Academic literature on the topic 'Hyperphantasia'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Hyperphantasia.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Hyperphantasia"

1

Duch, Włodzisław. "Imagery Agnosia and its Phenomenology." Roczniki Psychologiczne 24, no. 3-4 (2022): 307–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.18290/rpsych21242-5s.

Full text
Abstract:
Lack of vivid sensory imagery has recently become an active subject of research, under the name of aphantasia. Extremely vivid imagery, or hyperphantasia, is at the other end of the spectrum of individual differences. While most research has focused on visual imagery in this paper I argue that from a neuropsychological perspective this phenomenon is much more widespread, and should be categorized as imagery sensory agnosia. After over twenty years of learning to play music phenomenology of auditory imagery agnosia is described from the first-person perspective. Reflections on other forms of imagery agnosia and deficits of autobiographical memories are presented and a hypothesis about putative brain processes that can account for such phenomena is discussed. Extreme individual differences in imagery and in autobiographical memory have implications for many fields of study, from consciousness research to education.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kristina, Šekrst. "Having the Foggiest Idea: A Gradual Account on Mental Images." Journal of NeuroPhilosophy 1, no. 2 (2022): 203–11. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7254024.

Full text
Abstract:
First described by Galton in 1880 and then remaining unnoticed for a century, recent investigations in neuroscience have shown that a condition called<em> aphantasia</em> appears in certain individuals, which causes them to be unable to experience visual mental imagery. Comparing aphantasia to <em>hyperphantasia</em> &ndash; i.e., photo-like memory &ndash; and considering the neurological basis of perceptual phenomena, we are revisiting Hume&#39;s division of perceptions into impressions and ideas. By showing different vivacities of mental phenomena and comparing them to neurological research, we are stating that not only impressions and ideas differ &quot;in the degrees of force and liveliness&quot;, but ideas and impressions amongst themselves as well. Such a gradual range of perceptions and mental images bears significant consequences for not only representational theory and historical interpretations but linguistics and semiotics as well.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Xie, Guo-Hui. "Phantasia, aphantasia and the spectrum subtypes of imagination." Asian Educational Therapist 1, no. 1 (2023): 47–55. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.15220838.

Full text
Abstract:
Imagination remains one of the few uncharted complex terrains of the human mind. The original word for imagination comes from the Greek word phantasia. When the prefix a- (which means &lsquo;without&rsquo;) is added to the Greek word, the term aphantasia comes into existence. In fact, aphantasia was first coined by one of the world&rsquo;s few foremost experts on imagery extremes, Adam Zeman, who is currently aprofessor of cognitive and behavioral neurology at the University of Exeter, UK. Aphantasia is described as a handicapping condition in which an individual is unable to visualize. The opposite of aphantasia is hyperphantasia (known as image-free thinking), which is the condition of having extremely vivid mental imagery. Between them, there is a spectrum of imagination-related types or subtypes. This short paper is loosely extracted from the author&rsquo;s recently published monograph The Spectrum of Imagination in Autism (Lambert Academic Publishing, 2023), which he has coauthored with Ming-Jie Deng, a postgraduate neuroscience candidature at the&nbsp;King&rsquo;s College, London, UK.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Palermo, Liana, Maddalena Boccia, Laura Piccardi, and Raffaella Nori. "Congenital lack and extraordinary ability in object and spatial imagery: An investigation on sub-types of aphantasia and hyperphantasia." Consciousness and Cognition 103 (August 2022): 103360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2022.103360.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Zeman, Adam. "18 The eye’s mind: perspectives on visual imagery." Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 91, no. 8 (2020): e8.1-e8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2020-bnpa.18.

Full text
Abstract:
Prof. Zeman trained in Medicine at Oxford University Medical School, after a first degree in Philosophy and Psychology, and later in Neurology in Oxford, at The National Hospital for Neurology in Queen Square, London and Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge. He moved to Edinburgh in 1996, as a Consultant and Senior Lecturer (later Reader) in the Department of Clinical Neurosciences and to the Peninsula Medical School (now University of Exeter Medical School) in September 2005 as Professor of Cognitive and Behavioural Neurology. His specialised clinical work is in cognitive and behavioural neurology, including neurological disorders of sleep.His main research interests are disorders of visual imagery and forms of amnesia occurring in epilepsy. He has an active background interest in the science and philosophy of consciousness, publishing a wide-ranging review of the field in Brain (2001; 124:1263–1289) and an accessible introduction to the subject for a general readership (Consciousness: a user’s guide, Yale University Press, 2002). In 2008 he published an introduction to neurology for the general reader, A Portrait of the Brain (Yale UP), and in 2012, Epilepsy and Memory (OUP) with Narinder Kapur and Marilyn Jones-Gotman. From 2007–2010 he was Chairman of the British Neuropsychiatry Association. He launched and continues to direct its training course in neuropsychiatry.For most of us visual imagery is a conspicuous ingredient of the imaginative experience which allows us to escape from the here and now into the past, the future and the worlds conceived by science and art. But there appears to be wide inter-individual variation in the vividness of visual imagery. Although the British psychologist Galton together with the Parisian neurologist Charcot and his psychiatrist colleague Cotard - recognised that some individuals may lack wakeful imagery entirely, the existence of ‘extreme imagery’ has been oddly neglected since this early work. In 2015 we coined the term ‘aphantasia’ to describe the lack of the mind’s eye, describing 21 individuals who reported a lifelong inability to visualise (Cortex, 2015;73:378–80). Since then we have heard from around 14,000 people, most reporting lifelong aphantasia, or its converse hyperphantasia, but also less common ‘acquired’ imagery loss resulting from brain injury or psychological disorder. Preliminary analyses suggests association between vividness extremes, occupational preference and reported abilities in face recognition and autobiographical memory. Many people with lifelong aphantasia nevertheless dream visually. Imagery in other modalities is variably affected. Extreme imagery appears to run in families more often than would be expected by chance. I will describe the findings of our recent pilot study of neuropsychological and brain imaging signatures of extreme imagery, and place our study of a- and hyper-phantasia in the context of the Eye’s Mind project, an interdisciplinary collaboration funded by the AHRC (http://medicine.exeter.ac.uk/research/neuroscience/theeyesmind/). In addition to our work on extreme imagery, we have reviewed the intellectual history of visual imagery (MacKisack et al, Frontiers in Psychology, 515:1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00515), undertaken a recent ALE meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of visualisation (Winlove et al, Cortex, 20182018; 105:4–25) and organised an exhibition of work by artists with extreme imagery vividness (Extreme Imagination: inside the mind’s eye Exeter University Press, 2018.)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Milton, Fraser, Jon Fulford, Carla Dance, et al. "Behavioral and Neural Signatures of Visual Imagery Vividness Extremes: Aphantasia versus Hyperphantasia." Cerebral Cortex Communications 2, no. 2 (2021). http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/texcom/tgab035.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Although Galton recognized in the 1880s that some individuals lack visual imagery, this phenomenon was mostly neglected over the following century. We recently coined the terms “aphantasia” and “hyperphantasia” to describe visual imagery vividness extremes, unlocking a sustained surge of public interest. Aphantasia is associated with subjective impairment of face recognition and autobiographical memory. Here we report the first systematic, wide-ranging neuropsychological and brain imaging study of people with aphantasia (n = 24), hyperphantasia (n = 25), and midrange imagery vividness (n = 20). Despite equivalent performance on standard memory tests, marked group differences were measured in autobiographical memory and imagination, participants with hyperphantasia outperforming controls who outperformed participants with aphantasia. Face recognition difficulties and autistic spectrum traits were reported more commonly in aphantasia. The Revised NEO Personality Inventory highlighted reduced extraversion in the aphantasia group and increased openness in the hyperphantasia group. Resting state fMRI revealed stronger connectivity between prefrontal cortices and the visual network among hyperphantasic than aphantasic participants. In an active fMRI paradigm, there was greater anterior parietal activation among hyperphantasic and control than aphantasic participants when comparing visualization of famous faces and places with perception. These behavioral and neural signatures of visual imagery vividness extremes validate and illuminate this significant but neglected dimension of individual difference.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Wright, David J., Matthew W. Scott, Sarah N. Kraeutner, et al. "An international estimate of the prevalence of differing visual imagery abilities." Frontiers in Psychology 15 (October 15, 2024). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1454107.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this research was to establish prevalence estimates for aphantasia, hypophantasia, typical imagery ability, and hyperphantasia in a large multi-national cohort. In Study 1, the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire was completed by 3,049 participants. Results indicated prevalence estimates of 1.2% for aphantasia, 3% for hypophantasia, 89.9% for typical imagery ability, and 5.9% for hyperphantasia. In Study 2, to replicate these findings in a larger sample, the Study 1 data were combined with openly available data from previous prevalence studies to create a total sample of 9,063 participants. Re-analysis of this data confirmed prevalence estimates of 0.9% for aphantasia, 3.3% for hypophantasia, 89.7% for typical imagery ability, and 6.1% for hyperphantasia. These robust and up-to-date estimates provide enhanced clarity to researchers regarding the prevalence of differing visual imagery abilities and provide a platform for future studies exploring the role of visual imagery in various cognitive and behavioral tasks.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Zeman, Adam. "Aphantasia and hyperphantasia: exploring imagery vividness extremes." Trends in Cognitive Sciences, March 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2024.02.007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Incekara, Fatih, and Jan Dirk Blom. "Carl Jung: a life on the edge of reality with hypnagogia, hyperphantasia, and hallucinations." Frontiers in Psychology 15 (March 7, 2024). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1358329.

Full text
Abstract:
Whether the Swiss psychiatrist Carl Jung (1875–1961) became psychotic after his mid-thirties is much debated. His recently published Black Books, a seven-volume journal, reveal new insights into this debate. Based on a phenomenological analysis of his self-reports in these books and in other writings, we here identify several types of anomalous perceptual experiences: hypnagogic-hypnopompic experiences, hyperphantasia, hallucinations, personifications, and sensed presence. We argue that these experiences were not indicative of a psychotic disorder, but rather stemmed from extremely vivid mental imagery, or hyperphantasia, a condition Jung’s contemporaries and later biographers were unable to take into account because it had not yet been conceptualised. Recently, the degree of vividness of mental imagery and its potential to become indistinguishable from regular sense perception has been the subject of extensive studies. Unknowingly, Jung may have foreshadowed this line of research with his psychoanalytic concept of reality equivalence, i.e., the substitution of an external world for an inner mental reality that he encountered in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. There is a need for future research to investigate the possible role of hyperphantasia in psychotic experiences, but to Jung, psychosis was ‘a failure to contain and comprehend’ the content of one’s experiences in the context of one’s own life, whereas he himself did manage to put the content of his perceptual experiences into context, to find meaning in them, and to share them with others - to great acknowledgement and acclaim.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Larner, AJ, AP Leff, and PC Nachev. "Phantasia, aphantasia, and hyperphantasia: empirical data and conceptual considerations." Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, July 2024, 105819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105819.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Hyperphantasia"

1

Akant, Sara Deniz. Hyperphantasia. Rescue Press, 2022.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography