Academic literature on the topic 'Interresponse intervals'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Interresponse intervals.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Interresponse intervals"
Kumai, Masayuki, and Kuniaki Sugai. "Relation between Synchronized and Self-Paced Response in Preschoolers' Rhythmic Movement." Perceptual and Motor Skills 85, no. 3_suppl (December 1997): 1327–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.85.3f.1327.
Full textHammond, Geoffrey, Yvette Bolton, Yvonne Plant, and Jocelyn Manning. "Hand Asymmetries in Interresponse Intervals During Rapid Repetitive Finger Tapping." Journal of Motor Behavior 20, no. 1 (March 1988): 67–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1988.10735433.
Full textSchallert, Timothy. "Brain stimulation and catecholaminergic drugs: A focus on self-selected response durations versus interresponse intervals." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, no. 1 (March 1985): 178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0002029x.
Full textLaRue, Jacues, Chantal Bard, Michelle Fleury, Normand Teasdale, Jacques Paillard, Robert Forget, and Yves Lamarre. "Is proprioception important for the timing of motor activities?" Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology 73, no. 2 (February 1, 1995): 255–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1139/y95-036.
Full textBowers, Matthew T., Jade Hill, and William L. Palya. "INTERRESPONSE TIME STRUCTURES IN VARIABLE-RATIO AND VARIABLE-INTERVAL SCHEDULES." Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 90, no. 3 (November 2008): 345–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2008.90-345.
Full textCleaveland, J. M. "INTERRESPONSE-TIME SENSITIVITY DURING DISCRETE-TRIAL AND FREE-OPERANT CONCURRENT VARIABLE-INTERVAL SCHEDULES." Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 72, no. 3 (November 1999): 317–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1999.72-317.
Full textTanno, Takayuki. "Response-bout analysis of interresponse times in variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules." Behavioural Processes 132 (November 2016): 12–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.09.001.
Full textWearden, J. H., and R. B. Clark. "Constraints on the process of interresponse-time reinforcement as the explanation of variable-interval performance." Behavioural Processes 20, no. 1-3 (December 1989): 151–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0376-6357(89)90020-x.
Full textInui, Nobuyuki. "Interactions of Speech and Manual Movement in a Syncopated Task." Perceptual and Motor Skills 105, no. 2 (October 2007): 447–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.105.2.447-457.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Interresponse intervals"
Reilly, Mark P. (Mark Peter). "The Effects of Interresponse Intervals on Behavioral Variability in Humans." Thesis, University of North Texas, 1993. https://digital.library.unt.edu/ark:/67531/metadc501037/.
Full textWade, Tammy R. "Differential reinforcement of fixed-interval interresponse times effects on choice /." Morgantown, W. Va. : [West Virginia University Libraries], 2002. http://etd.wvu.edu/templates/showETD.cfm?recnum=2453.
Full textTitle from document title page. Document formatted into pages; contains vii, 30 p. : ill. Includes abstract. Includes bibliographical references (p. 30).
Bitondi, Fernanda Rizzi. "Variabilidade comportamental e a seleção de uma sequência de baixa probabilidade inicial: comparando dois procedimentos." Pontifícia Universidade Católica de São Paulo, 2012. https://tede2.pucsp.br/handle/handle/16687.
Full textCoordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior
The aim of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of two procedures, Discrete Trial (DT) and Discrete Trial with Interresponse Interval (DT/IRI), on the production of behavioral variability in different conditions (directly reinforced and induced) and determine what condition, in each procedure, would facilitate the selection of an initial sequence of low probability. Were selected 18 college students with baseline performance measured by U-valeu, that was equal or less than 0,5. They were divided into the two procedures and subdivided into three groups with three participants (VAR, YOKE and CON). The task was to form images on the computer screen by pressing two keys (left and right) on lateral keyboards. The unit was four responses to the keys. In both procedures, a piece of the image, a tone and 0,5 seconds was used to reinforce sequences that attended the contingency s criterion, and a timeout of 1,0 second, if not. Only the DT/IRI had a 0,5 second interval after the first three responses of the sequence. The experiment had three phases. In Phase I, baseline, sequences was continuous reinforced (CRF). For two groups (DT- VAR and DT/IRI-VAR) in Phase 2, variability was direct reinforced on 15 sequences, mean that the less frequent and less recent sequence had more probability of been reinforced than the others, and a target sequence (less frequent sequence at baseline) was reinforced in CRF. In Phase 3, the distribution of reinforcement of the prior phase was yoked for the 15 sequences and another target sequence was reinforced in CRF. For the groups DT-YOKE and DT/IRI-YOKE, the phase s exposure was reversed, whereas in Phase 2 the distribution of reinforcement was yoked to the performance of the VAR groups. In groups DT-CON and DT/IRI-CON, only the target sequence was reinforced in both phases. The results showed that variability increased in the first session of Phase 2 for all participants. The DT-YOKE group showed the highest levels of variability within this procedure. In the DT/IRI procedure, this was true for the group DT/IRI-VAR. With regard to the selection of the target sequence the groups that had higher percentages of selection in all sessions were DT-CON and DT/IRI-VAR, respectively within each procedure. It was noted that both procedures produced the selection of the target sequence, but is seems that the use of IRI produced a more variable performance, when it was directly reinforced, allowing the selection of the target sequence to occur more rapidly
O objetivo do estudo foi avaliar a efetividade de dois procedimentos, Tentativa Discreta (TD) e Tentativa Discreta com Intervalo entre as Respostas (TD/IRI), na produção da variabilidade comportamental em diferentes condições (diretamente reforçada e induzida) e averiguar que condição, em cada procedimento, mais facilitaria a seleção de uma sequência de baixa probabilidade inicial. Selecionou-se 18 universitários com um desempenho na linha de base, medido pelo índice U, igual ou inferior a 0,5. Estes foram divididos nos dois procedimentos e subdivididos em três grupos com três participantes (VAR, ACO e CON). A tarefa consistia em formar figuras na tela do computador pressionando duas teclas (direita e esquerda) de teclados laterais. A unidade comportamental era quatro respostas às teclas. Nos dois procedimentos apresentava-se um pedaço da figura, um tom e 0,5 segundo, caso a sequência fosse passível de reforço, e um timeout de 1,0 segundo, caso não fosse. Somente no TD/IRI havia um IRI de 0,5 segundo após as três primeiras respostas da sequência. O experimento continha três fases. Na Fase 1, linha de base, vigorava o reforçamento contínuo (CRF) das 16 sequências. Para dois grupos (TD-VAR e TD/IRI-VAR) na Fase 2 havia o reforçamento direto da variabilidade, para 15 sequências, no qual a sequência menos frequente e menos recente teria maior probabilidade de ser reforçada, e CRF de uma sequência alvo (sequência menos frequente na linha de base). Na Fase 3, havia a distribuição acoplada dos reforços da fase anterior para as 15 sequências e CRF de outra sequência alvo. Para os grupos TD-ACO e TD/IRI-ACO a ordem de exposição às fases foi inversa, visto que na Fase 2 a distribuição de reforços foi acoplada ao desempenho dos participantes dos grupos VAR. Nos grupos TD-CON e TD/IRI-CON havia somente o CRF da sequência alvo nas duas fases. Os resultados mostraram que a variabilidade aumentou na primeira sessão da Fase 2 para todos os participantes. O grupo TD-ACO foi o que apresentou maior índices de variabilidade dentro deste procedimento. Já no TD/IRI, isto foi verdadeiro para o grupo TD/IRI-VAR. Com relação à seleção da sequência alvo, os grupos que apresentaram maiores porcentagens de seleção em todas as sessões foram TD-CON e TD/IRI-VAR, respectivamente dentro de cada procedimento. Notou-se que os dois procedimentos produziram seleção da sequência alvo, mas parece que o uso do IRI produziu um responder mais variável, quando este foi diretamente reforçado, o que possibilitou que a seleção da sequência alvo ocorresse mais rapidamente
Book chapters on the topic "Interresponse intervals"
"Interresponse Intervals in Continuation Tapping." In Timing of Behavior. The MIT Press, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6831.003.0006.
Full text