To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Musik, Melodik, Terz.

Journal articles on the topic 'Musik, Melodik, Terz'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Musik, Melodik, Terz.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Cornelius, Nathan, and Jenine L. Brown. "The interaction of repetition and difficulty for working memory in melodic dictation tasks." Research Studies in Music Education 42, no. 3 (April 22, 2019): 368–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1321103x18821194.

Full text
Abstract:
This research examines the effect of repetition on melodic dictation tasks in an undergraduate ear-training class. A pilot group of freshman music majors ( n = 17) were asked to notate four melodies, of which two were slightly more difficult since they contained more melodic leaps. Participants heard two melodies repeated three times and two other melodies six times. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) suggests that the number of repetitions had a significant effect on participants’ dictation accuracy, both for scores on pitch and on rhythm. In addition, dictation accuracy was significantly lower when the melodies contained more leaps (controlling for other factors). Overall, we found a statistical interaction between the number of repetitions and the number of leaps in the melody, both of which factors affect the working memory load in these dictation tasks. Given the similarity of the notated melodies, these findings suggest that ear-training pedagogues must carefully select melodic dictations appropriate for student ability and control the number of melodic leaps. Furthermore, we found evidence that the variance in working memory for music among this population is wider than Karpinski (2000) hypothesizes. These findings provide pedagogues with melodic characteristics well-suited for the average incoming freshman music major. Finally, this first empirical evidence of the dictation ability of incoming undergraduate music majors invites a long-term study on the extent to which working memory and/or chunking ability may increase during the multi-semester ear-training curriculum.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Büdenbender, Niklas, and Gunter Kreutz. "Long-term representations of melodies in Western listeners: Influences of familiarity, musical expertise, tempo and structure." Psychology of Music 45, no. 5 (October 19, 2016): 665–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735616671408.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigated the effects of familiarity, level of musical expertise, musical tempo, and structural boundaries on the identification of familiar and unfamiliar tunes. Healthy Western listeners ( N = 62; age range 14–64 years) judged their level of familiarity with a preselected set of melodies when the number of tones of a given melody was increased from trial to trial according to the so-called gating paradigm. The number of tones served as one dependent measure. The second dependent measure was the physical duration of the stimulus presentation until listeners identified a melody as familiar or unfamiliar. Results corroborate previous work, suggesting that listeners need less information to recognize familiar as compared to unfamiliar melodies. Both decreasing and increasing the original tempo by a factor of two delayed the identification of familiar melodies. Furthermore, listeners had more difficulty identifying unfamiliar melodies when tempo was increased. Finally, musical expertise significantly influenced identification of either melodic category, i.e., reducing the required number of tones. Taken together, the findings support theories which suggest that tempo information is coded in melody representation, and that musical expertise is associated with especially efficient strategies for accessing long-term representations of melodic materials.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Cenkerová, Zuzana, and Richard Parncutt. "Style-Dependency of Melodic Expectation." Music Perception 33, no. 1 (September 1, 2015): 110–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2015.33.1.110.

Full text
Abstract:
In theories of auditory scene analysis and melodic implication/realization, melodic expectation results from an interaction between top-down processes (assumed to be learned and schema-based) and bottom-up processes (assumed innate, based on Gestalt principles). If principles of melodic expectation are partly acquired, it should be possible to manipulate them – to condition listeners' expectations. In this study, the resistance of three bottom-up expectation principles to learning was tested experimentally. In Experiment 1, expectations for stepwise motion (pitch proximity) were manipulated by conditioning listeners to large melodic leaps; preference for small intervals was reduced after a brief exposure. In Experiment 2, expectations for leaps to rise and steps to fall (step declination) were manipulated by exposing listeners to melodies comprising rising steps and falling leaps; this reduced preferences for descending seconds and thirds. Experiment 3 did not find and hence failed to alter the expectation for small intervals to be followed by an interval in the same direction (step inertia). The results support the theory that bottom-up principles of melodic perception are partly learned from exposure to pitch patterns in music. The long-term learning process could be reinforced by exposure to speech based on similar organization principles.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Suryati, Suryati, G. R. Lono L. Simatupang, and Victor Ganap. "Ornamentasi Seni Baca Al-Qur’an dalam Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an sebagai Bentuk Ekspresi Estetis Seni Suara." Resital: Jurnal Seni Pertunjukan 17, no. 2 (October 30, 2018): 67–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.24821/resital.v17i2.2219.

Full text
Abstract:
Ornamentasi atau hiasan merupakan suatu istilah musik yang memiliki arti penambahan beberapa nada atau notasi pada melodi, biasanya satu suku kata untuk beberapa nada yang disebut dengan istilah melisma. Ornamentasi atau hiasan nada sangat diperlukan dalam seni suara untuk memperindah suatu melodi. Ornamentasi melodi juga terdapat pada lantunan seni baca Al-Qur’an dengan gaya Qira’ah atau mujawwad. Seni baca Al-Qur’an tersebut melagukan secara penuh melismatis dengan hiasan-hiasan atau ornamentasi melodi agar lantunan menjadi indah. Seni baca Al-Qur’an termasuk seni suara yang sering dilombakan dalam Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an (MTQ). Penelitian ini mengkaji ornamentasi melodi dan cara-cara melantunkan seni baca Al-Qur’an dalam Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an (MTQ), melalui pendekatan musikologis dan antropologis perilaku pelantun Al-Qur’an. Hasil penelitian ini menunjukkan bahwa ornamentasi yang terjadi pada lantunan seni baca Al-Qur’an dengan gaya Qira’ah merupakan bentuk ekspresi estetis seni suara dari Pelantun Al-Qur’an (Qori/Qoriah) sesuai kemampuan dan kreativitas pelantun dalam berolah vokal. Ornamentation the Art of Qur’anic recitation in Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an as a Form of Aesthetic Expression of the Art of Sound. Ornamentation is a musical term that means adding a few notes or notation on the melody, normally one word for several notes known as the melisma. Ornamentation or ornamented notes are needed in the art of sound to reshape a melody. There are also additional melodic chanting on the art of Qur’anic recitation in the style the Qira'ah the mujawwad. The art of Qur’anic recitation practice in full melismatic with decorations or additional melodic chant in order to be beautiful. The art of Qur’anic recitation includes the sound art that is often competed in the Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an (MTQ). This research examines melodic ornamentation and the ways art of Qur’anic recitation practiced in the Musabaqoh Tilawatil Qur’an (MTQ), through musikological and anthropological approaches to the behavior of in reading. The results of this study suggest that ornamentation piece of art that happens to read the Qur'anic in style is a form of aesthetic expression the art of sound of Qira'ah of its Chanter (Qori/Qoriah) fits the ability and creativity of chanter in doing the vocals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Bailes, Freya, and Charles Delbé. "Long-term melodic expectation: The unexpected observation of distant priming effects." Musicae Scientiae 13, no. 2 (September 2009): 315–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/102986490901300205.

Full text
Abstract:
The report provides a brief account of an experiment whose control conditions produced interestingly counter-intuitive results. The method adapted priming techniques to explore whether imagining well-known melodies would facilitate perceptual discrimination of congruent compared to incongruent melodic continuations in a syllable identification task. This was shown to be the case, but in a subsequent control experiment, imagining an irrelevant lure melody also showed a priming effect. The persistent priming effect apparently related the target sequence to the aurally presented, nonadjacent opening notes, and not to the intervening mental image. A number of statistical analyses of the pitch relationships in match and mismatch targets were performed and a further experiment is reported in which participants explicitly selected between match and mismatch versions of the stimuli for fit within the prime context. It seems that the pitch proximity of the first target note to the final note of the sounded prime may be responsible for the priming effect. An outline of further research to explain the phenomenon is suggested, including experiments to test the strength of melodic priming governed by pitch proximity, by systematically varying the length of the period between prime and target.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Habibi, Assal, Vinthia Wirantana, and Arnold Starr. "Cortical Activity During Perception of Musical Pitch." Music Perception 30, no. 5 (December 2012): 463–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2013.30.5.463.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigates the effects of music training on brain activity to violations of melodic expectancies. We recorded behavioral and event-related brain potential (ERP) responses of musicians and nonmusicians to discrepancies of pitch between pairs of unfamiliar melodies based on Western classical rules. Musicians detected pitch deviations significantly better than nonmusicians. In musicians compared to nonmusicians, auditory cortical potentials to notes but not unrelated warning tones exhibited enhanced P200 amplitude generally, and in response to pitch deviations enhanced amplitude for N150 and P300 (P3a) but not N100 was observed. P3a latency was shorter in musicians compared to nonmusicians. Both the behavioral and cortical activity differences observed between musicians and nonmusicians in response to deviant notes were significant with stimulation of the right but not the left ear, suggesting that left-sided brain activity differentiated musicians from nonmusicians. The enhanced amplitude of N150 among musicians with right ear stimulation was positively correlated with earlier age onset of music training. Our data support the notion that long-term music training in musicians leads to functional reorganization of auditory brain systems, and that these effects are potentiated by early age onset of training.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Meeùs, Nicolas. "Inhalt (‘content’) as a technical term in musical semiotics." Rasprave Instituta za hrvatski jezik i jezikoslovlje 44, no. 2 (2018): 539–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.44.2.14.

Full text
Abstract:
Inhalt (‘content’) is so common that it could hardly pass as a technical term. The purpose of this article is to show that from the 18th to the 20th century it was nevertheless used particularly to denote the specifically musical meaning arising from what music ‘contains’ of notes, rhythms, melodic cells, etc. Hegel, Marx, Hauptmann, Hanslick, Schenker, Schoenberg and probably others shared the same view that music has a content of its own, one that cannot be translated in verbal language.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Christiner, Markus, Christine Gross, Annemarie Seither-Preisler, and Peter Schneider. "The Melody of Speech: What the Melodic Perception of Speech Reveals about Language Performance and Musical Abilities." Languages 6, no. 3 (August 2, 2021): 132. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/languages6030132.

Full text
Abstract:
Research has shown that melody not only plays a crucial role in music but also in language acquisition processes. Evidence has been provided that melody helps in retrieving, remembering, and memorizing new language material, while relatively little is known about whether individuals who perceive speech as more melodic than others also benefit in the acquisition of oral languages. In this investigation, we wanted to show which impact the subjective melodic perception of speech has on the pronunciation of unfamiliar foreign languages. We tested 86 participants for how melodic they perceived five unfamiliar languages, for their ability to repeat and pronounce the respective five languages, for their musical abilities, and for their short-term memory (STM). The results revealed that 59 percent of the variance in the language pronunciation tasks could be explained by five predictors: the number of foreign languages spoken, short-term memory capacity, tonal aptitude, melodic singing ability, and how melodic the languages appeared to the participants. Group comparisons showed that individuals who perceived languages as more melodic performed significantly better in all language tasks than those who did not. However, even though we expected musical measures to be related to the melodic perception of foreign languages, we could only detect some correlations to rhythmical and tonal musical aptitude. Overall, the findings of this investigation add a new dimension to language research, which shows that individuals who perceive natural languages to be more melodic than others also retrieve and pronounce utterances more accurately.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Cuddy, Lola L., Jacalyn M. Duffin, Sudeep S. Gill, Cassandra L. Brown, Ritu Sikka, and Ashley D. Vanstone. "Memory for Melodies and Lyrics in Alzheimer's Disease." Music Perception 29, no. 5 (June 1, 2012): 479–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.29.5.479.

Full text
Abstract:
this research addressed the question: is musical memory preserved in dementia, specifically, dementia of the Alzheimer type (AD)? Six tests involving different aspects of melody and language processing were administered to each of five groups of participants: 50 younger adults, 100 older adults, and 50 AD older adults classified into three levels of AD severity—mild, moderate and severe. No test was immune to, but not all tests were equally sensitive to, the presence of dementia. Long-term familiarity for melody was preserved across levels of AD, even at the severe stage for a few individuals. Detecting pitch distortions in melodies was possible for mild and some of the moderate AD participants. The ability to sing a melody when prompted by its lyrics was retained at the mild stage and was retained by a few individuals through the severe stages of AD. Long-term familiarity with the lyrics of familiar melodies was also found across levels of AD. In contrast, detection of grammatical distortions in the lyrics of familiar melodies and the ability to complete familiar proverbs were affected even at the mild stage of AD. We conclude that musical semantic memory may be spared through the mild and moderate stages of AD and may be preserved even in some individuals at the severe stage.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Dowling, W. Jay. "Context Effects on Melody Recognition: Scale-Step versus Interval Representations." Music Perception 3, no. 3 (1986): 281–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285338.

Full text
Abstract:
A basic question in cognitive psychology concerns ways in which sensory information is represented in memory. Listeners performed a long-term transposition recognition task in which brief melodies were presented with a chordal context that defined their scale-step interpretations. Context either remained constant or changed at test. In two experiments listeners with moderate amounts of musical experience performed well with constant context but at chance with shifting context. Inexperienced listeners (as well as professionals in one of the studies) performed equally well regardless of context. This result suggests that inexperienced listeners represented melodies as sequences of pitch intervals that remained invariant across context shifts. In contrast, moderately experienced listeners appear to have represented melodies as scale-step sequences that were affected by context. Professionals, while capable of scale-step representation, were able to use a flexible memory-retrieval system to avoid errors with changed context. A third experiment showed that moderately experienced listeners were able to base long-term recognition on either contour or scale-step information, depending on instructions. These results suggest that the scale-step representation used by moderately experienced listeners involved both contour and scale information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Hébert, Sylvie, and Isabelle Peretz. "Recognition of music in long-term memory: Are melodic and temporal patterns equal partners?" Memory & Cognition 25, no. 4 (July 1997): 518–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03201127.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Gosfield, Avery. "I Sing it to an Italian Tune . . . Thoughts on Performing Sixteenth-Century Italian-Jewish Sung Poetry Today." European Journal of Jewish Studies 8, no. 1 (June 25, 2014): 9–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/1872471x-12341256.

Full text
Abstract:
Although we know that Jewish musicians and composers were active in Renaissance Italy, very few compositions by Jewish authors or music specifically destined for the Jewish community has survived. There are few exceptions: Salamone Rossi’s works, the tunes from Guglielmo Ebreo da Pesaro’s dance manuals, Ercole Bottrigari’s transcriptions of Jewish liturgy, a handful of fragments. If we limit the list to pieces with specifically Jewish content, it becomes shorter still: Rossi’s HaShirim asher liShlomo and Bottrigari’s fieldwork. However, next to these rare musical sources, there are hundreds of poems by Jewish authors that, although preserved in text-only form, were probably performed vocally. Written in Italian, Hebrew and Yiddish, they usually combine Italian form with Jewish content. The constant transposition and transformation of form, language and content found in works such as Josef Tzarfati’s Hebrew translation of Tu dormi, io veglio, Elye Bokher’s Bovo Bukh, or Moses of Rieti’s Miqdash Me’at (an artful reworking of Dante’s Divina Commedia) mirror the shared and separate spaces that defined Jewish life in sixteenth-century Italy. None of these poems have come down to us with musical notation. However, several have extant melodic models, while others have indications, or are written in meters—like the ottava or terza rima—that point to their being sung, probably often to orally transmitted melodies. Even if it is sometimes impossible to ascertain the exact tune used in performance, sung poetry’s predominance in Jewish musical life remains undeniable. HaShirim asher liShlomo, usually considered the most important collection of Jewish Renaissance music, might not have ever been performed during its composer’s lifetime, while Rieti’s Miqdash Me’at survives in over fifty manuscripts, including four Italian translations. In one of these, translator/author Lazzaro of Viterbo writes, tellingly, about looking forward to hearing his verses sung by his dedicatee, Donna Corcos.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Wilson, Sarah J., Kate Parsons, and David C. Reutens. "Preserved Singing in Aphasia." Music Perception 24, no. 1 (September 1, 2006): 23–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.23.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examined the efficacy of Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT) in a male singer (KL) with severe Broca’s aphasia. Thirty novel phrases were allocated to one of three experimental conditions: unrehearsed, rehearsed verbal production (repetition), and rehearsed verbal production with melody (MIT). The results showed superior production of MIT phrases during therapy. Comparison of performance at baseline, 1 week, and 5 weeks after therapy revealed an initial beneficial effect of both types of rehearsal; however, MIT was more durable, facilitating longer-term phrase production. Our findings suggest that MIT facilitated KL’s speech praxis, and that combining melody and speech through rehearsal promoted separate storage and/or access to the phrase representation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

FRANKLIN, JUDY A. "JAZZ MELODY GENERATION USING RECURRENT NETWORKS AND REINFORCEMENT LEARNING." International Journal on Artificial Intelligence Tools 15, no. 04 (August 2006): 623–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0218213006002849.

Full text
Abstract:
Recurrent (neural) networks have been deployed as models for learning musical processes, by computational scientists who study processes such as dynamic systems. Over time, more intricate music has been learned as the state of the art in recurrent networks improves. One particular recurrent network, the Long Short-Term Memory (LSTM) network shows promise for learning long songs, and generating new songs. We are experimenting with a module containing two inter-recurrent LSTM networks to cooperatively learn several human melodies, based on the songs' harmonic structures, and on the feedback inherent in the network. We show that these networks can learn to reproduce four human melodies. We then present as input new harmonizations, so as to generate new songs. We describe the reharmonizations, and show the new melodies that result. We also present a hierarchical structure for using reinforcement learning to choose LSTM modules during the course of melody generation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Povel, Dirk-Jan, and Erik Jansen. "Perceptual Mechanisms in Music Processing." Music Perception 19, no. 2 (2001): 169–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2001.19.2.169.

Full text
Abstract:
To gain a better understanding of the processes by which human listeners construct musical percepts within the Western tonal system, we conducted two experiments in which the perception of brief tone series was studied. The tone series consisted of (fragments from) different orderings of the collection C4 E4 F##4 G4 Bþþ4 and were preceded by two chords to induce a key. Two different tasks were used: (1) rating the melodic "goodness" of the tone series and (2) playing a few tones that complete the tone series. In Experiment 1, tone series of different lengths were presented in blocks. In Experiments 2a and 2b, increasing fragments of tone series were presented to examine the development of musical percepts. The majority of the data can be explained by two perceptual mechanisms: chord recognition and anchoring. Chord recognition is the mechanism that describes a series of tones in terms of a chord, a mental unit stored in long-term memory. Anchoring is the mechanism by which a tone is linked to a tone occurring later in the series. The paradigm appears to be a powerful tool for tracing perceptual mechanisms at work in the on-line processing of music.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Vatri, Alessandro. "Between Song and Prose: the meaning(s) of Harmonia in Aristotle's Rhetoric and Poetics." Rhetorica 34, no. 4 (2016): 372–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/rh.2016.34.4.372.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examines the uses of the term harmonia in Aristotle's Rhetoric and Poetics and aims at identifying a consistent meaning of this word when applied to the literary arts. A difficulty arises from the fact that harmonia commonly denotes the melodic component of music and speech, but is mentioned in connection with the hexametric rhythm in two parallel passages from the Poetics and the Rhetoric, the latter of which is textually problematic. The solution presented in this article suggests an interpretation which assigns to harmonia the meaning of ‘speech melody’ and supports the least disruptive emendation of the contested passage from the Rhetoric.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Petrović, Milena, and Marija Golubović. "The use of metaphorical musical terminology for verbal description of music." Rasprave Instituta za hrvatski jezik i jezikoslovlje 44, no. 2 (2018): 627–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.31724/rihjj.44.2.20.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this paper is to indicate the importance of the metaphorical terminology and verbal description of music in education and performance due to inevitable role of emotions and embodiment in music experience. Metaphorical music terminology should follow the interpretative maturity, such as for the term scherzo, which would be joke for younger, but forced joke or all but prank for older musicians. For music beginners we can use extramusical verbal symbols: the pulse is represented as the stickman; major with the symbol of sun and minor with the symbol of rain; sequencing is presented with the picture of stairs; the picture of butterfly implies image-schematicity in interpreting the wave melodic contour; children understood duple meter through the picture of a soldier, while triple meter appreciated through the picture of a ballerina; staccato is experienced as a movement, but also as a visual and auditory metaphor. Multimodality plays an important role in music education, because it implies the integration of movement, sound, picture and verbal metaphors. Therefore, the musical experience is described and performance interpreted by following the direction from the emotional sound experience to its cognitive processing. Mul- timodal approach would increase associative thinking and enlarge individual associations on musical terms, which gives a better understanding of music and widens perspective in music education.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Rainey, David W., and Janet D. Larsen. "The Effect of Familiar Melodies on Initial Learning and Long-term Memory for Unconnected Text." Music Perception 20, no. 2 (2002): 173–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.173.

Full text
Abstract:
In two experiments we tested the hypothesis that music, in the form of a familiar melody, can serve as an effective mnemonic device. Prior research has provided very little support for this commonly held belief. In both studies, participants learned a list of names that they heard either spoken or sung to a familiar tune. In Experiment 1, the melody was "Pop Goes the Weasel"; in Experiment 2, the melody was "Yankee Doodle." We measured the number of trials to learn the list initially and the number of trials to relearn the list a week later. In both studies, there was no advantage in initial learning for those who learned the names to the musical accompaniment. However,in both studies, participants who heard the sung version required fewer trials to relearn the list of names a week later than did participants who heard the spoken version.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Oura, Yoko. "Constructing a Representation of a Melody: Transforming Melodic Segments into Reduced Pitch Patterns Operated on by Modifiers." Music Perception 9, no. 2 (1991): 251–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285531.

Full text
Abstract:
A reduced-pitch-pattern model for melodic processing is proposed. The model assumes that experienced listeners divide a melody into segments, derive a reduced pitch pattern from each segment, and then try to match each pattern to one of the prototypes stored in long-term memory. As a result, the melody is memorized accurately and quickly. This model and two competing models (the contour model and the harmonic progression model) make different predictions concerning what aspects of a melody would be preserved and what types of error in recall would appear. Recall data of a tonal melody of 12 measures in length were used to examine these predictions. The data were gathered from eight college music majors. Analysis of erroneous reproductions showed that reduced pitch patterns and harmonic progressions were preserved well and that errors predicted by the reduced-pitch-pattern model occurred more often than those predicted by the two competing models. It is concluded that the reduced-pitch-pattern model is the most tenable of the three.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Boldurescu, Yulia V. "Creative Flute Playing in Work with Beginners." ICONI, no. 1 (2020): 76–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.33779/2658-4824.2020.1.076-087.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is addressed to faculty members of departments of wind and percussion instruments in children’s music schools and children’s schools for the arts for collaborative work with beginners in elementary fl ute classes. The article proceeds to describe and formulate questions and playing assignments with utilization of the repertoire of folk melodies for bringing out the instructor’s and pupil’s artistic approaches in working with original musical texts and cognizing its content-related side on the early stages of studies in children’s music schools and children’s schools for the arts. The term “playing assignments” presume role playing in the dialogues of two fl utes or fl ute and piano with a revision and transformation of the primary musical text (the arrangement). Due to the technique of semantic analysis, the instructor may demonstrate to children the secrets of arrangements; they will understand how to make use of various means of transformation of the musical texts, as well as the semantic fi gures involved, acquaint themselves with the peculiarities of construction of folk song melodies and learn to disclose the boundaries of musical retorts in musical dialogues.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Klempe, Sven Hroar. "Implicit polyphony: A framework for understanding cultural complexity." Culture & Psychology 24, no. 1 (July 3, 2017): 60–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1354067x17716390.

Full text
Abstract:
Musical terms like ‘polyphony’ are often applied in psychology and other disciplines in a more or less metaphorical way. However, this article investigates how polyphony can be applied in a non-metaphorical manner, i.e. in the same way, as it is understood in musicology. The fundamental hypothesis is that music represents a basic capacity of the human mind, and that this has impact on other human capacities, like language. If so, this should be traceable in different ways in different cultures. To investigate this, ‘implicit polyphony’ is launched as a term that refers to music, which is melodic, but at the same time reveals a more or less hidden polyphonic structure. This musical phenomenon is demonstrated by examples from Bach and Ravel. It is demonstrated that polyphony is at the core of music, not only in Western classical music, but also African and other ethnical music. Implicit polyphony defined as two voices condensed into one is also found in Norwegian Sámi music. The latter leads to a conclusion, which says that continuity in music is related to verticality. Investigations in linguistics show that the oral use of language is highly comparable with implicit polyphony in music. The same is modernistic literature where the aim has been to turn language into music, as in parts of James Joyce’s novel Ulysses. By bringing in examples of lexical and conceptual blending, the final conclusion is that ‘implicit polyphony’ may serve as a tool for understanding the complexity in human thinking and culture.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Rotstein, Andrea. "CRETANΝΟΜΟΙ: ARCHILOCHUS, FR. 232W WITHOUT HERACLIDES LEMBUS." Classical Quarterly 68, no. 2 (December 2018): 384–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0009838818000538.

Full text
Abstract:
Archil. fr. 232 West (= 50 Tarditi = 133 Bergk = 230 LB) reads as follows:νόμος δὲ Κρητικὸς διδάσκεταιa Cretan law is taught (transl. Dilts)That the term νόμος should be interpreted here in a legal sense has never been contested, and justly so, since its attested meanings are ‘usage, custom, legal norm, statute, law’. However, from the fifth centuryb.c.e.on, νόμοι are also related to music, referring to ‘melodies’ in general or, as a more technical term, to established ‘musical patterns’. The notion of distribution, of an order socially accepted as valid, seems to underlie the use of νόμος in both the legal and the musical fields.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Hantz, Edwin C., Garry C. Crummer, John W. Wayman, Joseph P. Walton, and Robert D. Frisina. "Effects of Musical Training and Absolute Pitch on the Neural Processing of Melodic Intervals: A P3 Event-Related Potential Study." Music Perception 10, no. 1 (1992): 25–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285536.

Full text
Abstract:
During perceptual tasks involving the discrimination of musical intervals, event-related potentials, specifically the P3, were measured for three subject groups: musicians without absolute pitch, musicians with absolute pitch, and nonmusicians. The two interval-discrimination tasks were a simple two-note contour task and a difficult interval-size discrimination task. Clear effects on the neural waveforms were found for both training and the presence of the absolute pitch ability. In general, training increases the amplitude and shortens the latency of the P3, while the absolute pitch ability reduces the amplitude and shortens the latency, or eliminates the P3 altogether. The absolute pitch effect may be due to the use of a long-term memory strategy involved in the correct performance of the discrimination task rather than performing the task by updating working memory each time a target occurs. Finally, these data are contrasted with those from studies involving sine tones and timbrediscrimination tasks.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Stetsiuk, B. O. "Types of musical improvisation: a classification discourse." Problems of Interaction Between Arts, Pedagogy and the Theory and Practice of Education 57, no. 57 (March 10, 2020): 178–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.34064/khnum1-57.11.

Full text
Abstract:
This article systemizes the types of musical improvisation according to various approaches to this phenomenon. It uses as the basis the classification by Ernst Ferand, which presently needs to be supplemented and clarified. It was stressed that the most general approach to the phenomenon of musical improvisation is its classification based on the layer principle (folklore, academic music, “third” layer). Within these layers, there are various forms of musical improvisation whose systemization is based on different principles, including: performer composition (collective or solo improvisation), process technology (full or partial improvisation), thematic orientation (improvisation theme in a broad and narrow context), etc. It was emphasized that classification of musical improvisation by types is manifested the most vividly when exemplified by jazz, which sums up the development of its principles and forms that shaped up in the previous eras in various regions of the world and have synthetized in the jazz language, which today reflects the interaction between such fundamental origins of musical thought as improvisation and composition. It was stated that the basic principles for classification of the types of musical improvisation include: 1) means of improvisation (voices; keyboard, string, wind and percussion instruments); 2) performer composition (solo or collective improvisation); 3) textural coordinates (vertical, horizontal, and melodic or harmonic improvisation, respectively); 4) performance technique (melodic ornaments, coloring, diminutiving, joining voices in the form of descant, organum, counterpoint); 5) scale of improvisation (absolute, relative; total, partial); 6) forms of improvisation: free, related; ornamental improvisation, variation, ostinato, improvisation on cantus firmus or another preset material (Ernst Ferand). It was stressed that as of today, the Ferand classification proposed back in 1938 needs to be supplemented by a number of new points, including: 1) improvisation of a mixed morphological type (music combined with dance and verbal text in two versions: a) invariable text and dance rhythm, b) a text and dance moves that are also improvised); 2) “pure” musical improvisation: vocal, instrumental, mixed (S. Maltsev). The collective form was the genetically initial form of improvisation, which included all components of syncretic action and functioned within the framework of cult ritual. Only later did the musical component per se grow separated (autonomous), becoming self-sufficient but retaining the key principle of dialogue that helps reproduce the “question-answer” system in any types of improvisation – a system that serves as the basis for creation of forms in the process of improvisation. Two more types of improvisation occur on this basis, differing from each other by communication type (Y. Lotman): 1) improvisation “for oneself” (internal type, characterized by reclusiveness and certain limitedness of information); 2) improvisation “for others” (external type, characterized by informational openness and variegation). It was emphasized that solo improvisation represents a special variety of musical improvisation, which beginning from the Late Renaissance era becomes dominating in the academic layer, distinguishable in the initial phase of its development for an improvising writing dualism (M. Saponov). The classification criterion of “composition” attains a new meaning in the system of professional music playing, to which improvisation also belongs. Its interpretation becomes dual and applies to the performance and textural components of improvisation, respectively. With regard to the former, two types occur in the collective form of improvisation: 1) improvisation by all participants (simultaneous or consecutive); 2)improvisation by a soloist against the background of invariable fixed accompaniment in other layers of music performance. The following types of improvisation occur in connection with the other – textural – interpretation of the term “composition”, which means inner logical principle of organization of musical fabric (T. Bershadska): 1) monodic, or monophonic (all cases of solo improvisation by voice or on melodic wind instruments); 2) heterophonic (collective improvisation based on interval duplications and variations of the main melody); 3) polyphonic (different-picture melodies in party voices of collective improvisation); 4) homophonic-harmonic (a combination of melodic and harmonic improvisations, typical for the playing on many-voiced harmonic instruments). It was emphasized that in the theory of musical improvisation, there is a special view at texture: on the one hand, it (like in a composition) “configures” (E. Nazaikinskyi) the musical fabric, and on the other hand, it is not a final representation thereof, i.e., it does not reach the value of Latin facio (“what has been done”). A work of improvisation is not an amorphous musical fabric; on the contrary, it contains its own textural organization, which, unlike a written composition, is distinguishable for the mobility and variability of possible textural solutions. The article’s concluding remarks state that classification of the types of musical improvisation in the aspect of its content and form must accommodate the following criteria: 1) performance type (voices, instruments, performance method, composition of participants, performance location); 2) texture type (real acoustic organization of musical space in terms of vertical, horizontal and depth parameters); 3) thematic (in the broad and narrow meanings of this notion: from improvisation on “idea theme” or “image theme” to variation improvisations on “text theme”, which could be represented by various acoustic structures: modes, ostinato figures of various types, melody themes like jazz evergreens, harmonic sequences, etc.).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Монич, М. Л. "Contrapuntal Probes As a Special Type of a Music Manuscript." OPERA MUSICOLOGICA, no. 1 (March 15, 2021): 57–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.26156/om.2021.13.1.003.

Full text
Abstract:
В середине 1870-х — начале 1880-х годов С. И. Танеев, воодушевленный идеей поиска «русского стиля», активно пополняет теоретические знания в области контрапункта и практикуется в полифонической работе с национальным мелодическим материалом: песенным фольклором и православными церковными напевами. В результате этой деятельности появляются многочисленные рукописи с контрапунктическими опытами, направленными не только на самообучение, но и на создание музыкальных произведений. Материалом для настоящей статьи стали документы указанного периода творчества композитора — черновые автографы к неоконченному циклу хоровых обработок причастных стихов и к Увертюре на русскую тему, хранящиеся в архиве Государственного мемориального музыкального музея-заповедника П. И. Чайковского в Клину. Танеев работает с напевами причастных стихов и трех песен «Про татарский полон» особым образом: многократно и последовательно испытывает каждый сегмент мелодии различными видами контрапунктической техники, в результате чего получает обширный комплект в основном имитационных, но также неимитационных построений и построений, совмещающих оба вида. Для своего времени танеевский метод и порождаемый им графический текст достаточно специфичны. Подобный тип рукописей предлагается обозначить термином контрапунктические пробы. Ориентируясь на форму, скрепленную полифонической техникой, композитор создает текст, имеющий сходство с уже атрибутированными в отечественной музыкальной текстологии образцами композиционных рукописей: набросками, эскизами, черновиками, учебными работами. Тем не менее, танеевские штудии принципиально от них отличаются и нуждаются в более точном описании и определении. In the mid 1870-s — beginning of 1880-s Sergey Taneev, inspired by the idea of the “Russian style,” began to replenish his theoretical knowledge in the field on counterpoint and to practice polyphonic writing based on national melodic material: folk songs and Orthodox church chants. These studies resulted in numerous manuscript fragments, the purpose of which was not just self-training but there were also drafts for future pieces. The article is based on the documents from this period, including the drafts for unfinished choir arrangements of communion verses and an Overture on the Russian Theme, from the archive of Pyotr I. Tchaikovsky’s Museum in Klin. The way Taneev works with the melodies of the communion verses as well as the songs “About the Tatar Captivity” is very particular: he tries each segment of the melody multiple times consistently applying different types of contrapuntal technique; this resulted in a large set of imitative and non-imitative fragments as well as fragments in which both types of polyphony are combined. For Taneev’s time, such a method and the generated graphic text are unique. We would suggest introducing a term contrapuntal probes to describe it. These texts, determined by the polyphonic forms and techniques, are in many ways similar to the drafts, sketches, and exercises by Taneev which have already been thoroughly examined by Russian specialists. However, there are also essential differences, which implies a more precise and specific description and classification.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Truong, Nhut Mai, Quoc Khai Le, and Quang Linh Huynh. "EEG – based study on sleep quality improvement by using music." Science & Technology Development Journal - Engineering and Technology 3, SI3 (December 2, 2020): First. http://dx.doi.org/10.32508/stdjet.v3isi3.670.

Full text
Abstract:
Napping is essential for human to reduce drowsiness, contribute to improving cognitive function, reflex, short-term memory, and state. Some studies have shown that a certain amount of time for a nap can boost the body's immunity and reduce the danger of cardiovascular disease. Using music for relaxation and enjoyment to fall asleep is an effective solution that earlier studies have shown. There are many genres of music that have been used for stimulation, such as binaural beats or melodic sounds. The aim of the study was to confirm the positive effect of music on sleep quality by analyzing electroencephalography signal. There were four types of music is being used in this study: instrumental music, Ballad music, K-pop music, and Jazz. The study applied the pre-processing include filtering block, features extraction, and clustering steps to analyze raw data. This research calculated the power spectrum of Alpha wave and Theta wave, to detect the transition of wake - sleep stages by K-means clustering algorithm. Sleep latency is one of the factors that determine the quality of sleep. The sleep onset is detected based on the phase shift of the Alpha and Theta waves. The exact timing of the sleep onset was important in this study. The user interface was developed in this study to compute sleep latency in normal and musical experiment. As a result, music is an intervention in helping people fall asleep easier (mean of sleep latency in normal and musical experiment was 9.0714 min and 5.6423 min, respectively) but the standard deviation of this result was rather high due to the little number of experiments. However, the study concludes that listening to music before naptime can improve sleep latency in some participants.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Yu, Yi, Abhishek Srivastava, and Simon Canales. "Conditional LSTM-GAN for Melody Generation from Lyrics." ACM Transactions on Multimedia Computing, Communications, and Applications 17, no. 1 (April 16, 2021): 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3424116.

Full text
Abstract:
Melody generation from lyrics has been a challenging research issue in the field of artificial intelligence and music, which enables us to learn and discover latent relationships between interesting lyrics and accompanying melodies. Unfortunately, the limited availability of a paired lyrics–melody dataset with alignment information has hindered the research progress. To address this problem, we create a large dataset consisting of 12,197 MIDI songs each with paired lyrics and melody alignment through leveraging different music sources where alignment relationship between syllables and music attributes is extracted. Most importantly, we propose a novel deep generative model, conditional Long Short-Term Memory (LSTM)–Generative Adversarial Network for melody generation from lyrics, which contains a deep LSTM generator and a deep LSTM discriminator both conditioned on lyrics. In particular, lyrics-conditioned melody and alignment relationship between syllables of given lyrics and notes of predicted melody are generated simultaneously. Extensive experimental results have proved the effectiveness of our proposed lyrics-to-melody generative model, where plausible and tuneful sequences can be inferred from lyrics.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Bombardirova, Elena P., Milana A. Basargina, Natalia A. Kharitonova, and Mariya D. Mitish. "Music therapy: auxiliary method of habilitation in infants with perinatal pathology during the first months of life." L.O. Badalyan Neurological Journal 1, no. 4 (December 26, 2020): 224–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.17816/2686-8997-2020-1-4-224-231.

Full text
Abstract:
A review of the literature presents the history of the use of music therapy in the treatment of a variety of diseases, both in adult clinical practice and in neonatology and pediatrics; there are described various musical and therapeutic programs considered as a part of complex, specially developed, methods of non-drug rehabilitation, with the presentation of indications and contraindications for this type of therapy in newborns and infants; possible neurophysiological justifications for the use of melodic and rhythmic support of infants with perinatal pathology are presented, as well as the prospects of using music therapy in the practice of nursing newborns, including premature babies, suffering from combined perinatal pathology, in the structure of restorative treatment (habilitation) of children, as an auxiliary method that mildly potentiates the effects of the main methods: physical and medicinal. Currently, in the leading perinatal centers of the country, the opportunity is being created for babies to listen to music as part of multi-sensory developmental care. Specially processed short fragments of instrumental and vocal works by V. Mozart are used in recording, fragments of works of other classical composers, singing lullabies and folk songs performed by his mother. The reliable potentiating effect of musical therapy on adaptation responses of the immature nervous system is established, the connection of enrichment of the external environment with long-term outcomes of the perinatal lesion is discussed. The combination of tactile kangaroo stimulation with the vocal influence of the mother has been proved to have a greater effect than the use of musical fragments in recording, and a strictly individual approach is needed to use music therapy in premature immature infants to avoid undesirable consequences.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Li, Shuyu, Sejun Jang, and Yunsick Sung. "Melody Extraction and Encoding Method for Generating Healthcare Music Automatically." Electronics 8, no. 11 (October 31, 2019): 1250. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/electronics8111250.

Full text
Abstract:
The strong relationship between music and health has helped prove that soft and peaceful classical music can significantly reduce people’s stress; however, it is difficult to identify and collect examples of such music to build a library. Therefore, a system is required that can automatically generate similar classical music selections from a small amount of input music. Melody is the main element that reflects the rhythms and emotions of musical works; therefore, most automatic music generation research is based on melody. Given that melody varies frequently within musical bars, the latter are used as the basic units of composition. As such, there is a requirement for melody extraction techniques and bar-based encoding methods for automatic generation of bar-based music using melodies. This paper proposes a method that handles melody track extraction and bar encoding. First, the melody track is extracted using a pitch-based term frequency–inverse document frequency (TFIDF) algorithm and a feature-based filter. Subsequently, four specific features of the notes within a bar are encoded into a fixed-size matrix during bar encoding. We conduct experiments to determine the accuracy of track extraction based on verification data obtained with the TFIDF algorithm and the filter; an accuracy of 94.7% was calculated based on whether the extracted track was a melody track. The estimated value demonstrates that the proposed method can accurately extract melody tracks. This paper discusses methods for automatically extracting melody tracks from MIDI files and encoding based on bars. The possibility of generating music through deep learning neural networks is facilitated by the methods we examine within this work. To help the neural networks generate higher quality music, which is good for human health, the data preprocessing methods contained herein should be improved in future works.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Carroll-Phelan, Berenice, and Peter J. Hampson. "Multiple Components of the Perception of Musical Sequences: A Cognitive Neuroscience Analysis and Some Implications for Auditory Imagery." Music Perception 13, no. 4 (1996): 517–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/40285701.

Full text
Abstract:
A neurologically plausible model of the auditory perception of musical sequences is proposed, and some implications are derived for auditory imagery. In line with a cognitive neuroscience approach, a componential analysis of the major functions required of auditory perception and imagery for musical sequences is first carried out, and a minimal model of auditory imagery and perception is outlined. The minimal model makes a clear distinction between the processing subsystems required for analyzing pitch and rhythm. It also incorporates an auditory buffer for the brief retention of relatively unprocessed auditory input, an attention subsystem that permits selective intake of relevant auditory information, a memory subsystem for melodic strings that combines the outputs of pitch and rhythmic analyses into a long-term, flexible representation of the musical sequence, and an associative memory system that links the results of perceptual analyses of music with extraperceptual (semantic and episodic) information about the musical string. The present theory builds on a recent model of music perception proposed by Peretz (1993) and also accounts for data from studies of patients with disorders of rhythmic processing, thus indicating the close relationship between auditory perception and imagery for rhythmic sequences, timing, and motor processes. Recent data are used to refine and extend the minimal model and provide an organizing framework for current laboratory work.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Kurniawan, Rama, and Syeilendra Syeilendra. "BENTUK MUSIK SALUANG SIROMPAK VERSI ABAH EMI PADA ACARA ALEK PEMUDA DI PARIK DALAM NAGARI TAEH BARUAH KABUPATEN LIMA PULUH KOTA PAYAKUMBUH." Jurnal Sendratasik 10, no. 1 (December 5, 2020): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.24036/jsu.v9i2.110538.

Full text
Abstract:
This study aims to describe Saluang Sirompak music of Abah Helmi version at Alek Pemuda event in Parik Dalam, Taeh Baruah village, Lima Puluh Kota regency, Payakumbuh City. This research belongs to a qualitative research using a descriptive approach. The main instrument in this study was the researcher itself and was assisted by supporting instruments such as writing tools and cameras. The data were collected through literature study, observation, interview, and documentation. The data analysis was conducted by collecting the data, clarifying the data, and summarizing the data. The results show that Saluang Sirompak of Abah Emi version is a folk performance art which is previously used as a ritual mean. Its song is in the form of a rhyme accompanied by Saluang and Gasiang Tangkurak musical instruments. There is no difference between the previous and current Saluang Sirompak song text anad between the old and present Saluang melodies. The use of the term Saluang Sirompak comes from the word Rompak /Rampok, so the lyrics are related to someone's desire to own someone by force. The musical form of Saluang Sirompak is a combination of Dendang accompanied by two musical instruments: Saluang Sirompak and Gasiang Tangkurak which form a complete unit. The music consists of three parts: part one (opening) of Saluang Sirompak and Gasiang Tangkurak instruments, part two (content) of Dendang accompanied by Saluang and Gasiang Tangkurak instruments, part three (closing) of saluang sirompak and gasiang tangkurak instruments. The music in Saluang Sirompak show has a minimum duration of 20 minutes. It starts with the Gasiang player who screams as a symbol of the beginning of part one. Dendang on Saluang Sirompak music is in the form of rhyme repeated alternately between one singer and another by singing one song, and this is the part two (content) of Saluang Sirompak music.Keywords: Saluang Sirompak music, Abah Emi’s version, Alek Pemuda event
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Larsson, Matz, Joachim Richter, and Andrea Ravignani. "Bipedal Steps in the Development of Rhythmic Behavior in Humans." Music & Science 2 (January 1, 2019): 205920431989261. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2059204319892617.

Full text
Abstract:
We contrast two related hypotheses of the evolution of dance: H1: Maternal bipedal walking influenced the fetal experience of sound and associated movement patterns; H2: The human transition to bipedal gait produced more isochronous/predictable locomotion sound resulting in early music-like behavior associated with the acoustic advantages conferred by moving bipedally in pace. The cadence of walking is around 120 beats per minute, similar to the tempo of dance and music. Human walking displays long-term constancies. Dyads often subconsciously synchronize steps. The major amplitude component of the step is a distinctly produced beat. Human locomotion influences, and interacts with, emotions, and passive listening to music activates brain motor areas. Across dance-genres the footwork is most often performed in time to the musical beat. Brain development is largely shaped by early sensory experience, with hearing developed from week 18 of gestation. Newborns reacts to sounds, melodies, and rhythmic poems to which they have been exposed in utero. If the sound and vibrations produced by footfalls of a walking mother are transmitted to the fetus in coordination with the cadence of the motion, a connection between isochronous sound and rhythmical movement may be developed. Rhythmical sounds of the human mother locomotion differ substantially from that of nonhuman primates, while the maternal heartbeat heard is likely to have a similar isochronous character across primates, suggesting a relatively more influential role of footfall in the development of rhythmic/musical abilities in humans. Associations of gait, music, and dance are numerous. The apparent absence of musical and rhythmic abilities in nonhuman primates, which display little bipedal locomotion, corroborates that bipedal gait may be linked to the development of rhythmic abilities in humans. Bipedal stimuli in utero may primarily boost the ontogenetic development. The acoustical advantage hypothesis proposes a mechanism in the phylogenetic development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Jovanovic, Jelena. "Searching for the right form: A self-taught village player recalling performance live." Muzikologija, no. 8 (2008): 203–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/muz0808203j.

Full text
Abstract:
Repertoire of the excellent self-taught traditional dvojnice-player, Miladin Arsenijevic, from the vicinity of Topola (central Serbia) consists of lyrical songs of a newer rural repertoire. During a 'cognitive interview', in his attempt to recall and reconstruct old-time traveler's (putnicko) playing through performance, a 'real music situation', he has gradually condensed the developed form of the homophonic shepherd's song into the fragmentary form of heterophonic traveler's playing. In this paper the accent is on the player's search and creative process in his attempt to derive the right musical form of a piece, using his long-term memory, since he has not heard or played the piece for quite a long time. It is also a successful attempt to bring musical data from passive to active musical memory, and a transit from one collective semantic musical code to another. The motoric component plays an important role as well. The analysis of musical change shows that musical memory is a distributive system, and data is organized in groups. Musical parameters change: rhythm and tone are changed first, while other parameters seem to depend mostly on the shape of the melodic model; they gradually change while this element finds its right form.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Cabrini, Michele. "Breaking Form through Sound: Instrumental Aesthetics, Tempêête, and Temporality in the French Baroque Cantata." Journal of Musicology 26, no. 3 (2009): 327–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/jm.2009.26.3.327.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Between Lully's death (1687) and Rameau's operatic debut (1733), composers of the tragéédie en musique experimented with instrumental effects, greatly expanding the dramatic role of the orchestra. The profusion of these effects coincides with a new aesthetic reappraisal of instrumental music in France, as can be observed in the writings of Du Bos. The tempêête constitutes one of the most remarkable examples. Its sonic violence was too strong to end with the instrumental movement that depicted it; indeed, composers often prolonged the storm scene into a series of movements all connected by thematic material and key to produce a verisimilar effect of the storm's momentum, thereby creating what I term ““the domino effect.”” By the early eighteenth century, the tempêête had become such a well established and popular topos that it began migrating to non-staged genres like the cantata. The transference of the tempest topos from the tragéédie lyrique to the French baroque cantata entailed the breaking of formal frames. Unlike the supple dramatic structure of French opera, the cantata adopted the more rigid mold of the Italian opera seria——the recitative-aria unit——which separated the flow of time into active and static moments. Three case studies——Bernier's Hipolite et Aricie (1703), Jacquet de la Guerre's Jonas (1708), and Morin's Le naufrage d'Ulisse (1712)——demonstrate how composers manipulated this mold to satisfy a French aesthetic that valued temporal continuity for the sake of verisimilitude. All three composers employ key and instrumental music to portray the storm's forward momentum across recitatives and arias, relying primarily on rhythmic energy and melodic activity to create continuity. Although each composer's musical response varies according to personal style, what emerges is a shared aesthetic and compositional strategy employed to portray an event whose relentless power transcends the temporal boundaries between recitative and aria. This aesthetic of continuity and linearity shown by French baroque composers influenced the treatment of the tempest topos in the later eighteenth-century repertory, vocal and instrumental alike, including opera, the concerto, the overture-suite, and the characteristic symphony.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Fedorak, Dar’ia. "Hildegard of Bingen’s musical work in the aspect of the phenomenon of author’s style." Aspects of Historical Musicology 19, no. 19 (February 7, 2020): 312–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.34064/khnum2-19.18.

Full text
Abstract:
Statement of the problem. Today national musicology is beginning to actively show interest in the study of Western European medieval monody. However, there is still no scientific information about the unique personality of the Middle Ages – Hildegard of Bingen and her musical creativity – in particular about the liturgical drama «Ordo virtutum», although there are some musicology methods for analyzing this music. The relevance of this study is due to the filling of this gap. Taking in account that a musical work of the 11–12th centuries is usually considered only within the context of the “historical style”, it seems interesting to have the opposite approach – to identify the characteristic features of authorship in the work of a medieval composer, whose music is becoming more and more popular in the world concert repertoire. The purpose of the article is to consider the work of Hildegard of Bingen in the aspect of the phenomenon of the author’s style and to identify the invariant features of the individual style model. The liturgical drama “Ordo Virtutum” (“Series of Virtues”) of 1150 by Hildegard of Bingen was chosen as the material for the study, in which several types of art – music, literature and theater are combined, and which is the earliest survived sample of this genre. The “libretto” of the drama is written by Hildegard own and fixed in the so-called “Rizenkodeks” – the majestic manuscript book of 25 pounds, which stored in Wiesbaden Landesbibliothek. The author of this study used the following research methods: historical and contextual due to the need to identify the specifics of creative thinking of Hildegard of Bingen in the context of the theory and practice of the liturgical monody of her time; intonation-dramaturgical analysis aimed at a holistic comprehension of the musical content as such, which is guided by the search for unifying patterns of the intonation plan, and the text-musical semantic analysis of the holy chants fot covering the synergistic aspect of understanding style. Results of the study. Theological themes were the main issues of Hildegard’s life, because from the age of eight she lived and studied in a Benedictine monastery, and later founded her own monastery in Rupertsberg. So, the work of Hildegard of Bingen, along with the music of such well-known, but much younger than her, contemporaries, masters of polyphony, like Leonin and Perotin, provides a unique opportunity to trace the peculiarities of the manifestation of authorship in the monody of the 12th century. The Gregorian chant became a genre that fully embodies the aspirations of the church. However, from the 11th century onwards, secular elements were gradually introduced into church music: from Easter or Christmas tropes, which contained intonations of folk songs, to theatrical episodes based on Scriptures, or “actions” called liturgical drama. The musical drama “Ordo Virtutum” (“A Series of Virtues”) was created to consecrate the Hildegard Convent in Rupertsberg and is impressive primarily because it is the first fully preserved, not fragmentary, liturgical drama. Unlike traditional liturgical drama, the work also surprises with its unusualness and multidimensionality. The text of the drama is related to the themes, characters and prophetic visions presented in one of the main theological works of Hildegard – “Scivias”. As for music, it is a monody, which, thanks to its innovations, significantly expands the tonal and intonational boundaries of music of that time. “Ordo Virtutum” is a Christian philosophical parable dedicated to the struggle for the human soul between the sixteen Virtues (Faith, Hope, Love, Humility, Docility, Innocence, Modesty, Divine Love, Divine Knowledge, Prudence, Patience, Chastity etc.) and the devil. This is the story of a “prodigal daughter” tempted by the devil, who gradually repented and returned with joy to the bosom of the Church. The manuscript of the drama is not divided into actions, but modern editions divide the work into six parts: the prologue, four scenes and the finale. There are a total of 82 different melodies, 80 of which are performed by women. The presence of a large number of female roles (as evidenced by the mostly high register of singing) indicates that the drama “Ordo Virtutum” was composed and performed for the first time in a nunnery. A peculiar struggle takes place between the features of the traditional Gregorian genre, secular influences and signs of Hildegard’s own style of singing, which leads to their synthesis in her compositional work and the opening of new musical horizons. The content of her songs is based on spiritual and cultural context, on the one hand, and personal and psychological attitudes, on the other. Hildegard’s monody is individual in relation to the models of Gregorian chants described in the scientific literature and is unorthodox. Following the text, the melody is divided into lines, which are combined into structural constructions of a higher level – stanzas. The structural and semantic unity of the whole is achieved due to the commonality of melodic motives, and the structure of lines and stanzas is determined by the motive formula. The presence of the above-mentioned integrating principle together with the multiplicity of its incarnations within the unique author’s individuality makes it possible to assert that Hildegard of Bingen’s music is a systemic phenomenon and demonstrates its own compositional style, like the music of Leonin or Perotin. On the example of the analysis of the musical characteristics of different heroes of the work, we see that the liturgical drama “Ordo Virtutum” is not just a collection of typified chorales, as it may seem at first glance. We have before us a real composer opus, endowed with its own unique authorial style, which is “lighting” through each element of this harmonious systemic compositional and semantic integrity. Conclusions. The liturgical drama of Hildegard of Bingen, in fact, was the first, which means that it is advisable to talk about the “phenomenon of a musical work” (the term of N. Gerasimova-Persidskaya), which is inevitably associated with authorship. It was also revealed that the characteristic features that add originality to the musical writing of St. Hildegard are the construction of special short intonational-motive formulas, as well as the frequent use of melismas and musical figures of ascending leaps, extended to an octave. The interaction of these and other qualities forms the uniquely individual author’s style of Hildegard of Bingen, the phenomenon of which lies in his exceptional integrity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Sudirana, I. Wayan. "Improvisation in Balinese Music: An Analytical Study of Three Different Types of Drumming in the Balinese Gamelan Gong Kebyar." Journal of Music Science, Technology, and Industry 1, no. 1 (August 31, 2018): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.31091/jomsti.v1i1.502.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTGong kebyar emerged in early 20th century and was initially an instrumental genre. In its later advancement, the ensemble became accustomed to accompanying dance compositions, which are decorated with miscellaneous dance improvisations corresponding to the characteristic style of gong kebyar. There are three types of Balinese drumming that are considered improvisational, in particular the styles that are played in the repertory of gamelan gong kebyar. Gamelan gong kebyar is the most popular and influential genre of twentieth century music developed in Bali. In gamelan baru, the function of the drum (in Bali it is called kendang) in the ensemble is more important than it was in older styles and it is considered to be the leader of the ensemble. Drummers are skilled musicians and usually teachers, who know all of the parts that are played by other instruments in the ensemble. Krumpungan, Cedugan, and Gupekan are examples of drumming style that gives incorporate the idea of improvisation, though in Bali we do not have a special term for improvisation. We do however have the same ideas and sense of the meaning of improvisation itself in the way some drumming is played spontaneously in the course of performance, by using drummer’s abilities to create spontaneously new pattern on stage. There are some important aspects that need to be underlined in creating those patterns, such as: melody accompaniment, dancer’s cues, good partnership (in krumpungan and cedugan), and the ability to lead the ensemble. Those aspects have the prominent role in the acheivement of drum improvisation in the performance. Drumming improvisation remains something that has to be learned more comprehensively in the future. It is still an abstract concept for many musicians as well as Balinese gamelan lovers.Keywords: improvisation, drumming, kendang, gong kebyar. ABSTRAKGong kebyar muncul pada awal abad ke-20 dan pada mulanya merupakan genre instrumental. Dalam perkembangan selanjutnya, ensambel tersebut menjadi terbiasa dengan komposisi tari yang menyertainya, yang dihiasi dengan improvisasi tari yang berbeda sesuai dengan gaya khas gong kebyar. Ada tiga jenis drum Bali yang dianggap improvisasi, khususnya gaya-gaya yang dimainkan dalam repertoar gamelan gong kebyar. Gamelan gong kebyar adalah genre musik abad ke-20 yang paling populer dan berpengaruh yang dikembangkan di Bali. Dalam gamelan baru ini, fungsi drum (di Bali disebut kendang) dalam ensambel lebih penting daripada pada gaya lama dan dianggap sebagai pemimpin ensembel. Drumer adalah musisi yang terampil dan biasanya adalah guru, yang tahu semua bagian yang dimainkan oleh instrumen lain dalam ensembel. Krumpungan, Cedugan, dan Gupekan adalah contoh gaya drum yang memberikan ide improvisasi, meskipun di Bali tidak ada istilah khusus untuk improvisasi. Namun di sini ada ide yang sama dan rasa makna improvisasi itu sendiri dengan cara memainkan beberapa drum secara spontan dalam jalannya pertunjukan, dengan menggunakan kemampuan pemain drum untuk menciptakan pola spontan baru di panggung. Ada beberapa aspek penting yang perlu digarisbawahi dalam menciptakan pola-pola tersebut, seperti: iringan melodi, isyarat penari, kemitraan yang baik (dalam krumpungan dan cedugan), dan kemampuan untuk memimpin ensambel. Aspek-aspek tersebut memiliki peran penting dalam pencapaian improvisasi drum dalam pettunjukan. Improvisasi drum tetap sesuatu yang harus dipelajari lebih komprehensif di masa depan. Ini masih merupakan konsep abstrak bagi banyak musisi dan juga pecinta gamelan Bali. Kata kunci: improvisasi, drum, kendang, gong kebyar.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Karahan, Ahmet Suat. "Availability of the multiple-choice test method for the exams of Ear Training Courses and its effect on the students' success levelsMüzik Öğretmenliği Programında yer alan Müziksel İşitme Okuma Yazma dersi yazılı sınavlarında çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin kullanılabilirliği ve öğrencilerin başarı düzeylerine etkisinin belirlenmesi." International Journal of Human Sciences 13, no. 1 (March 14, 2016): 1558. http://dx.doi.org/10.14687/ijhs.v13i1.3704.

Full text
Abstract:
Ear Training is a basic course in Music Education Program. The success levels of the students in this course positively affect both their major area courses and their professional achievement levels. In other words, Ear Training courses have a very important place in the training process of a qualified music teacher. However, when various research results are analysed, students' success levels for Ear Training are observed to be unsatisfying. Students' interests in the lesson, their out-of class activities, and the close monitoring of the students' improvement in the lesson are considered as key factors in this case. However, there are mid-term and final exams, not extra quizzes, to evaluate the students in the Ear Training lessons. Quizzes cannot be made because of the reasons such as the teachers' excessive course loads, and long and laborious evaluation processes of written exams. For this reason, the aim of the research is to determine the availability of the multiple-choice tests, which ease the evaluation process, in Ear Training written exams and to determine to what extent they affect the students' success levels in the written exams. In the research in which experimental model was preferred, pre-test and post-test model was used and multiple-choice tests and written exams were compared in terms of their effects on students' success levels of theoretical and practical interval, rhythmic and melodic dictation. SPSS 22 was used in the processing of the data and the results obtained with dependent sample t test were interpreted by the p<.05 level.As a result of the research, it is found out that the experimental group students given multiple-choice test method were more successful in each questions of the theoretical and practical interval, rhythmic and melodic dictation than the control group students given written exams and there was a significant difference according to p<.05 level for the experimental group. The results obtained in the research show that multiple-choice test method could be successfully used for Ear Training written exam and the method increased the students' success levels considerably. Moreover, it was concluded that multiple-choice test method shortened the evaluation process of the written exams and teachers could prepare quizzes with multiple-choice test methods and their evaluation process would be greatly short. ÖzetMüziksel İşitme Okuma Yazma MİOY Müzik Öğretmenliği Programındaki temel bir derstir. Bu ders kapsamında öğrencilerin başarı düzeyleri hem diğer alan derslerini hem de mesleki başarı düzeylerini olumlu yönde etkilemektedir. Yani MİOY dersleri, nitelikli müzik öğretmeni yetiştirme sürecinde çok önemi bir yere sahiptir. Ancak, çeşitli araştırma sonuçları incelendiğinde öğrencilerin MİOY dersi başarı düzeylerinin yetersiz bir seviyede kaldığı görülmektedir. Bu durumun oluşmasında öğrencilerin derse gösterdikleri ilgi düzeylerinin ve ders dışı çalışma süreçlerinin ayrıca öğrencilerin gelişim süreçlerinin yakından izlenmesi önemli etkenler olarak değerlendirilmektedir. Ancak, MİOY derslerinde öğrencilerin genel olarak vize ve final olmak üzere iki sınav ile değerlendirildiği ara sınavların genelde yapılmadığı görülmektedir. Ara sınavların yapılamamasında öğretmenlerin ders yüklerinin fazla olması ve klasik yazılı sınavı değerlendirme sürecinin uzun ve zahmetli olması vb. temel etkenlerdir. Bu sebeple araştırmanın amacı, değerlendirme sürecini önemli ölçüde kolaylaştıran çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin MİOY dersi yazılı sınavlarında kullanılabilirliğini belirlemek ve yöntemin öğrencilerin yazılı sınav başarı düzeylerini ne ölçüde etkilediğini tespit etmektir.Deneysel modelin tercih edildiği araştırmada ön-test son-test model kullanılmış ve çoktan seçmeli test yöntemiyle yapılan MİOY yazılı sınavlarıyla klasik yöntemle yapılan MİOY yazılı sınavları, öğrencilerin teorik ve uygulamalı aralık, akor, tartım ve ezgi diktesi yazma başarı düzeyleri üzerinden değerlendirilmiştir. Ulaşılan verilerin işlenmesinde SPSS 22 Programı kullanılmış, bağımlı örneklem t testiyle elde edilen sonuçlar p<.05 düzeyine göre yorumlanmıştır. Araştırma sonucunda, çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin uygulandığı deney grubu öğrencilerinin klasik yazılı yönteminin uygulandığı kontrol gurubu öğrencilerinden teorik ve uygulamalı aralık ve akor ayrıca tartım diktesi ve ezgi diktesi sorularının her birinde daha başarılı olduğu ve deney grubu yönünde p<.05 düzeyine göre anlamlı fark olduğu belirlenmiştir. Araştırma kapsamında ulaşılan bu sonuçlar, çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin MİOY yazılı sınavı kapsamında başarıyla uygulanabildiğini ve yöntemin öğrencilerin MİOY yazılı sınavı başarı düzeylerini kayda değer ölçüde arttırdığı sonuçlarına ulaşılmıştır. Araştırma sonucunda, çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin yazılı sınav değerlendirme süresini kayda değer bir ölçüde kısalttığı belirlenmiş ve bu sonuca dayalı olarak çoktan seçmeli test yönteminin MİOY derslerinde vize ve final sınavlarına ek olarak ara sınav değerlendirmelerinde kullanılması önerilmiştir.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Dubka, O. S. "Sonata for the trombone of the second half of the 16th – the beginning of the 19th centuries in the context of historical and national traditions of development of the genre." Problems of Interaction Between Arts, Pedagogy and the Theory and Practice of Education 54, no. 54 (December 10, 2019): 55–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.34064/khnum1-54.04.

Full text
Abstract:
The present article is devoted to the general characteristics of the historical process of the formation of the sonata for the trombone (or with the participation of the trombone) in the European music of the Renaissance – Early Classicism era. A particular attention in the research has been paid to the study of the national stylistic, which was the main driving force in the evolution of the trombone at the level of the chamber instrumental and concert genres. It has been noted that since the time of A. Willaert and A. and J. Gabrieli brothers, the trombone and trombone consorts have been the permanent components of the concerts da chiesa, and later – da camera. Due to its construction and melodic-declamatory nature of the sounding, the trombone was in good agreement with both the voices of the choir and other instruments. Gradually, along with collective (concert) varieties of trombone sonatas, solo sonatas with bass began to appear, and they reflected the practice of the Baroque-era concert style. The article reviews a number of trombone sonatas of the Italian, Czech, Austro-German schools, which later became the model for composers of the Newest Time, who fully revealed the possibilities of the trombone semantics and techniques in the sonata genre. The article has noted that the formation of the instrumental sonata in Europe was associated with the practice of concerts in the church, which was for a long time practically the only place where academic music could be performed. The term “sonata” was understood then as the music intended for the instrumental performance, which, however, was closely connected with the vocal one. Therefore, the first samples of sonatas with the participation of the trombone were mixed vocal-instrumental compositions created by the representatives of the Venetian school of the second half of the 16th century – A. Willaert and A. and J. Gabrieli brothers. It has been noted that the key and largely “landmark” composition opening the chronicle of a concert sonata with the participation of trombones was the sonata called “Piano e forte” (1597), where the functions of trombone voices are already beginning to the counterpoint independence, rather than to duplicating the vocal ones. G. Gabrieli is the creator of one of the most large-scale, this time exclusively trombone compositions – “Canzon Quarti Toni” for 12 trombones, cornet and violin – one of the first trombone ensembles based on the genre of canzone as the progenitor of all the baroque instrumental-concert forms. It has been emphasized that among Italian masters of the subsequent period (the early Baroque), the trombone received a great attention from C. Monteverdi, who in his concert opuses used it as the substitute for viola da brazzo (three pieces from the collection called “Vespro della Beata Vergine”). It is noted that in the era of the instrumental versioning, when compositions were performed by virtually any instrumental compound, the trombone was already distinguished as an obligate instrument capable of competing with the cello. Sonata in D minor Op. 5 No. 8 by A. Corelli is considered a model of such a “double” purpose. It has been proved that the Italian schools of the 16th – 17th centuries, which played the leading role in the development of the sonata and concert instrumentalism, mainly the stringed and brass one and the brass one as well, were complemented by the German and Austrian ones. Among the masters of the latter one can distinguish the figure of G. Sch&#252;tz, who created “Fili mi, Absalon” for the trombone quartet and basso-continuo, where trombones are interpreted as instruments of cantilena sounding, which for a long time determines their use in opera and symphonic music, not to mention the sonata genre (introductions and slow parts). Along with the chamber sonata, which was written in the Italian style, German and Austrian masters of the 17th century turn to “tower music” (Tower music), creating their own opuses with almost obligatory participation of one or several trombones. Among such compositions there are the collection by G. Reich called “Quatricinua” of 24 tower sonatas (1696) for the cornet and three trombones, where, modelled on A. Corelli’s string-and-bow sonatas, the plays of a homophonic and polyphonic content are combined. The article notes that the creation of a solo sonata with bass for the trombone was historically associated with the Czech composing school of the second half of the 17th century. The first sample of such composition is the Sonata for the trombone and the thorough-bass (1669), written by a certain monk from the monastery of St. Thomas in Bohemia, where the instrument is shown in a wide range of its expressive possibilities. A significant contribution to the development of a trombone sonata was made by the Czech composer of the late 17th century P. Y. Veyvanovsky, who created a number of sonatas, which, despite the typical for that time performing versioning (trombone or viola da brazzo), were a milestone in the development of the genre in question. The traditions of the trombone sonata-quality genre in its three main expressions – da chiesa, da camera, “tower music” – have been preserved for a certain time in the era of Classicism. This is evidenced, for example, by F. Schneider’s 12 “Tower sonatas” for 2 pipes and 3 trombones (1803–1804). In general, in the classic-romantic era in the evolution of the trombone sonata genre there is a “pause”, which refers to both its collective and solo varieties. The true flourishing of the trombone sonata appeared only in the Newest time (from the end of the 19th century), when the instrumental music of a concert-chamber type declared itself not only as the one demanded by the public, but also as the leading, “title” field of creativity of a number of the leading composers. Among the instruments involved in the framework of the “new chamber-ness” (B. Asafiev) was also the trombone, one of the recognized “soloists” and “ensemblers” of the music from the past eras. The conclusions of the article note that the path travelled by the sonata for the trombone (or with the participation of the trombone) shows, on the one hand, the movement of the instrument to the solo quality and autonomy within the framework of “little-ensemble” chamber-ness (the sonata duet or the solo sonata without any accompaniment), on the other hand, the sustainable preservation of the ensemble origins of this genre (the trombone ensemble, sometimes in combination with other representatives of the brass group).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Stetsiuk, R. O. "Saxophone jazz improvisation: texture and syntax parameters." Problems of Interaction Between Arts, Pedagogy and the Theory and Practice of Education 57, no. 57 (March 10, 2020): 88–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.34064/khnum1-57.06.

Full text
Abstract:
Thisarticle offers a comprehensive overview of the “saxophonejazzimprovisation” phenomenon. It was noted that in the contemporary jazz studies, the components of this notion are, as a rule, not combined but studied separately. This work is the first study that proposes to combine them based on the textureandsyntaxparameters. For that purpose, a number of perceptions already developed in academic music studies have been corrected in this work, including the perception of the instrument’s textural style (A. Zherzdev), specifics of its reflection in improvisation, syntax as a “system of anticipations” (D. Terentiev), which has its own specifics in saxophonejazzimprovisation. Being one of the style “emblems” of jazz, saxophone combines the specifics and universalism of its aggregate sound, which makes its sound image communicatively in-demand. It was emphasized that the methodology and methodic of the topic presented in this work need to be concretized on the example of saxophone jazz styles, which offers prospects for further studies of this topic. The theory of jazz improvisation inevitably includes the question of instrument (instruments, voices) used to make it. At this point, we need to tap into information about the instrumental-type style (style of any types of music according to V. Kholopova) available in jazz practice in both of its historical forms: traditional and contemporary. Saxophone becomes one of the key objects of this study, being an instrument of new type capable of conveying the entire range of jazz intoning shades represented in such origins of jazz as blues, ballad, religious chants, popular “classical music”, academic instruments. To generalize, it is worth noting that information about saxophonejazzimprovisation is concentrated in two areas of study: organological (jazz instruments and their use: solo, ensemble, orchestral) and personal (portraits of outstanding jazz saxophonists made, as a rule, in an overview and opinionbased style). The historical path of saxophone as one of the most in-demand instruments of jazz improvisation was quite tortuous and thorny. The conservative public considered this instrument “indecent” and believed that its use in jazz does not meet the requirements of high taste (A. Onegger). It was emphasized that specifics of jazz saxophone sound indeed lay in the instrumentalization of expressive vocal and declamatory intonations originating from blues with its melancholy and “esthetics of crying”. It is manifested especially vividly, and with even greater share of shock value than in jazz, in the use of saxophone in rock music, which exerted reverse influence over jazz that gave birth to it (V. Ivanov). The timbre-articulatory diversity found in saxophone is identified when taking its organological characteristics out of the dialectics of the pair of notions “specifics – universalism”, where the deepening of the former (specifics) means overcoming thereof towards the latter, universalism (E. Nazaikinskyi). As a result, we have a textural style of saxophone based on melodic nature of this instrument, its specific timbre enriched by the influence of other instrumental sounds, including trumpet, piano, and later, electric guitar. Among the existing definitions of texture in music, there are three key, determinant parameters of the approach to the study of texture style of saxophone in jazz. The first of them is spatial-configurative (E. Nazaikinskyi), the second is procedural-dynamic (G. Ignatchenko), and the third is performance-based (V. Moskalenko). On aggregate, the textural style of jazz saxophone is defined in this article as the synthesis of the instrument’s “voice” and the “voice” of the improviser saxophonist. The former defines the typical in this style, and the latter defines the individual, unique. The specifics of texture in jazz, including saxophone jazz, are special, because this improvisation art does not have the component of final “finishing” of musical fabric. The formulas existing in saxophone jazz texture are divided into three types: specific (typical for jazz itself), specifized (stemming from the folklore and “third” layers), and transduction-reduction (according to S. Davydov, borrowed from the academic layer). The syntactic composition of saxophone jazz improvisation correlates by the textural one, taking the shape of textural-structural components (a term by G. Ignatchenko) – units of the first scaled level of the perception of form, which are related to the one and the other. The mechanism of anticipation – a forestalling perception of the next segment of the process of improvisation, and the intuitionallogical orientation of an improviser saxophonist toward the number “7” have great significance (E. Barban). Like in academic practice, syntax in jazz improvisation is built on the basis of “stability” and “instability” semantics (D. Terentiev), forming a complex system of paradigms and syntagmas (the former are typical for traditional jazz, the latter for contemporary one). The rules of jazz improvisation semantize, because the most important thing for a jazz musician is the process, not the result. At this point, the aspect of temporal distance from the “cause” to the “effect” becomes especially distinguishable: the farther they are from each other the less predictable improvisation becomes, and vice versa. The process of improvisation is largely structured by choruses, which represent sections of a form related to variant reproduction of a theme (standard theme or author’s theme). In addition, improvisation (including saxophone improvisation) may contain elements of general forms of sound used as the bridges connecting sections inside choruses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Zharkova, Valeriya. "Music by Claude Debussy and Maurice Ravel: a Modern View of the Problem of Style Identification." Scientific herald of Tchaikovsky National Music Academy of Ukraine, no. 130 (March 18, 2021): 24–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.31318/2522-4190.2021.130.231181.

Full text
Abstract:
The relevance of the article is determined by the appeal to the debatable issues of stylistic differentiation of the works by Claude Debussy and Maurice Ravel as the French musical culture leading representatives of the late 19th and the first third of the 20th centuries. The research reflections about the connections betwen Debussy and Ravel on the principle “for / against” have not subsided for more than a hundred years. This testifies to the special urgency of this problem and the need to search for modern approaches to understanding the artistic identity of two brilliant contemporaries.Scientific novelty. For the first time, the multidirectionality of the composing strategies by Debussy and Ravel is indicated through the the concept of style in its interdisciplinary philosophicalcategorical status and the explanationof its functions of identification and communication in the general cultural understanding (O. Ustyugova). For the first time the difference between the cultural phenomena processes integration in the era of modernism into the new artistic wholes, with unique properties, which is appropriate to define as “Debussy’s style” and “Ravel’s style”, is revealed.The purpose of the article is to reveal the multidirectionality of the composing strategies of Debussy and Ravel through an appeal to the main stylistic functions of identification and communication in general cultural understanding (O. Ustyugova); to designate the non-coincidence of channels of integration of cultural phenomena in the era of modernism into new artistic wholes, which have unique properties such as “Debussy’s style” and “Ravel’s style”.The research methodology includes the use of historical, stylistic, comparative methods.Main results and conclusions. The existing musicological literature emphasizes the influence of romanticism, post-romanticism, impressionism, symbolism, neoclassicism, Art Nouveau, moderne style on the formation of the individual style of Debussy and Ravel. Each of these directions had a certain reflection in the work of composers. However, let us try to highlight in the conceptual space of the many-sided “isms” of the cultural context of the era of modernism the hidden sources of the deployment of the creative intentions of the both brilliant contemporaries. We will choose the fundamental work of E. Ustyugova “Style and Culture: Experience of Building a General Theory of Style” (2003) as a methodological basis for this. E. Ustyugova proposes to go beyond the understanding style as a “migratory structure” (term by J. Rebane) and a convenient “classification tool” (J. Burnham) in structural and typological studies of art and move on to a comprehensive study of the essence of this phenomenon. For this, according to the researcher, it is necessary to carry out two analytical procedures. The first is based on the awareness of the experience of the mismatch between the object and the subject. The second involves considering the style in the aspect of intersubjective communication.With this view on the problem of identifying the patterns of formation and development of cultural phenomena, it is not the nominative parameters and the “herbarization” of genrelinguistic units that come to the fore, but the comprehension of the multilevel subject-object relations that formed these phenomena; “live reproduction” of the matrix of the world perception as channels of communication between the “I” and everything that appears as “not-I”.The creative paths of Debussy and Ravel represent diferent creative strategies. The “pure meaning”, unspeakable by words and free from all earthly, to which Debussy aspired, creates parallels with the texts of symbolist poets and destroy the boundaries between “I” and “not-I”. In the fundamental monographs of French researchers dedicated to the composer an idea has long been entrenched: the composer’s creative laboratory was poetry, and Debussy’s address to the poetic word throughout all his creative decades constantly expanding the semantic horizons of his “artistic realities”.Debussy’s spiritual intentions merged into a single sound-glow in the indivisible space of being. The word in all its dimensions (from literal to metaphysical) indicated the stages of the process of dissolving the personal “I” and going beyond (au-délà) the established forms of artistic expression. Therefore, various kinds of the names (or “afterwords”, as in the Preludes), epigraphs, numerous super-detailed directions remained an integral part of an integral sound structure. His musical language, destroying the connections in time between the past and the future (rejection of the system of functional gravities that should be “stretched” in musical memory), created a certain correspondence (“here and now”) with the phenomenon of being.Hence the following characteristics of the composer’s musical works: 1) the impeccable construction of the whole, which is “thought out to the smallest detail” (E. Denisov), subtle multilevel “correspondences” and symmetries; 2) total thematization of texture (K. Zenkin); 3) selfsufficient semantic expressiveness of the “pure sound forms” (K. Zenkin), which became the embodiment of “an agonizing thirst for undeniably pure” (S. Velikovsky).These properties of Debussy’s style open up the possibility to get into the spiritual dimensions filled with pure beauty, which so attracted the followers of Baudelaire. Using the typology of teh subject-object relations proposed by E. Ustyugova, Debussy’s style can be attributed throughout the paradigm of hidden subjectivity. Debussy was well aware of his “non-romantic” position.The artistic aspirations of Maurice Ravel more clearly resonate with the creative attitudes of Art Nouveau artists, who were looking for new forms of plastic expressiveness mainly in spatial forms of art. It seems that it is with this direction that a special feeling of the plasticity of the musical material and the entire musical composition as a unique phenomenon is associated, which determines the composer’s creative credo.The concept of “plasticity” indicates such a connection between coordinated phenomena, which appears through the reincarnation (transformation) of a certain material substance, when we keep in memory its output characteristics. Ballet works and the reliance on dance genres (and more broadly, various types of plasticity of gesture and movement) reveal the hidden basis of the composer’s thinking. This approach allows one to re-evaluate Ravel’s connections with the ancient heritage (it is symptomatic that the composer called his first “adult” work, devoted to the press, “Antique Minuet”) and to understand the meanings of constant antique reminiscences with which he filled his life.Like a real dandy who lets the vibrations of the world pass through himself, Ravel is sensitive to them and “cuts off” random, “ugly”, “unnecessary” ones. Hence — the special beauty of the artistic structures created by the composer. They are built not in a “filtered” ideal-beautiful dimension, but in the space of shimmering opposites (the corporeal — free from the corporeal, the familiar — the unknown). Ravel’s inherent tendency towards the graphic relief of the melodic line creates parallels with the “famous lines of Art Nouveau” (Fahr-Becker Gabriele) and is especially distinct, characterizes the composer’s later works.The non-everyday register of semantic reverberations of what is happening in the process of metamorphosis in the composer’s music (his plastic questioning about the existential nature of the source material) demanded a special listener’s responsiveness. Mistifications, hiding behind a mask, playing with the listener are Ravel’s usual communication strategies. Therefore, according to the typology of the subject-object relations proposed by E. Ustyugova, we can speak here of the paradigm of “open subjectivity”, which is characterized by the direct orientation of the subject towards himself. Hence — the principle of auto-citation characteristic of Ravel. The quintessence of its use are the composer’s later works — the opera Child and Magic, as well as the Piano Concerto in G major — the Dandy summa summarum of the composer’s previous career.The game of “correspondences” (Baudelaire) was manifested by composers in various ways and conditioned various channels of communication. Debussy makes the semantics of sound education a semantic unit, appeals to the listener with the expressiveness of the structure itself. Therefore he always emphasizes, appeals to the elite listener. Ravel, on the other hand, hides behind masks and theatrical illusions. He needs a listener who has a culture of distance (who owns wide meaning contextual fields). The contextual layers associated with musical texts express that “degree of distance” from the object of attention, which the composer himself chooses and whose parameters are constantly changing. Therefore, Ravel never turns twice in the genre, style or stylistic model he has already used.So, if the works by Debussy can be perceived “from scratch” because of their structural completeness and semantic tightness, then the works by Ravel require the listener to know the musical context and readiness to lay it out “fold by fold” (J. Deleuze) in new semantic projections.At the turn of the 19th and 20th centuries, French culture was looking for a means of creating a “state of resonance” (G. Bachelard) as an extraordinary impression, “awakening”, without which a person cannot take place. Debussy and Ravel moved in this direction. Therefore, only through the identification of all the “correspondences” of the era of a total change of creative guidelines and a departure from unambiguous stylistic “avatars” can one feel its essential discoveries. The study of the lines of intersection of the Debussy music and the Ravel music with various artistic phenomena of the past and the present illuminates certain reflections of the “style of the era”. However understanding the deep patterns of the creative manner of the two contemporaries requires differentiating the definitions of “Debussy’s style” and “Ravel’s style” and their further studying.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Yunita, Ayu Tresna. "Nasionalisme Eropa dan Pengaruhnya Pada Lagu Seriosa di Indonesia." Resital: Jurnal Seni Pertunjukan 13, no. 2 (November 2, 2013). http://dx.doi.org/10.24821/resital.v13i2.522.

Full text
Abstract:
Gerakan nasionalisme berkembang di Eropa pada tahun 1830 dan menyebar ke berbagai negara di dunia termasuk di Indonesia. Gerakan nasionalisme Eropa pada perkembangannya memberi pengaruh yang besar terhadap perkembangan nasionalisme di kawasan Asia-Afrika khususnya di Indonesia dan perkembangan dalam sejarah musik. Gerakan nasionalisme dalam musik diawali di Rusia lalu kemudian diikuti gerakan nasionalisme di negara-negara Skadinavia, Spanyol, Italia, Hongaria, Inggris dan Amerika Serikat. Nasionalisme Eropa mempengaruhi beberapa komponis dalam menciptakan karya musiknya. Mereka memasukkan unsur-unsur melodi dan syair yang sesuai dengan musik rakyat dan yang sudah dikenal oleh masyarakat mereka. Di Indonesia, nasionalisme membuat para komponis Indonesia menciptakan lagu dengan tujuan mengobarkan semangat berjuang untuk melepaskan diri dari penjajah. Beberapa komponis Indonesia pada waktu jaman itu antara lain, W.R. Supratman, Kusbini, Ismail Marzuki dan Cornel Simanjuntak. Lagu seriosa yang diciptakan para komponis Indonesia mempunyai peranan yang besar terhadap perjuangan mencapai kemerdekaan. Lagu-lagu seriosa yang diciptakan dengan menggunakan ilmu-ilmu musik dari Barat seperti tangganada diatonis, harmoni, struktur bentuk lagu, ritmes dan lain sebagainya merupakan hasil pengaruh musikal dari Barat.Kata kunci: Nasionalisme, pengaruh musikal, lagu seriosaABSTRACTNasionalism in Europ and Its Impact on Indonesian Seriosa Song. The growing of nationalism movements in Europe in 1830 had spread out to all over the world, as well as in Indonesia. It gave considerable influence on the development of nationalism in Asia and Africa, especially in Indonesia, in term of the development in the history of music. The nationalism movement in music began in Russia and then was followed by the movement of nationalism in Scandinavian countries, Spain, Italy, Hungary, the United Kingdom and the United States. European nationalism has affected several composers in creating their music as they incorporate elements of melody and lyric in accordance with folk music which they have been familiar with. In Indonesia, nationalism made Indonesian composers created songs as an expression of their spirit against the Dutch colonial government. Some of Indonesian composers at that time, among others, were WR Supratman, Kusbini, Ismail Marzuki and Cornel Simanjuntak. Seriosa Song composed by Indonesian composers who had an important role to fight for the Indonesian independence. Seriosa songs which are created by using western musical’s standard as diatonic scales, harmony, the structure of a song form, rhyme, and so forth can be said as a result of the western musical influences.Keywords: Nationalism, musical influences, seriosa song
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Draper, Kevin. "Music and Stroke Rehabilitation: A Narrative Synthesis of the Music-Based Treatments used to Rehabilitate Disorders of Speech and Language following Left-Hemispheric Stroke." Voices: A World Forum for Music Therapy 16, no. 1 (February 9, 2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.15845/voices.v16i1.789.

Full text
Abstract:
Stroke is a leading cause of long-term disability. A stroke can damage areas of the brain associated with communication, resulting in speech and language disorders. Such disorders are frequently acquired impairments from left-hemispheric stroke. Music-based treatments have been implemented, and researched in practice, for the past thirty years; however, the number of published reports reviewing these treatments is limited. This paper uses the four elements of the narrative synthesis framework to investigate, scrutinise and synthesise music-based treatments used in the rehabilitation of patients with speech and language disorders. A systematic review revealed that fifteen studies meet the inclusion criteria set out. It was found that the music-based treatments utilised included: Melodic Intonation Therapy (MIT), Modified Melodic Intonation Therapy (MMIT), adapted forms of MIT, the Singing Intonation, Prosody, breathing (German: Atmung), Rhythm and Improvisation (SIPARI) method and a variety of methods using singing and songs. From a synthesis of the data, three themes emerged which were key elements of the interventions; they were: (a) singing songs and vocal exercises, (b) stimulating the right hemisphere and (c) use of speech prosody. These themes are discussed and implications for newly-qualified practitioners are explored.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Shoemark, Helen. "Sweet Melodies: Combining the Talents and Knowledge of Music Therapy and Elite Musicianship." Voices: A World Forum for Music Therapy 9, no. 2 (July 1, 2009). http://dx.doi.org/10.15845/voices.v9i2.347.

Full text
Abstract:
This article reports on a collaborative short-term project between the Melbourne Symphony Orchestra and the Music Therapy team at The Royal Children's Hospital, Melbourne to provide live music on the Neonatal ward of the hospital. The focus was for the music therapists and musicians to develop their understanding and capability to provide live music in an environment for medically fragile patients. To extend the benefit of this experience, guidelines for future musicians were also produced. The guidelines were created through a process of narratives and discussions which were transcribed, and subjected to a thematic analysis. Mid and final review meetings for the whole team encouraged discussion about the emerging themes and final collation of guidelines. The guidelines were intended to offer guidance for new Arts in Health programs or for solo musicians volunteering in a hospital without such a program.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Siswanto, Silo. "Bentuk Lagu Mars FKIP Universitas PGRI Palembang Iringan Musik Format Orkestra." Besaung : Jurnal Seni Desain dan Budaya 5, no. 2 (May 12, 2020). http://dx.doi.org/10.36982/jsdb.v5i2.998.

Full text
Abstract:
<p class="SammaryHeader" align="center"><strong>Abstract</strong></p><p><em>The motto of PGRI Palembang University is that the campus advances with quality. Then a need for faculties to make a stragi in providing encouragement and motivation to the academic community to carry out the tri dharma of higher education. (FKIP) Palembang PGRI University through its highest leadership in the faculty gave a letter of assignment to work on the Mars song FKIP PGRI Palembang University to one of the music art lecturers in the performing arts education study program named Silo Siswanto, M.Sn. Then the Mars song was finished on July 23, 2018 and was launched on July 31, 2018 in the FKIP University of Palembang PGIP judicial event held at the Science Center building at the PGRI University in Palembang. Regarding the form in the Mars song, of course, this song has pieces of melody that are often known by the term phrases or musical sentences that make up the song. while the accompaniment music is arranged in an orchestra format. The formulation of the problem. (1) What is the form of Mars FKIP University PGRI Palembang song. (2) How is the accompaniment of Mars FKIP Palembang PGRI University music with orchestra format. The results of the discussion FKIP march song has a poly metric musical form where the antecedent phrase and consequent phrase do not have the same number of bars for example period C has 9 bars consisting of the phrase d 4 bars and the phrase e 5 bars.</em></p><p><strong><em>Keywords :</em></strong><em> Song Form</em><em>, Mars Song, Orchestra</em></p><p align="center"><strong>Abstrak</strong></p><p><em>Motto Universitas PGRI Palembang yakni kampus melaju dengan mutu. Maka suatu kebutuhan bagi fakultas-fakultas membuat suatu stragi dalam memberikan dorongan dan motovasi kepada civitas akademika untuk melaksanakan tri dharma perguruan tinggi. (FKIP) Universitas PGRI Palembang melalui pimpinan tertingginya di fakultas memberikan surat tugas untuk penggarapan lagu Mars FKIP Univesitas PGRI Palembang kepada salah satu dosen seni musik di prodi pendidikan seni pertunjukan bernama Silo Siswanto, M.Sn. Kemudian lagu Mars tersebut selesai dibuat tanggal 23 juli 2018 dan di launching pada tanggal 31 juli 2018 dalam acara yudisium FKIP Universitas PGRI Palembang bertempat di gedung Science Center Universitas PGRI Palembang. Mengenai bentuk dalam lagu Mars tersebut, tentunya lagu ini memiliki potongan-potongan melodi yang sering dikenal dengan istilah frase atau kalimat musik yang membentuk lagu tersebut. sedangkan musik iringannya diarransemen dengan format orkestra. Adapun rumusan masalahnya. (1) Bagaimana bentuk lagu Mars FKIP Universitas PGRI Palembang. (2) Bagaimana musik iringan lagu Mars FKIP Universitas PGRI Palembang dengan format orchestra. Hasil pembahasan Lagu mars FKIP memiliki bentuk musik poli metrik dimana frase antecedent dan frase consequent tidak memiliki jumlah birama yang sama misalnya periode C memiliki 9 birama yang terdiri dari frase d 4 birama dan frase e 5 birama.</em></p><strong><em>Kata kunci </em></strong><strong><em>:</em></strong><em> Bentuk Lagu, Lagu Mars, Orkestra</em>
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Lavengood, Megan L. "The Cultural Significance of Timbre Analysis." Music Theory Online 26, no. 3 (September 2020). http://dx.doi.org/10.30535/mto.26.3.3.

Full text
Abstract:
This article is in three interrelated parts. In Part 1, I present a methodology for analyzing timbre that combines spectrogram analysis and cultural analysis. I define a number of acoustic timbral attributes to which one may attune when analyzing timbre, organized as oppositional pairs of marked and unmarked terms, in order to both aid in spectrogram analysis and account for some of this cultural and perceptual work. In Part 2, building from Allan Moore’s definition of four functional layers in pop texture, I argue for the adoption of a fifth layer, which I term the novelty layer. I study its construction in 1980s hit singles via the Yamaha DX7 synthesizer. The novelty layer is imbued with several layers of semiotic significance: it functions in opposition to the melodic layer, comprises instruments whose timbral characteristics are more resistant to blending with the rest of the ensemble, and often uses “world instruments” in 1980s popular music. This latter point is a reflection of the problematic treatment of world music by 1980s music culture. I use my approach to timbre analysis to define the timbral norms for the novelty layer as opposed to Moore’s other layers. In Part 3, I create a dialogic narrative analysis of “Do They Know It’s Christmas?” by Band Aid (1984) that demonstrates what it might mean to transgress these norms. This analysis, in acknowledging the problematic cultural associations of the song, illustrates the rich discourse that can be produced when timbre is made central to the analytical process.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Lavengood, Megan L. "The Cultural Significance of Timbre Analysis." Music Theory Online 26, no. 3 (September 2020). http://dx.doi.org/10.30535/mto.26.3.3.

Full text
Abstract:
This article is in three interrelated parts. In Part 1, I present a methodology for analyzing timbre that combines spectrogram analysis and cultural analysis. I define a number of acoustic timbral attributes to which one may attune when analyzing timbre, organized as oppositional pairs of marked and unmarked terms, in order to both aid in spectrogram analysis and account for some of this cultural and perceptual work. In Part 2, building from Allan Moore’s definition of four functional layers in pop texture, I argue for the adoption of a fifth layer, which I term the novelty layer. I study its construction in 1980s hit singles via the Yamaha DX7 synthesizer. The novelty layer is imbued with several layers of semiotic significance: it functions in opposition to the melodic layer, comprises instruments whose timbral characteristics are more resistant to blending with the rest of the ensemble, and often uses “world instruments” in 1980s popular music. This latter point is a reflection of the problematic treatment of world music by 1980s music culture. I use my approach to timbre analysis to define the timbral norms for the novelty layer as opposed to Moore’s other layers. In Part 3, I create a dialogic narrative analysis of “Do They Know It’s Christmas?” by Band Aid (1984) that demonstrates what it might mean to transgress these norms. This analysis, in acknowledging the problematic cultural associations of the song, illustrates the rich discourse that can be produced when timbre is made central to the analytical process.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Başkan, Filiz, Ünsal Doğan Başkır, Erdinç Erdem, and Funda Sarıcı. "A music for all times: Arabesk as a nostalgic commodity for New Turkey’s entertainment industry." European Journal of Cultural Studies, February 24, 2021, 136754942199423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367549421994237.

Full text
Abstract:
This article deals with the relation between recent manifestations of arabesk music (nostalgic arabesk albums by popular figures and Serkan Kaya, a new singer of arabesk) and the transformation of Turkey’s sociocultural climate during the 2010s. Drawing from concepts of nostalgia and neoliberal consumerism, it examines how contemporary arabesk music turned into a retro product or a nostalgic commodity to correspond to consumers’ constant desire for ‘new’. We compare Kaya’s lyrics and melodies with classical arabesk songs to reveal ruptures and continuities between classical and contemporary arabesk. Based on our findings, we initially claim that nostalgic arabesk albums, which simply reproduce a golden arabesk past, embrace restorative nostalgia through turning classical arabesk songs into historical souvenirs rather than reflecting on what contemporary arabesk may become. By the same token, Serkan Kaya’s version of arabesk somehow questions how to interpret this musical trajectory to capture the Zeitgeist and, in this connection, accommodates reflective nostalgia. Since both new manifestations of arabesk in the 2010s are products of/for the entertainment industry, fueled by neoliberal consumer culture, and replicate classical arabesk songs in a nostalgic manner, we argue that they lose their ability to create something new in the complete sense of the term.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Ewell, Philip. "On the Russian Concept of Lād, 1830–1945." Music Theory Online 25, no. 4 (December 2019). http://dx.doi.org/10.30535/mto.25.4.4.

Full text
Abstract:
Universally translated into English as “mode,” the Russian term лад (“lād”) first appeared in 1830 as a translation from German Tonart, which is usually translated into English as “tonality.” To Tchaikovsky a lād was, in fact, a tonality, but by century’s end lād had come to signify its pre-tonal cousin, mode. Boleslav Yavorsky’s work on the subject in the early twentieth century gave lād new post-modal and post-tonal meaning with respect to quasi-tonal and post-tonal music. In this article, I delve deeply into the history of this uniquely Russian concept, from its inception to its highly modified mid-twentieth century form. Rather than trying to find an English equivalent, I leave “lād” in its transliterated form, which disentangles it from inaccurate translations. I examine a 1945 Chopin analysis by Yavorsky’s student, Sergei Protopopov, which outlines new interpretations for Russian lād. Sketches for this analysis, from the Russian National Museum of Music, provide a backdrop for a reexamination of basic tonal constructs such as cadence, phrase, form, harmonic function, and melodic diminution. I then look at a famous 1930 conference on Yavorsky’s theories as an example of the high stakes involved in creating a Marxist musical science, in which lād played a primary role. I also briefly discuss Yavorsky’s theories as a counterweight to Hugo Riemann’s encroaching functionality, which was brought to Russia by Gregori Catoire in the early twentieth century. It is my hope that this work on lād will fill in many gaps for the English-language reader, and possibly spur further studies on this uniquely Russian concept.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Brown, Andrew R. "Code Jamming." M/C Journal 9, no. 6 (December 1, 2006). http://dx.doi.org/10.5204/mcj.2681.

Full text
Abstract:
Jamming culture has become associated with digital manipulation and reuse of materials. As well, the term jamming has long been used by musicians (and other performers) to mean improvisation, especially in collaborative situations. A practice that gets to the heart of both these meanings is live coding; where digital content (music and/or visuals predominantly) is created through computer programming as a performance. During live coding performances digital content is created and presented in real time. Normally the code from the performers screen is displayed via data projection so that the audience can see the unfolding process as well as see or hear the artistic outcome. This article will focus on live coding of music, but the issues it raises for jamming culture apply to other mediums also. Live coding of music uses the computer as an instrument, which is “played” by the direct construction and manipulation of sonic and musical processes. Gestural control involves typing at the computer keyboard but, unlike traditional “keyboard” instruments, these key gestures are usually indirect in their effect on the sonic result because they result in programming language text which is then interpreted by the computer. Some live coding performers, notably Amy Alexander, have played on the duality of the keyboard as direct and indirect input source by using it as both a text entry device, audio trigger, and performance prop. In most cases, keyboard typing produces notational description during live coding performances as an indirect music making, related to what may previously have been called composing or conducting; where sound generation is controlled rather than triggered. The computer system becomes performer and the degree of interpretive autonomy allocated to the computer can vary widely, but is typically limited to probabilistic choices, structural processes and use of pre-established sound generators. In live coding practices, the code is a medium of expression through which creative ideas are articulated. The code acts as a notational representation of computational processes. It not only leads to the sonic outcome but also is available for reflection, reuse and modification. The aspects of music described by the code are open to some variation, especially in relation to choices about music or sonic granularity. This granularity continuum ranges from a focus on sound synthesis at one end of the scale to the structural organisation of musical events or sections at the other end. Regardless of the level of content granularity being controlled, when jamming with code the time constraints of the live performance environment force the performer to develop succinct and parsimonious expressions and to create processes that sustain activity (often using repetition, iteration and evolution) in order to maintain a coherent and developing musical structure during the performance. As a result, live coding requires not only new performance skills but also new ways of describing the structures of and processes that create music. Jamming activities are additionally complex when they are collaborative. Live Coding performances can often be collaborative, either between several musicians and/or between music and visual live coders. Issues that arise in collaborative settings are both creative and technical. When collaborating between performers in the same output medium (e.g., two musicians) the roles of each performer need to be defined. When a pianist and a vocalist improvise the harmonic and melodic roles are relatively obvious, but two laptop performers are more like a guitar duo where each can take any lead, supportive, rhythmic, harmonic, melodic, textual or other function. Prior organisation and sensitivity to the needs of the unfolding performance are required, as they have always been in musical improvisations. At the technical level it may be necessary for computers to be networked so that timing information, at least, is shared. Various network protocols, most commonly Open Sound Control (OSC), are used for this purpose. Another collaboration takes place in live coding, the one between the performer and the computer; especially where the computational processes are generative (as is often the case). This real-time interaction between musician and algorithmic process has been termed Hyperimprovisation by Roger Dean. Jamming cultures that focus on remixing often value the sharing of resources, especially through the movement and treatment of content artefacts such as audio samples and digital images. In live coding circles there is a similarly strong culture of resource sharing, but live coders are mostly concerned with sharing techniques, processes and tools. In recognition of this, it is quite common that when distributing works live coding artists will include descriptions of the processes used to create work and even share the code. This practice is also common in the broader computational arts community, as evident in the sharing of flash code on sites such as Levitated by Jared Tarbell, in the Processing site (Reas & Fry), or in publications such as Flash Maths Creativity (Peters et al.). Also underscoring this culture of sharing, is a prioritising of reputation above (or prior to) profit. As a result of these social factors most live coding tools are freely distributed. Live Coding tools have become more common in the past few years. There are a number of personalised systems that utilise various different programming languages and environments. Some of the more polished programs, that can be used widely, include SuperCollider (McCartney), Chuck (Wang & Cook) and Impromptu (Sorensen). While these environments all use different languages and varying ways of dealing with sound structure granularity, they do share some common aspects that reveal the priorities and requirements of live coding. Firstly, they are dynamic environments where the musical/sonic processes are not interrupted by modifications to the code; changes can be made on the fly and code is modifiable at runtime. Secondly, they are text-based and quite general programming environments, which means that the full leverage of abstract coding structures can be applied during live coding performances. Thirdly, they all prioritise time, both at architectural and syntactic levels. They are designed for real-time performance where events need to occur reliably. The text-based nature of these tools means that using them in live performance is barely distinguishable from any other computer task, such as writing an email, and thus the practice of projecting the environment to reveal the live process has become standard in the live coding community as a way of communicating with an audience (Collins). It is interesting to reflect on how audiences respond to the projection of code as part of live coding performances. In the author’s experience as both an audience member and live coding performer, the reception has varied widely. Most people seem to find it curious and comforting. Even if they cannot follow the code, they understand or are reassured that the performance is being generated by the code. Those who understand the code often report a sense of increased anticipation as they see structures emerge, and sometimes opportunities missed. Some people dislike the projection of the code, and see it as a distasteful display of virtuosity or as a distraction to their listening experience. The live coding practitioners tend to see the projection of code as a way of revealing the underlying generative and gestural nature of their performance. For some, such as Julian Rohrhuber, code projection is a way of revealing ideas and their development during the performance. “The incremental process of livecoding really is what makes it an act of public reasoning” (Rohrhuber). For both audience and performer, live coding is an explicitly risky venture and this element of public risk taking has long been central to the appreciation of the performing arts (not to mention sport and other cultural activities). The place of live coding in the broader cultural setting is still being established. It certainly is a form of jamming, or improvisation, it also involves the generation of digital content and the remixing of cultural ideas and materials. In some ways it is also connected to instrument building. Live coding practices prioritise process and therefore have a link with conceptual visual art and serial music composition movements from the 20th century. Much of the music produced by live coding has aesthetic links, naturally enough, to electronic music genres including musique concrète, electronic dance music, glitch music, noise art and minimalism. A grouping that is not overly coherent besides a shared concern for processes and systems. Live coding is receiving greater popular and academic attention as evident in recent articles in Wired (Andrews), ABC Online (Martin) and media culture blogs including The Teeming Void (Whitelaw 2006). Whatever its future profile in the boarder cultural sector the live coding community continues to grow and flourish amongst enthusiasts. The TOPLAP site is a hub of live coding activities and links prominent practitioners including, Alex McLean, Nick Collins, Adrian Ward, Julian Rohrhuber, Amy Alexander, Frederick Olofsson, Ge Wang, and Andrew Sorensen. These people and many others are exploring live coding as a form of jamming in digital media and as a way of creating new cultural practices and works. References Andrews, R. “Real DJs Code Live.” Wired: Technology News 6 July 2006. http://www.wired.com/news/technology/0,71248-0.html>. Collins, N. “Generative Music and Laptop Performance.” Contemporary Music Review 22.4 (2004): 67-79. Fry, Ben, and Casey Reas. Processing. http://processing.org/>. Martin, R. “The Sound of Invention.” Catapult. ABC Online 2006. http://www.abc.net.au/catapult/indepth/s1725739.htm>. McCartney, J. “SuperCollider: A New Real-Time Sound Synthesis Language.” The International Computer Music Conference. San Francisco: International Computer Music Association, 1996. 257-258. Peters, K., M. Tan, and M. Jamie. Flash Math Creativity. Berkeley, CA: Friends of ED, 2004. Reas, Casey, and Ben Fry. “Processing: A Learning Environment for Creating Interactive Web Graphics.” International Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques. San Diego: ACM SIGGRAPH, 2003. 1. Rohrhuber, J. Post to a Live Coding email list. livecode@slab.org. 10 Sep. 2006. Sorensen, A. “Impromptu: An Interactive Programming Environment for Composition and Performance.” In Proceedings of the Australasian Computer Music Conference 2005. Eds. A. R. Brown and T. Opie. Brisbane: ACMA, 2005. 149-153. Tarbell, Jared. Levitated. http://www.levitated.net/daily/index.html>. TOPLAP. http://toplap.org/>. Wang, G., and P.R. Cook. “ChucK: A Concurrent, On-the-fly, Audio Programming Language.” International Computer Music Conference. ICMA, 2003. 219-226 Whitelaw, M. “Data, Code & Performance.” The Teeming Void 21 Sep. 2006. http://teemingvoid.blogspot.com/2006/09/data-code-performance.html>. Citation reference for this article MLA Style Brown, Andrew R. "Code Jamming." M/C Journal 9.6 (2006). echo date('d M. Y'); ?> <http://journal.media-culture.org.au/0612/03-brown.php>. APA Style Brown, A. (Dec. 2006) "Code Jamming," M/C Journal, 9(6). Retrieved echo date('d M. Y'); ?> from <http://journal.media-culture.org.au/0612/03-brown.php>.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Stover, Chris. "Musical Bodies: Corporeality, Emergent Subjectivity, and Improvisational Spaces." M/C Journal 19, no. 1 (April 6, 2016). http://dx.doi.org/10.5204/mcj.1066.

Full text
Abstract:
IntroductionInteractive improvisational musical spaces (which is to say, nearly all musical spaces) involve affective relations among bodies: between the bodies of human performers, between performers and active listeners, between the sonic "bodies" that comprise the multiple overlapping events that constitute a musical performance’s unfolding. Music scholarship tends to focus on either music’s sonic materialities (the sensible; what can be heard) or the cultural resonances that locate in and through music (the political or hermeneutic; how meaning is inscribed in and for a listening subject).An embodied turn, however, has recently been manifesting, bringing music scholarship into communication with feminist theory, queer theory, and approaches that foreground subjectivity and embodiment. Exemplary in this area are works by Naomi Cumming (who asks a critical question, “does the self form the sound, or the sound the self?;” Cumming 7), Suzanne Cusick, Marion Guck, Fred Maus, and Susan McClary. All of these scholars, in various ways, thematise the performative—what it feels like to make or experience music, and what effect that making or experiencing has on subject-formation.All of these authors strive to foreground the role of the performer and performativity in the context of the extended Western art music tradition. While each makes persuasive, significant points, my contention in this paper is that improvised music is a more fruitful starting place for thinking about embodiment and the co-constitutive relationship between performer and sound. That is, while (nearly) all music is improvised to a greater or lesser degree, the more radical contexts, in which paths are being selected and large-scale shapes drawn in the “heat of the moment,” can bring these issues into stark relief and serve as more productive entry points for thinking through crucial questions of embodiment, perspective, identity, and emergent meaning.Music-Improvisational ContextsA musical improvisational space is a “context,” in Lawrence Grossberg’s sense of the term (26), where acts of territorialisation unfold an ongoing process of meaning-constitution. Territorialisation refers to an always-ongoing process of mapping out a space within which subjects and objects are constituted (Deleuze and Guattari 314). I posit that musical acts of territorialising are performed by two kinds of bodies in mutually constitutive relationships: interacting corporeal performing bodies, with individual pasts, tendencies, wills, and affective attunements (Massumi, Semblance), and what I term musical-objects-as-bodies. This second category represents a way of considering music’s sonic materiality from an affective perspective—relational, internally differentiating, temporal. On the one hand musical-objects-as-bodies refer to the materiality of the now-ongoing music itself: from the speeds and slownesses of air molecules that are received by the ear and interpreted as sound in the brain, to notes and rhythms and musical gestures; to the various ways in which abstract forms are actively shaped by performers and interpreted by listeners, with their own individuated constellations of histories, tendencies, wants, attunements, and corporeal perspectives. On the other hand, musical-objects-as-bodies can refer to the histories, genres, dislocations, and nomadic movements that partially condition how sonic materialities are produced and perceived. These last two concepts should be read both in terms of how histories and genres become dislocated from themselves through the actions of practitioners, and as a priori principles—that is, not as aberrations that disrupt a norm, but as norms themselves.This involves two levels of abstraction: ascribing body-status to sound-complexes, and then doing the same for historical trajectories, cultural conditionings, and dislocations. Elizabeth Grosz asks us to theorise the body as “the threshold or borderline concept that hovers perilously and undecidably at the pivotal joint of binary pairs” (Grosz, Volatile 23); one such binary that is problematised is that of production and perception, which within the context of an improvising music ensemble are really two perspectives on the same phenomenon. The producers are also the perceivers, in other words. This is true of listeners too: acts of perception are themselves productive in the sense that they create contexts in which meanings emerge.In Gilles Deleuze and Félix Guattari’s language (46–54), an emerging context represents a plurality of milieux that are brought together in acts of territorialisation (and deterritorialisation; see below). The term “milieu” refers to the notion that acts of territorialisation always take place in the middle—they are always already bound up in ongoing processes of context-building. Nothing ever emerges from whole cloth; everything modifies by differential degree the contexts upon which it draws. In musical contexts, we might consider four types of milieux. External milieux are articulated by such factors as syntactic norms (what makes a piece of music sound like it belongs within a genre) and cultural conditionings. Internal milieux refer to what gives the elements of a piece of music a sense of belonging together, including formal designs, motivic structures, and melodic or harmonic singularities. An intermediary milieu involves the way gestures acquire sign-status in a context, thereby becoming meaningful. Annexed milieux are locations where new materials are absorbed and incorporated from without.Bodies ImprovisingA small example should put these points into focus. Four jazz musicians are on stage, performing a version of the well-known (in that community) song “Stella by Starlight.” External milieux here include the conventions of the genre: syntactic expectations, prescribed roles for different instruments, certain perspectives on historical performance practices. Internal milieux include the defining features of this song: its melody, harmonic progression, formal design. The performers’ affective attunements to the history of the song’s complex life so far form an intermediary milieu; note that that history is in a process of modification by the very act of the now-ongoing performance. Annexed milieux might include flights into the unexpected, fracturings of stylistic norms, or incorporations of other contexts into this one. The act of territorialisation is how these (and more) milieux are drawn together as forces in this performance, this time. Each performer is an agent, articulating sounds that represent the now-emerging object, this “Stella by Starlight.” Those articulated sounds, as musical-objects-as-bodies, conjoin with each other, and with performers, in ongoing processes of subject-formation.A double movement is at play in this characterisation. The first is strategic: thinking of musical forces as bodies in order to consider how relationships unfold between them in embodied terms—in terms of affect. But simultaneous with this is a reverse move that begins with affective forces and from there constructs those very bodies—human performing bodies as well as musical-objects-as-bodies. In other words, in order to draw lines between bodies that suggest contextual co-determinations where each exists in a continual process of engendering the other, we can turn to a consideration of the encounters between, and impingements of, affective forces through which bodies are constructed and actions are mobilised. This double movement is a paradox that requires three presuppositions. First, that bodies are indeed constituted through encounters of affective forces—this is Deleuze’s Spinozist claim (Deleuze, Spinoza 49–50). Second, that identity is performative within the context of a discourse. This is Judith Butler’s position, which I modify slightly to consider the potential of non- (or pre-) linguistic discourse, such as what can stem from drastic (active, experiential) music-syntactic spaces (Abbate). And third, that concepts like agency and passivity involve force-relations between human actors (with embodied perspectives, agencies, histories, tendencies, and diverse ranges of affective attunements), and the musical utterances expressed by and between them. Therefore, there is value in considering both actor and utterance as unfolding along the same plane, each participating in the other’s constitution.What is at stake when we conceive of sonic materiality in bodily terms in this way? The sounds produced in interactive music-improvisational settings are products of human agency. But there is a passive element to human musical-sound production. There is a degree of passivity that owes to learned behaviors, habits, and the singularities of one’s own history—this is the passive nature of Deleuze’s first synthesis of time (Deleuze, Difference 71–79), where past experiences and activities are drawn into a now-present action, partially conditioning it. Even overtly active selection in the living present is founded on this passivity, since one can only draw upon one’s own history and experience, which provides a limiting force on technique, which in turn directs expressive possibilities. In music-improvisation pedagogy, this might be phrased as “you can only play what you can hear.” Another way to say this is that passive synthesis conditions active selection.One way to overcome the foreclosure of possibility that necessarily falls out of passive synthesis is through interaction and engagement with the affective forces at play in interactive encounters. Through encounters, conditions for new possibilities emerge. The limiting concept “you can only play what you hear” is mitigated by an encounter with newly received stimuli: a heard gesture that invites further excavation of a motivic idea or that sparks a “line of flight” into a thus-far unthought-of next action. The way a newly received stimulus inspires new action is an affective encounter, and it re-conditions—it deterritorialises—the ongoing process of subject-formation. The encounter is a direct line drawn between the two types of bodies—that is, between the situated body of a producing and perceiving subject and the sonic materiality of a musical-object-as-body. While there are other kinds of encounters that unfold in the course of interactive musical performance (visual cues, for example, or tactile nearnesses), the events of heard sounds are the primary locations where bodies are constituted or subjects are formed. This is made transparent in a recent study by Schober and Spiro, where jazz musicians improvised together with no visual or tactile connection, relying solely on sound for their points of interactive contact. This suggested that jazz musicians are able to communicate effectively with only sonic data exchanged. That many improvisers play with their eyes closed, or with their backs to one another, only reinforces this.There are three aspects of sound that I wish to offer as support for a reading of musical objects as bodies. First is that sounds are temporally articulated and perceived. The materiality of sound is bound up with its temporality in ways that are more directly perceivable than many other worldly materialities. The obviousness of its temporally bound nature is one reason that music is used so often as an entry point for thinking through the ontological nature of time and process; viz. Husserl’s utilisation of musical melodies to explicate his phenomenology of internal time-consciousness, and Deleuze and Guattari’s location of acts of territorialisation in the (musical) refrain. Of course the distinction between sonic and other materialities is only a matter of degree: all matter, including bodies, is “continually subjected to transformation, to becoming, to unfolding over time” (Grosz, Time 79), but music foregrounds temporality in ways that many philosophers have found vivid and constructive.Second, musical sounds acquire meaning through their relationships with other sounds in contexts, both in the immediate context of the now-ongoing performance and in extended contexts of genre, syntax, and so on. Those relationships are with histories of past sounds, now-ongoing sounds, and future sounds expressed as results of accumulations of meaning-complexes. A gesture is played, and it acquires meaning through the ways it is “picked up” by differently attuned performers and listeners.In this sense, third, the line is blurred between action and agent; the distinction between the gesture and the execution of the gesture is effectively erased. From the performer’s perspective, how a gesture is “picked up” is made somewhat evident by the sonic materiality of the next gesture. This next gesture is a sign that represents the singularity of the performer’s affective attunement, or an expression of a stage (or, better, some now-ongoing aspect) of what Whitehead would call her “eventful” subjectivity (166–167). What is expressed is the way the performer is (actively or passively) attuning to the constellations of meanings that resonate in the event of the encounter with the musical-object-as-body, as that musical-object-as-body in turn expresses the history of past encounters that (actively or passively) engendered it. The present action as most-contracted expression of the past is Deleuze’s second synthesis of time, while the eventful way an action cuts into the future marks the time of his third synthesis (Deleuze, Difference 80–91).What is at stake in a turn to corporeality in music analysis? Nietzsche admonishes us to turn from the “facts” that the senses take in, process, and evaluate and re-begin our inquiry by questioning the body (272). This means, for music analysis, turning away from certain quantifiable aspects of sonic materiality (pitches, chords, rhythms, formal designs), towards the ways in which sounds are articulated by bodies in interactive contexts. This has been attempted from various perspectives in recent music scholarship, but again the reading of musical bodies I am pursuing foregrounds affective forces, eventful subject-formation, and performativity as identity, on the ground of improvised interaction. Improvising bodies engage in spaces where “all kinds of affects play their game” (Nietzsche 264), and they exist in constant states of change as they are impinged on by events (and as they impinge on events), those events also forming conduits to other bodies. Subjects are not just impinged on by events; they are events, processes, accumulations, and distributions of affective forces. As Grosz puts it, “the body codes the meanings projected onto it” (Volatile 18). In musical improvisation, performers are always in the process of becoming a subject, conditioned by the ways in which they are impinged upon by affective forces and the creative ways those impingements are taken up.Musical-objects-as-bodies, likewise, unfold as ongoing processes, their identity emerging through accumulations and distributions of relationships with other musical-objects-as-bodies. A musical gesture acquires meaning through the emerging context in which it participates, just as a performer acquires a sense of identity through acts of production and perception in, and that help create, a context. Moreover, an affective consideration of performer (as corporeal body) and musical gesture (as sonic utterance) involves “the torsion of one into the other, the passage, vector, or uncontrollable drift of the inside into the outside and the outside into the inside” (Grosz, Volatile xii). Grosz is describing the essential irreducibility of body and mind, but her language is compelling for thinking through the relationships between bodies and musical-objects-as-bodies as an ongoing co-constitutive, boundary-dissolving process.Bodies and/as AffectAffect begins in the in-between, in the productive space of the event in which bodies encounter one another. This is not, however, a pure in-between. Bodies are constructed by the ways in which affective forces impinge on them, but affective forces also stem from bodies. Bodies affect and are affected by one another, as Deleuze is fond of repeating (Spinoza 49). No affect, no bodies, but also no bodies, no affect. What does this mean? The in-between does not subvert corporeality, perspective, intention, or subjectivity, nor is there a hierarchical relation between them (that is, bodies do not emerge because of affective relations, nor the reverse). If we think of bodies as emergent subjectivities—as processes of subject-formation irreducibly connected to the ecological conditions in which they are acting—then the ways in which their identities come to be constructed are intricately connected to the performative utterances they are making and the variable ways they are taking up those utterances and folding them into their emergent processes of becoming. Here, the utterer–utterance distinction begins to break down. Judith Butler (24-25) argues that the ways in which bodies are defined emerge from performative acts, and that every such act constitutes a political action that contributes to the constitution of identity. As Butler writes, “that the gendered body is performative suggests that it has no ontological status apart from the various acts which constitute its reality” (136). Gender is a status that emerges through one’s actions in contexts—we perform gender, and by performing it we undergo a process of inscribing it on ourselves. This is one of many key points where music scholarship can learn from feminist theory. Like gender, musical identity is performed—we inscribe upon ourselves an emergent musical subjectivity through acts of performance and perception (which is itself a performance too, as an interaction with a musical-object-as-body).Performative acts, therefore, are not simply enacted by bodies; if identity is performed, then the acts themselves are what define the very bodies performing them. Again, the hierarchy breaks down: rather than beginning with a body (a subject) that acts, actions comprise what a body is, as an emergent subject, as the product of its actions. For Deleuze and Guattari, performed acts involve masks; masks do not disguise expression or identity but rather are expressions through which identity is drawn. “The mask does not hide the face, it is the face” (115); “the mask assures the […] construction of the face, the facialization of the head and the body: the mask is now the face itself, the abstraction or operation of the face. […] Never does the face assume a prior signifier or subject” (Deleuze and Guattari 181). In Butler’s terms, the performance does not presuppose the performer; the performer is the performance.Affect corresponds, then, not only to the pre-linguistic (Deleuze’s “dark precursor;” Difference 119–121) but also to the super-discursive: to the multiple embedded meaning-trajectories implicit in any discursive utterance; to the creative ways in which those meaning-trajectories can be taken up variably within the performance space; to the micro-political implications of both utterance and taking-up. Bergson writes: “[m]y body is […] in the aggregate of the material world […] receiving and giving back movement, with, perhaps, this difference only, that my body appears to choose, within certain limits, the manner in which it shall restore what it receives” (Bergson 4–5; also cited in Grosz, The Nick 165). This is exactly Grossberg’s “context,” by the way. The “manner in which it shall restore what it receives” refers, in the case of musically performing (corporeal) bodies, to how a gesture is taken up in a next performed action. In the case of musical-objects-as-bodies, conversely, it refers to how a next gesture contributes to the ongoing sense of meaning-accumulation in response to the ongoing flux of musical-objects-as-bodies within which it locates.In music-improvisational spaces, not only does the utterer–utterance, agent–action, or performer–­performed gesture distinction break down, but the distinction between performed and received gesture likewise blurs, in two senses: because of the nature of eventful subject-formation (whereby a musical gesture’s meaning is being drawn within its emergent context), and because the events of individual musical gestures are subsumed into larger composite events. This problematises the utterer-utterance breakdown by blurring the threshold between individual performed events, inviting a consideration of a paradoxical, but productive, excluded middle where musical-objects-as-bodies are both expressions of corporeal performative acts (engendering contextual subject-formations) and constituent elements of an emergent musical subjectivity (“the performance.” See Massumi (Parables) for more on productive engagements with the excluded middle). While beyond the scope of this paper, we might consider the radical co-constitution of different kinds of bodies in this way as a system, following Gregory Seigworth’s description: “the transitive effect undergone by a body (human or otherwise) in a system—a mobile and open system—composed of the various, innumerable forces of existing and the relations between those forces” (161).Performing Bodies and the Emergent WorkThis, ultimately, is my thesis: how to think about musical performance beginning with performing bodies rather than with a reified notion of musical materiality. Performing bodies are situated within the emerging context of improvised, interactive music-making. Musical utterances are enacted by those bodies, which are also taking up the utterances made by other bodies—as musical-objects-as-bodies. The context that is being built through this process of affective exchange is the performance (the this performance, this time of the jazz example above). Christopher Hasty writes,to perform, from per-formare is to really, actually (fully) form or shape. The ‘-ance’ of performance connotes action and process. The thing performed apart from or outside the forming is problematic. Is it a fixed, ideal form above or beyond (transcending), or beneath or behind (founding) the actual doing, a thing that can be known quite apart from the situated knowing itself? (200)The work–performance dichotomy that animates Hasty’s question (as well as those of Abbate, Goehr, and others) is not my question, since I suggest that using improvised music as an entry point into musical inquiry makes a turn to performance axiomatic. The improvised work is necessarily an active, emergent process, its particularities, boundaries, and meanings being drawn through its performed actions. Perhaps the question that underlies my query is, instead, how do we think about the processes of subject-formation that unfold through interactive music-making; how are performing and performed bodies being inscribed through what kinds of relationships with musical materialities?Is there, in the end, simply a musical body that subsumes both utterer and utterance, both subjectively-forming body and material sonic gesture? I do not wish to go quite that far, but I do wish to continue to problematise where one body stops and the next begins. To paraphrase one of themes of this special issue, where do the boundaries, thresholds, and intersections of musical bodies lie? Deleuze, following Spinoza, tells us frequently that we do not yet know what a body is capable of. This must be at least in part because we know not what a body is at any given point—the body, like the subject which we might now think of as no more than a sign, is in a process of becoming; there is no is (ontology), there is only and (conjunction). And there is no body, there are only bodies, for a body only exists in a complex and emergent ecological relationship with other bodies (see Grosz, Volatile 19). To conceive of porous thresholds between performing bodies and musical-objects-as-bodies is to foreground the performative aspects of improvised music-making and to break down the hierarchy, and possibly even the distinction, between agent, action, and the content of that action. Bodies of all types inscribe one another in ongoing acts of meaning-constitution: this is the properly drastic starting place for inquiry into the nature of musical process.ReferencesAbbate, Carolyn. “Music—Drastic or Gnostic?” Critical Inquiry 30.3 (2004): 505–536.Bergson, Henri. Matter and Memory. Trans. Nancy Margaret Paul and W. Scott Palmer. London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd., 1919.Butler, Judith. Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. New York: Routledge, 1990.Cumming, Naomi. The Sonic Self: Musical Subjectivity and Signification. Bloomington, IN: Indiana UP, 2000.Cusick, Suzanne. “Feminist Theory, Music Theory, and the Mind/Body Problem.” Perspectives of New Music 32.1 (1994): 8–27.———. “On Musical Performances of Gender and Sex.” Audible Traces: Gender, Identity, and Music. Eds. Elaine Barkin and Lydia Hamessley. Zurich: Carciofolo Verlagshaus, 1999. 25–48.Deleuze, Gilles. Spinoza: Practical Philosophy. Trans. Robert Hurley. Eugene, OR: City Lights Books, 1988.———. Difference and Repetition. Trans. Paul Patton. New York: Columbia UP, 1994.Deleuze, Gilles, and Félix Guattari. A Thousand Plateaus: Capitalism and Schizophrenia. Trans. Brian Massumi. Minneapolis: U of Minnesota P, 1987.Goehr, Lydia. The Quest for Voice: On Music, Politics, and the Limits of Philosophy. Oxford: Oxford UP, 1998.Grossberg, Lawrence. Cultural Studies in the Future Tense. Durham: Duke UP, 2010.Grosz, Elizabeth. Volatile Bodies: Toward a Corporeal Feminism. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, 1994.———. The Nick of Time: Politics, Evolution, and the Untimely. Durham: Duke UP, 2004.———. Time Travels: Feminism, Nature, Power. Durham: Duke UP, 2005.Guck, Marion. “A Woman’s (Theoretical) Work.” Perspectives of New Music 32.1 (1994): 28–43.Hasty, Christopher. “If Music Is Ongoing Experience, What Might Music Theory Be? A Suggestion from the Drastic.” Zeitschrift der Gesellschaft für Musiktheorie (Sonderausgabe 2010): 197–216.Husserl, Edmund. On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time. Trans. John Barnett Brough. Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1991.Massumi, Brian. Parables for the Virtual: Movement, Affect, Sensation. Durham and London: Duke UP, 2002.———. Semblance and Event: Activist Philosophy and the Occurent Arts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2011.Maus, Fred Everett. “Musical Performance as Analytic Communication.” Performance and Authenticity in the Arts. Eds. Salim Kemal and Ivan Gaskell. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 1999. 129–153.McClary, Susan. “Constructions of Subjectivity in Schubert’s Music.” Queering the Pitch: The New Gay and Lesbian Musicology. Ed. Philip Brett, Elizabeth Wood, and Gary C. Thomas. New York: Routledge, 2006. 205–234.Nietzsche, Friedrich. The Will to Power. Trans. Walter Kaufmann and Reginald John Hollingdale. New York: Vintage Books, 1967.Schober, Michael, and Neta Spiro. “Jazz Improvisers’ Shared Understanding: A Case Study.” Frontiers in Psychology 5 (2014). 10 Mar. 2016 <http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00808/abstract>.Seigworth, Gregory. “From Affection to Soul.” Gilles Deleuze: Key Concepts. Ed. Charles J. Stivale. Montreal: McGill–Queens UP, 2005. 159–169.Whitehead, Alfred North. Process and Reality. New York: Free Press, 1978.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography