this a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Je, Changsoo, and Hyung-Min Park. "Homographic p-norms: Metrics of homographic image transformation." Signal Processing: Image Communication 39 (November 2015): 185–201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.image.2015.08.009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Lee, Dongjin, Youngjoo Kim, and Hyochoong Bang. "Vision-aided terrain referenced navigation for unmanned aerial vehicles using ground features." Proceedings of the Institution of Mechanical Engineers, Part G: Journal of Aerospace Engineering 228, no. 13 (2014): 2399–413. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0954410013517804.

Full text
Abstract:
A vision-aided terrain referenced navigation (VATRN) approach is addressed for autonomous navigation of unmanned aerial vehicles (UAVs) under GPS-denied conditions. A typical terrain referenced navigation (TRN) algorithm blends inertial navigation data with measured terrain information to estimate vehicle’s position. In this paper, a low-cost inertial navigation system (INS) for UAVs is supplemented with a monocular vision-aided navigation system and terrain height measurements. A point mass filter based on Bayesian estimation is employed as a TRN algorithm. Homograpies are established to esti
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Jiang, Ming Xin, and Hong Yu Wang. "A Robust Tracking Algorithm in Crowded Environment." Applied Mechanics and Materials 157-158 (February 2012): 1336–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.157-158.1336.

Full text
Abstract:
Reliable tracking of multiple people in cluttered or complex situations is a challenging visual surveillance problem since the high density of objects results in occlusion. In order to deal with this problem, multiple synchronized cameras were mounted at various heights in our experiment. To ensure the existence of the homography, it is necessary to assume that different views share a common dominant ground plane. Thus, corresponding people have to be located within the multi-camera surveillance system, accurate multi-people localizing is an important prerequisite to reliable tracking. In this
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Coghetto, Roland. "Group of Homography in Real Projective Plane." Formalized Mathematics 25, no. 1 (2017): 55–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/forma-2017-0005.

Full text
Abstract:
Summary Using the Mizar system [2], we formalized that homographies of the projective real plane (as defined in [5]), form a group. Then, we prove that, using the notations of Borsuk and Szmielew in [3] “Consider in space ℝℙ2 points P1, P2, P3, P4 of which three points are not collinear and points Q1,Q2,Q3,Q4 each three points of which are also not collinear. There exists one homography h of space ℝℙ2 such that h(Pi) = Qi for i = 1, 2, 3, 4.” (Existence Statement 52 and Existence Statement 53) [3]. Or, using notations of Richter [11] “Let [a], [b], [c], [d] in ℝℙ2 be four points of which no th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Matkowski, J. "Homographic commuting functions." Journal of Difference Equations and Applications 21, no. 11 (2015): 1082–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10236198.2015.1072520.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Laudanna, Alessandro, William Badecker, and Alfonso Caramazza. "Priming homographic stems." Journal of Memory and Language 28, no. 5 (1989): 531–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596x(89)90011-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Elston-Güttler, Kerrie E., and Thomas C. Gunter. "Fine-tuned: Phonology and Semantics Affect First- to Second-language Zooming In." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, no. 1 (2009): 180–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21015.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigate how L1 phonology and semantics affect processing of interlingual homographs by manipulating language context before, and auditory input during, a visual experiment in the L2. Three experiments contained German–English homograph primes (gift = German “poison”) in English sentences and was performed by German (L1) learners of English (L2). Both reaction times and event-related brain potentials were measured on targets reflecting the German meaning of the interlingual homograph. In Experiment 1, participants viewed a pre-experiment English film, then half of the participants (n = 1
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Share, David L., and Amalia Bar-On. "Learning to Read a Semitic Abjad: The Triplex Model of Hebrew Reading Development." Journal of Learning Disabilities 51, no. 5 (2017): 444–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219417718198.

Full text
Abstract:
We introduce a model of Hebrew reading development that emphasizes both the universal and script-specific aspects of learning to read a Semitic abjad. At the universal level, the study of Hebrew reading acquisition offers valuable insights into the fundamental dilemmas of all writing systems—balancing the competing needs of the novice versus the expert reader (Share, 2008). At the script-specific level, pointed Hebrew initially employs supplementary vowel signs, providing the beginning reader a consistent, phonologically well-specified script while helping the expert-to-be unitize words and mo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Zeinik-Manor, L., and M. Irani. "Multiview constraints on homographies." IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence 24, no. 2 (2002): 214–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/34.982901.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Zhou, Qiang, and Xin Li. "STN-Homography: Direct Estimation of Homography Parameters for Image Pairs." Applied Sciences 9, no. 23 (2019): 5187. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/app9235187.

Full text
Abstract:
Estimating a 2D homography from a pair of images is a fundamental task in computer vision. Contrary to most convolutional neural network-based homography estimation methods that use alternative four-point homography parameterization schemes, in this study, we directly estimate the 3 × 3 homography matrix value. We show that after coordinate normalization, the magnitude difference and variance of the elements of the normalized 3 × 3 homography matrix is very small. Accordingly, we present STN-Homography, a neural network based on spatial transformer network (STN), to directly estimate the norma
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Altarriba, Jeanette, and Jennifer L. Gianico. "Lexical Ambiguity Resolution Across Languages: A Theoretical and Empirical Review." Experimental Psychology 50, no. 3 (2003): 159–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1026//1617-3169.50.3.159.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Words that involve completely different meanings across languages but possess significant overlap in form are referred to as homographic noncognates or interlexical homographs (e.g., red is a color word in English but means “net” in Spanish). An important question in the investigation of the processing of these words is whether or not both meaning and form are integral to their representation leading to language-specific processing of these items. In contrast, some theories have been put forth indicating that the processing of these words is nonselective with regards to language. Sim
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Jain, Vanita, Qiming Wu, Shivam Grover, et al. "Generating Bird’s Eye View from Egocentric RGB Videos." Wireless Communications and Mobile Computing 2021 (November 8, 2021): 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2021/7479473.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper, we present a method for generating bird’s eye video from egocentric RGB videos. Working with egocentric views is tricky since such the view is highly warped and prone to occlusions. On the other hand, a bird’s eye view has a consistent scaling in at least the two dimensions it shows. Moreover, most of the state-of-the-art systems for tasks such as path prediction are built for bird’s eye views of the subjects. We present a deep learning-based approach that transfers the egocentric RGB images captured from a dashcam of a car to bird’s eye view. This is a task of view translation,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Donald, Paul. "Homographic Cartilage in Facial Implantation." Facial Plastic Surgery 8, no. 03 (1992): 157–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1064646.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Mylläri, Aleksandr. "Stability of expanding homographic configurations." ACM SIGSAM Bulletin 39, no. 4 (2005): 136–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1140378.1140386.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Chum, Ondřej, Tomáš Pajdla, and Peter Sturm. "The geometric error for homographies." Computer Vision and Image Understanding 97, no. 1 (2005): 86–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cviu.2004.03.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Li, Zheng Guo, Bo Wang, Yong Sheng Zhang, Xiao Chong Tong, Wei Can Meng, and Quan Fu Bao. "A Fast Orthoimage Mosaic Method for Application of Grid." Applied Mechanics and Materials 151 (January 2012): 685–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.151.685.

Full text
Abstract:
Traditional orthoimage mosaic methods do not perform well in computational speed and geometric precision. This paper proposed a fast orthoimage mosaic method for the application of grid. First of all, down-sample the original images and extracts feature, and adopt SANSAC to estimate the relative initial homography; second, refine homography matrix by Levenberg-Marquardt method and use the sparse bundle adjustment method to estimate the precise homography matrix; Third, passed the homography matrix to the original level of image by the homography relationship of the down-sampling and original i
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Cho, Tai-Hoon, and Hyun-Min Kang. "Gaze Tracking Using a Modified Starburst Algorithm and Homography Normalization." Journal of the Korea Institute of Information and Communication Engineering 18, no. 5 (2014): 1162–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.6109/jkiice.2014.18.5.1162.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!