To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Mood and information processing.

Journal articles on the topic 'Mood and information processing'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Mood and information processing.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Zhong, Bu. "Readers' Mood Affects News Information Processing." Newspaper Research Journal 32, no. 3 (June 2011): 52–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/073953291103200305.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Schmid, Petra C., Marianne Schmid Mast, Dario Bombari, Fred W. Mast, and Janek S. Lobmaier. "How Mood States Affect Information Processing During Facial Emotion Recognition: An Eye Tracking Study." Swiss Journal of Psychology 70, no. 4 (December 2011): 223–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000060.

Full text
Abstract:
Existing research shows that a sad mood hinders emotion recognition. More generally, it has been shown that mood affects information processing. A happy mood facilitates global processing and a sad mood boosts local processing. Global processing has been described as the Gestalt-like integration of details; local processing is understood as the detailed processing of the parts. The present study investigated how mood affects the use of information processing styles in an emotion recognition task. Thirty-three participants were primed with happy or sad moods in a within-subjects design. They pe
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Turner, Jim. "Incidental Information Processing: Effects of Mood, Sex and Caffeine." International Journal of Neuroscience 72, no. 1-2 (January 1993): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00207459308991619.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Saito, H., H. Matsuoka, T. Ueno, T. Inosaka, and M. Sato. "Information processing deficits in mood disorder: An ERP study." Biological Psychiatry 35, no. 9 (May 1994): 672. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0006-3223(94)90864-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Carter, Frances A., Cynthia M. Bulik, Rachel H. Lawson, Patrick F. Sullivan, and Jenny S. Wilson. "Effect of Mood and Food Cues on Information Processing in Women with Bulimia Nervosa and Controls." Behaviour Change 14, no. 2 (June 1997): 113–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0813483900003570.

Full text
Abstract:
Information-processing speed and cue reactivity were evaluated in women with bulimia nervosa and controls in response to neutral, mood, and food cues in isolation, and mood and food cues in combination. Significant differences were consistently observed between women with bulimia nervosa and control women on information-processing speed for food/body-related words, but not for words unrelated to food/body concerns. As expected, women with bulimia nervosa demonstrated slower processing of information related to food/body concerns. In addition, the presentation of mood and food cues affected spe
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Bless, Herbert, and Axel M. Burger. "Mood and the Regulation of Mental Abstraction." Current Directions in Psychological Science 26, no. 2 (April 2017): 159–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721417690456.

Full text
Abstract:
Individuals can apply different processing strategies to deal with situations they encounter. One central question in social-cognition research refers to the factors that determine reliance on different processing strategies. Parting from a functional perspective, which holds that processing strategies need to be adjusted to the requirements of the situation, we argue that individuals’ mood carries information about the benign versus problematic nature of the situation and thus that mood can regulate cognitive processing. Focusing on mental abstraction, we propose that positive mood contribute
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Forgas, Joseph P., and Diana Matovic. "Mood Effects on Humor Production: Positive Mood Improves the Verbal Ability to Be Funny." Journal of Language and Social Psychology 39, no. 5-6 (May 21, 2020): 701–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0261927x20917994.

Full text
Abstract:
Can mood influence people’s ability to produce humorous verbal messages? Based on recent theories linking affect to social cognition and information-processing strategies, this experiment predicted and found that positive mood increased people’s ability to generate more creative, humorous, and elaborate verbal contents. Participants viewed positive, neutral, or negative videos, then produced verbal captions to fit four different cartoon images. Their messages were rated for creativity, humor, and elaboration by two trained raters, and the processing latency to produce each message was also rec
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Mohanty, Sachi Nandan, and Damodar Suar. "Decision Making under Uncertainty and Information Processing in Positive and Negative Mood States." Psychological Reports 115, no. 1 (August 2014): 91–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/20.04.pr0.115c16z2.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examines whether mood states (a) influence decision making under uncertainty and (b) affect information processing. 200 students at the Indian Institute of Technology Kharagpur participated in this study. Positive mood was induced by showing comedy movie clips to 100 participants and negative mood was induced by showing tragedy movie clips to another 100 participants. The participants were administered a questionnaire containing hypothetical situations of financial gains and losses, and a health risk problem. The participants selected a choice for each situation, and stated the reas
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Ohtomo, Shoji, Kumiko Takeshima, and Yukio Hirose. "The effects of mood state on the information processing of advertisement." Journal of Human Environmental Studies 8, no. 2 (2010): 123–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.4189/shes.8.123.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Grol, Maud, and Rudi De Raedt. "The effect of positive mood on flexible processing of affective information." Emotion 18, no. 6 (September 2018): 819–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000355.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Rusting, Cheryl L. "Personality, mood, and cognitive processing of emotional information: Three conceptual frameworks." Psychological Bulletin 124, no. 2 (1998): 165–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.165.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

刘, 晓敏. "The Influence of Personality Traits and Mood State on Information Processing." Advances in Social Sciences 05, no. 01 (2016): 22–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.12677/ass.2016.51004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Merens, Wendelien, A. J. Willem Van der Does, and Philip Spinhoven. "The effects of serotonin manipulations on emotional information processing and mood." Journal of Affective Disorders 103, no. 1-3 (November 2007): 43–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2007.01.032.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Matovic, Diana, and Joseph P. Forgas. "The Answer Is in the Question? Mood Effects on Processing Verbal Information and Impression Formation." Journal of Language and Social Psychology 37, no. 5 (March 20, 2018): 578–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0261927x18764544.

Full text
Abstract:
Can good or bad mood influence how people process verbal information about others? Based on affect-cognition theories, this experiment predicted and found that the way a question is phrased has a greater influence on impressions than actual answers when judges are experiencing a negative rather than a neutral or positive mood. After an audiovisual mood induction, participants witnessed interview questions and responses by two target characters. The same level of extroversion was communicated, either by affirmative responses to questions about extraversion, or by negative responses to questions
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Hara, Natsuko. "The effect of unpleasant mood and involvement on information processing in persuasion." Japanese journal of psychology 65, no. 6 (1995): 487–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.65.487.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Das, Enny, and Bob M. Fennis. "In the mood to face the facts: When a positive mood promotes systematic processing of self-threatening information." Motivation and Emotion 32, no. 3 (June 13, 2008): 221–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-008-9093-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Calloni, Judith C., and Michael J. Ross. "Affect Intensity and Self- versus other-Referent Information Processing." Psychological Reports 66, no. 2 (April 1990): 495–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1990.66.2.495.

Full text
Abstract:
High and low affectively intense subjects were compared on endorsement ratings and incidental recall of positive, negative, and neutral trait adjectives rated for self-descriptiveness or other-descriptiveness. The effects of affect intensity were limited to endorsement patterns of positively valenced sell-referenced words; no significant findings were obtained for recall data. These findings suggest that intensity of affect does not appear to be a confounding factor in studies of mood and self-schematic information processing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Zhou, Yixin, Zheng Zhang, Kexin Wang, Shuang Chen, and Mingjie Zhou. "How Social Media Shapes One’s Public Mood: The Three-Way Interaction Effect of Sphere, Information Valence, and Justice Sensitivity." Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology 15 (January 2021): 183449092199142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1834490921991425.

Full text
Abstract:
Public mood is a key concept in explaining collective activity, but the way social media shapes an audience’s public mood is still not fully understood. This study aims to explore how social media posts with various characteristics change public mood. The authors asked 351 participants to read 30 microblog newsletters with a 2 × 2 between-subject design (public × private sphere; positive × negative value). The results showed that (a) positive private information decreased negative public mood, (b) positive public information decreased positive public mood rather than increasing it, and (c) neg
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Constantinou, Elena, Katleen Bogaerts, Ilse Van Diest, and Omer Van den Bergh. "Influences of mood on information processing styles in high and low symptom reporters." Health Psychology Report 4 (2015): 300–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.5114/hpr.2016.55402.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Leppänen, Jukka M. "Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings." Current Opinion in Psychiatry 19, no. 1 (January 2006): 34–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.46411.00.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Stephens, Robert S., and Lisa Curtin. "Alcohol and depression: Effects on mood and biased processing of self-relevant information." Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 9, no. 4 (December 1995): 211–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-164x.9.4.211.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Lassiter, G. Daniel, Linda J. Koenig, and Kevin J. Apple. "Mood and Behavior Perception: Dysphoria Can Increase and Decrease Effortful Processing of Information." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22, no. 8 (August 1996): 794–810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167296228003.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Brown, Jonathon D., and Shelley E. Taylor. "Affect and the processing of personal information: Evidence for mood-activated self-schemata." Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 22, no. 5 (September 1986): 436–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(86)90044-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Warburton, D. M., and Giovanna Mancuso. "Evaluation of the information processing and mood effects of a transdermal nicotine patch." Psychopharmacology 135, no. 3 (January 22, 1998): 305–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002130050514.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Barch, Deanna M. "Risk for Mood Pathology: Neural and Psychological Markers of Abnormal Negative Information Processing." Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 53, no. 5 (May 2014): 497–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2014.01.012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Palfai, Tibor P., and Peter Salovey. "The Influence of Depressed and Elated Mood on Deductive and Inductive Reasoning." Imagination, Cognition and Personality 13, no. 1 (September 1993): 57–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/fyya-gcru-j124-q3b2.

Full text
Abstract:
Depressed and elated mood states may produce distinct information processing styles that can affect performance on deductive and inductive reasoning tasks differentially. Seventy-two undergraduates were asked to view a set of two film clips designed to induce either elated, neutral or depressed moods. One clip preceded each of two reasoning tasks, a deduction task and an induction task. We predicted that subjects in a depressed mood would exhibit impoverished performance relative to the other two conditions on the inductive reasoning problems but enhanced performance on those that involved ded
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Wytykowska, Agata. "The Type of Temperament, Mood, and Strategies of Categorization." Journal of Individual Differences 33, no. 4 (January 2012): 227–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000073.

Full text
Abstract:
In Strelau’s theory of temperament (RTT), there are four types of temperament, differentiated according to low vs. high stimulation processing capacity and to the level of their internal harmonization. The type of temperament is considered harmonized when the constellation of all temperamental traits is internally matched to the need for stimulation, which is related to effectiveness of stimulation processing. In nonharmonized temperamental structure, an internal mismatch is observed which is linked to ineffectiveness of stimulation processing. The three studies presented here investigated the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Inoue, Sakina, Shinichi Oura, Kazuya Matsuo, and Yoshikazu Fukui. "Effects of internal working models of attachment on global/local processing: Changes in information processing by negative mood." Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 81 (September 20, 2017): 3C—034–3C—034. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.81.0_3c-034.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Kitamura, Hideya. "Effects of mood states on information processing strategies: Two studies of automatic and controlled processing using misattribution paradigms." Asian Journal Of Social Psychology 8, no. 2 (August 2005): 139–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839x.2005.00163.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Eubank, Martin, Dave Collins, and Nick Smith. "Anxiety and Ambiguity: It’s All Open to Interpretation." Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 24, no. 3 (September 2002): 239–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jsep.24.3.239.

Full text
Abstract:
Beck’s (1976) theoretical account of emotional vulnerability predicts that individuals who are vulnerable to anxiety will exhibit a cognitive processing bias for the threatening interpretation of ambiguous information. As anxiety direction (Jones, 1995) may best account for individual differences, the aim of this study was to establish whether such processing bias is a function of anxiety interpretation. Anxiety facilitators and debilitators underwent a modified Stroop test by reacting to neutral and ambiguous word types in neutral, positive, and negative mood conditions. A significant 3-way i
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Das, Enny, Charlotte Vonkeman, and Tilo Hartmann. "Mood as a resource in dealing with health recommendations: How mood affects information processing and acceptance of quit-smoking messages." Psychology & Health 27, no. 1 (January 2012): 116–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2011.569888.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Rafienia, Parvin, Parviz Azadfallah, Ali Fathi-Ashtiani, and Kazem Rasoulzadeh-Tabatabaiei. "The role of extraversion, neuroticism and positive and negative mood in emotional information processing." Personality and Individual Differences 44, no. 2 (January 2008): 392–402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.08.018.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Gasper, Karen, and Gerald L. Clore. "Attending to the Big Picture: Mood and Global Versus Local Processing of Visual Information." Psychological Science 13, no. 1 (January 2002): 34–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00406.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Ellenbogen, Mark A., Alex E. Schwartzman, Jane Stewart, and Claire-Dominique Walker. "Stress and selective attention: The interplay of mood, cortisol levels, and emotional information processing." Psychophysiology 39, no. 6 (November 2002): 723–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3960723.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

TSOURTOS, G., J. C. THOMPSON, and C. STOUGH. "Evidence of an early information processing speed deficit in unipolar major depression." Psychological Medicine 32, no. 2 (February 2002): 259–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291701005001.

Full text
Abstract:
Background. Slowing of the speed of information processing has been reported in geriatric depression, but it is not clear if the impairment is present in younger patients, if motor retardation is responsible, or if antidepressant medications play a role.Method. Twenty unmedicated unipolar depressed inpatients were compared with 19 medicated depressed in-patients and 20 age-, sex- and verbal IQ-matched controls on inspection time (IT), a measure of speed of information processing that does not require a speeded motor response. We also examined the relationship between IT and current mood and le
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Tompkins, Connie A. "Automatic and Effortful Processing of Emotional Intonation After Right or Left Hemisphere Brain Damage." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 34, no. 4 (August 1991): 820–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3404.820.

Full text
Abstract:
This study assessed the effects of unilateral right (RHD) or left hemisphere brain damage (LHD) on the knowledge and processing of emotional information imparted by vocal intonation Semantically neutral statements that conveyed a mood through prosody were used as targets in a mood priming task. These targets were preceded by story primes. The events described in the primes were either congruent with the mood conveyed by the intonation of a target phrase, incongruent with target mood, or emotionally neutral. Prime-target pairs were presented in two attention conditions designed to favor either
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Sterzer, P., T. Hilgenfeldt, P. Freudenberg, F. Bermpohl, and M. Adli. "Access of emotional information to visual awareness in patients with major depressive disorder." Psychological Medicine 41, no. 8 (January 5, 2011): 1615–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291710002540.

Full text
Abstract:
BackgroundAccording to cognitive theories of depression, negative biases affect most cognitive processes including perception. Such depressive perception may result not only from biased cognitive appraisal but also from automatic processing biases that influence the access of sensory information to awareness.MethodTwenty patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) and 20 healthy control participants underwent behavioural testing with a variant of binocular rivalry, continuous flash suppression (CFS), to investigate the potency of emotional visual stimuli to gain access to awareness. While a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Zajusz-Gawędzka, Dominika, and Magdalena Marszał-Wiśniewska. "Context-dependent effect of mood: the regulatory role of personality." Polish Psychological Bulletin 46, no. 1 (March 1, 2015): 144–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ppb-2015-0019.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This study explored the influence of the context-dependent effect of mood as well as individual differences in neuroticism and action vs. state/volatility orientation on predecisional processing in a multiattribute choice task. One hundred and twenty participants acquired information about choice options after filling out personality questionnaires. Results showed that participants in a positive mood processed the information longer in enjoy than in done-enough context. In turn, participants in a negative mood processed the information more selectively in enjoy than in done-enough con
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Harper, Bridgette D., Elizabeth A. Lemerise, and Sarah L. Caverly. "The Effect of Induced Mood on Children’s Social Information Processing: Goal Clarification and Response Decision." Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 38, no. 5 (September 10, 2009): 575–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9356-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Koster, Ernst H. W., Rudi De Raedt, Lemke Leyman, and Evi De Lissnyder. "Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: Exploring the coherence among information-processing biases." Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, no. 3 (March 2010): 219–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Ingram, Rick E., Kelly Bailey, and Greg Siegle. "Emotional Information Processing and Disrupted Parental Bonding: Cognitive Specificity and Avoidance." Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 18, no. 1 (January 2004): 53–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/jcop.18.1.53.28051.

Full text
Abstract:
Although studies have assessed the association between affective dysfunction and parental bonding, little research has assessed the information processing characteristics of individuals with disrupted parental bonding. The current study investigated differences in attentional processing between individuals with relatively poor versus secure parental bonding, and also assessed this processing in conjunction with a mood priming procedure that has been used in previous vulnerability research. Using a Stroop procedure, results indicated that poorly bonded individuals were less distracted by depres
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Itoh, Mika. "Mood-congruent effect in self-relevant information processing. A study using an autobiographical memory recall task." Japanese journal of psychology 71, no. 4 (2000): 281–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.71.281.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

LINNOILA, MARKKU, MATTHEW V. RUDORFER, KRISTINE V. DUBYOSKI, ROBERT R. RAWLINGS, and MICHAEL J. ECKARDT. "Effects of One-Week Lithium Treatment on Skilled Performance, Information Processing, and Mood in Healthy Volunteers." Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 6, no. 6 (December 1986): 356???358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004714-198612000-00006.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Sudarma, Made, and I. Gede Harsemadi. "Design and Analysis System of KNN and ID3 Algorithm for Music Classification based on Mood Feature Extraction." International Journal of Electrical and Computer Engineering (IJECE) 7, no. 1 (February 1, 2017): 486. http://dx.doi.org/10.11591/ijece.v7i1.pp486-495.

Full text
Abstract:
Each of music which has been created, has its own mood which is emitted, therefore, there has been many researches in Music Information Retrieval (MIR) field that has been done for recognition of mood to music. This research produced software to classify music to the mood by using K-Nearest Neighbor and ID3 algorithm. In this research accuracy performance comparison and measurement of average classification time is carried out which is obtained based on the value produced from music feature extraction process. For music feature extraction process it uses 9 types of spectral analysis, consists
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Zhang, Lu, and Lydia Hanks. "Consumer skepticism towards CSR messages." International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management 29, no. 8 (August 14, 2017): 2070–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ijchm-11-2015-0666.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose This study aims to investigate the joint effect of three factors – processing fluency, the individuals’ need for cognition (NFC) and mood – on consumer skepticism toward corporate social responsibility (CSR) messages. Design/Methodology/Approach Study 1 uses a 2 (fluency: high versus low) × 2 (NFC: high versus low) design. In Study 2, a three-way interaction between fluency, NFC and mood was explored. Findings Individuals high in need for cognition responded more positively after reading a CSR message that is difficult to process. On the other hand, people low in NFC exhibited a higher
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Leyman, L., R. De Raedt, M. A. Vanderhasselt, and C. Baeken. "Influence of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the inhibition of emotional information in healthy volunteers." Psychological Medicine 39, no. 6 (October 6, 2008): 1019–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291708004431.

Full text
Abstract:
BackgroundEvidence suggests that repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) might be a promising new treatment procedure for depression. However, underlying working mechanisms of this technique are yet unclear. Multiple sessions of rTMS may – apart from the reported antidepressant effects – cause primary improvements in attentional control over emotional information, modulated by changes in cortical brain excitability within stimulated prefrontal regions.MethodIn two experiments, we examined the temporary effects of high-frequency (
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Demestre, Josep, and José E. García-Albea. "The On-Line Resolution of the Sentence Complement/Relative Clause Ambiguity: Evidence from Spanish." Experimental Psychology 51, no. 1 (January 2004): 59–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.51.1.59.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Two self-paced reading experiments investigated syntactic ambiguity resolution in Spanish. The experiments examined the way in which Spanish subjects initially interpret sentences that are temporarily ambiguous between a sentence complement and a relative clause interpretation. Experiment 1 examined whether the sentence complement preference found in English is observed in Spanish speaking subjects. In Experiment 2, verbal mood was manipulated in order to study the influence of verb-specific information on sentence processing. Since subcategorization for a subjunctive complement clau
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Cooley, Eileen L., and Anthony Y. Stringer. "Word-Type Effects in Word-Stem Priming: Evidence for Semantic Processing in the Perceptual Representation System?" Perceptual and Motor Skills 87, no. 1 (August 1998): 263–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1998.87.1.263.

Full text
Abstract:
While a presemantic Perceptual Representation System is believed to mediate implicit memory tasks such as word-stem priming, clinical studies suggest semantic information can be processed during priming. To clarify the nature of this system, we investigated word-type effects in word-stem priming in a nonclinical sample of 41 undergraduates who rated the pleasantness of threatening and non threatening words, performed implicit and explicit memory tasks, and completed measures of mood state. More nonthreatening words were primed and scores on the Beck Depression Inventory were negatively correla
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Chavoix, Chantal, Martine Desi, and Monique de Bonis. "Relationship between Mood State and Information Processing of Negative versus Positive Emotional Stimuli in Brain-Damaged Patients." Psychopathology 20, no. 1 (1987): 34–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000284477.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Antypa, Niki, August HM Smelt, Annette Strengholt, and AJ Willem Van der Does. "Effects of omega-3 fatty acid supplementation on mood and emotional information processing in recovered depressed individuals." Journal of Psychopharmacology 26, no. 5 (October 16, 2011): 738–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269881111424928.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!