To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Need for closure.

Journal articles on the topic 'Need for closure'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Need for closure.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Hollings, Christopher D. "The need for closure." Mathematical Gazette 103, no. 557 (2019): 248–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/mag.2019.56.

Full text
Abstract:
When defining a group, do we need to include closure? This is a detail that is often touched upon when the notion of a group is introduced to undergraduates. Should closure be listed as an axiom in its own right, or should it be regarded as an inherent property of the binary operation? There is no clear answer to this question, although there are firm opinions on both sides. Indeed, a very brief survey of group theory textbooks found in [1, pp. 458-459] suggests that there is a rough 50 : 50 split between authors who include closure explicitly and those who do not. In this Article, we go back to the beginning of the twentieth century to provide some historical perspective on this problem.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Theodorou, Annalisa, and Ankica Kosic. "Need for Closure, Morality, and Prejudice." Social Psychology 52, no. 6 (2021): 343–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000462.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Several studies demonstrated that a high need for closure (NFC) is associated with higher prejudice toward the out-group. This study aims to investigate how this effect can be moderated by attributions of morality to the in-group and the out-group. A questionnaire was administered to 725 participants. The results showed a positive relationship between NFC and prejudice when the in-group was evaluated as more moral than the out-group. This relationship was weaker when the out-group was evaluated as more moral than the in-group. These findings implicated that it is possible to reduce prejudice in individuals with high NFC by manipulating perceptions of in-group and out-group morality.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

MS, Simi. "What You Need to Know About Vacuum - Assisted Wound Closure (VAC)." International Journal of Science and Research (IJSR) 12, no. 2 (2023): 773–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.21275/sr23207150833.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Begal, Manshi, Vigraanth Bapu K G, and Priyanka V R. "Effect of Forgiveness on Need for Closure." International Journal of Health Sciences and Research 14, no. 5 (2024): 135–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.52403/ijhsr.20240515.

Full text
Abstract:
Aim of the present study is to find the relationship between Forgiveness and Need for Closure and to find the influence of one on the other. Convenience sampling was used to collect data from a sample of 200 young adults from different parts of India, using Heartland Forgiveness Scale (Yamhure Thompson et al., 2002) and Need for Closure Scale (Paliwal & Kumar, 2022) with good reliability and validity. SPSS 29 analysis shows no significant gender differences in forgiveness and the need for closure. Observations suggested a weak negative correlation (r = 0.161, p < 0.05) between Forgiveness and Need for Closure. Regression analysis further indicated that Forgiveness has a moderate influence on the Need for Closure (R² = 0.025), emphasizing its role as a coping mechanism that can mitigate the desire for cognitive closure. These findings open the door to future research that can deepen our understanding of the intricate relationship between forgiveness and the need for closure. Key words: Forgiveness, Need for Closure, Gender Differences, Correlation, Regression.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Giacomantonio, Mauro, Antonio Pierro, Conrad Baldner, and Arie Kruglanski. "Need for Closure, Torture, and Punishment Motivations." Social Psychology 48, no. 6 (2017): 335–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000321.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. When considering how criminals should be punished, most individuals prefer retributive (i.e., punishment compensating for the harm caused by the perpetrator) over utilitarian justice (i.e., punishment with the intent to deter future crime). However, past research has found that individuals with a high (vs. low) need for cognitive closure (NCC) are more likely to endorse utilitarian punishment. In three studies, we replicated past research on the association between need for closure and utilitarian justice (Study 1), and found that this relationship is mediated by moral concerns pertaining to group unity and cohesion (Study 2). In Study 3 we examine another instance of utilitarian policy: torture. Our data provide evidence that preference for utilitarian punishment is rooted in basic moral concerns.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Panno, Angelo, Giuseppe Carrus, Ambra Brizi, Fridanna Maricchiolo, Mauro Giacomantonio, and Lucia Mannetti. "Need for Cognitive Closure and Political Ideology." Social Psychology 49, no. 2 (2018): 103–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000333.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Little is known about epistemic motivations affecting political ideology when people make environmental decisions. In two studies, we examined the key role that political ideology played in the relationship between need for cognitive closure (NCC) and self-reported eco-friendly behavior. Study 1: 279 participants completed the NCC, pro-environmental, and political ideology measures. Mediation analyses showed that NCC was related to less pro-environmental behavior through more right-wing political ideology. Study 2: We replicated these results with a nonstudent sample (n = 240) and both social and economic conservatism as mediators. The results of Study 2 showed that social conservatism mediated the relationship between NCC and pro-environmental behavior. Finally, NCC was associated with pro-environmental attitude through both social and economic conservatism.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Kosic, Ankica. "Need for cognitive closure and coping strategies." International Journal of Psychology 37, no. 1 (2002): 35–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590143000153.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Voice, Paul. "Democracy and the Need for Normative Closure." International Journal of Applied Philosophy 29, no. 1 (2015): 153–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/ijap201562646.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Webster, Donna M., and Arie W. Kruglanski. "Individual differences in need for cognitive closure." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67, no. 6 (1994): 1049–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Chernikova, Marina, Arie Kruglanski, Dino Giovannini, Loris Vezzali, and Junjie Su. "Need for closure and reactions to innovation." Journal of Applied Social Psychology 47, no. 9 (2017): 473–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12451.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

DeBacker, Teresa K., and H. Michael Crowson. "Measuring need for closure in classroom learners." Contemporary Educational Psychology 33, no. 4 (2008): 711–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.06.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Stalder, Daniel R. "Investigation of the Two-Factor Model for the English Version of the Need for Closure Scale." Psychological Reports 110, no. 2 (2012): 598–606. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/03.09.20.pr0.110.2.598-606.

Full text
Abstract:
Using the English version of the Need for Closure Scale, this research investigated previous, though largely ignored, recommendations to exclude the close-mindedness subscale and to treat the Need for Closure Scale as a two-factor (vs single-factor) scale. Reliability and correlational analyses supported the recommendation to exclude close-mindedness. The two-factor model without close-mindedness yielded better fit indexes than a single-factor model or a two-factor model with close-mindedness. Despite the strong cross-cultural use of the scale, no investigation had yet tested the two-factor model without close-mindedness using any language other than Dutch. The present results and discussion can improve the study of need for closure by encouraging more researchers to consider the supported recommendations. Roets and Van Hiel's proposed modification to make the Need for Closure Scale a single-factor scale was also discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Sari, Mustika. "Pengaruh Model Pengukuran Formatif Terhadap Need For Closure Konsumen." Economic Management Business Research Journal 1, no. 1 (2025): 20–25. https://doi.org/10.57235/ember.v1i1.6075.

Full text
Abstract:
Need for closure is a new construct in consumer behavior and marketing management research. This construct is considerably robust to measure consumer’s epistemic motivation. Epistemic motivation is a desire based action to obtain relevant knowledge to make judgment or buying decision. Previous study measures need for closure construct with reflective measurement model specification. Conceptual analysis shows that need for closure construct is a formative model due to the equality of its dimensions. This study shows strong evidence that formative measurement model specification is more robust than reflective model to test the model of influence of need for closure of online consumer and flow level in the internet environment
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Emeka, Chukwubuike Kevin. "Closure of Abdominal Incisions Following Laparotomy for Peritonitis in Children: Primary Closure or Delayed Primary Closure?" International Journal of Surgery & Surgical Techniques 6, no. 2 (2022): 1–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.23880/ijsst-16000173.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: There is need to determine the optimal management strategy for dirty abdominal surgical wounds. The aim of this study was to evaluate the post-operative wound complications in children whose dirty surgical wounds were closed primarily, in relation to those closed by delayed primary closure. Materials and Methods: This was a prospective evaluation of children, aged 15 years and younger, who had laparotomy for peritonitis in a teaching hospital in Enugu, Nigeria. This study covered a 5-year period. The following data were evaluated: patients’ age, gender, presenting symptoms, laboratory results at presentation, duration of symptoms before presentation, time interval between presentation and intervention, intra-operative finding, definitive operative procedure performed, complications of treatment, and outcome of treatment of the 2 groups of patients. Results: A total of 204 cases of peritonitis in children were operated upon during the study period. The incisions were closed by primary closure (Group A) in 104 (51%) while 100 (49%) surgical wounds were closed by delayed primary closure (Group B). More males were involved. All the patients had abdominal pain and about half the patients were anemic and had electrolyte imbalance at presentation. Typhoid intestinal perforation was the most common cause of peritonitis and closure of ileal perforation was the most frequent performed surgical procedure. Comparing the 2 groups of patients, surgical site infection and stitch related were less common in group B patients whose surgical incisions were closed by delayed primary closure. Conclusion: Delayed primary closure of dirty laparotomy incisions has less complication in terms of surgical site infection and stitch related complications. Therefore, delayed primary closure of dirty laparotomy wounds is advocated in pediatric population.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Vermeir, Iris, and Maggie Geuens. "Need for Closure and Youths' Leisure Time Preferences." Psychological Reports 98, no. 2 (2006): 463–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.98.2.463-476.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Strojny, Paweł, Małgorzata Kossowska, and Agnieszka Strojny. "When the need for closure promotes complex cognition." Roczniki Psychologiczne 19, no. 1 (2016): 27–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.18290/rpsych.2016.19.1-1en.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Thomas, Tessa. "Successful home birth groups need support not closure." British Journal of Midwifery 9, no. 3 (2001): 135–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.12968/bjom.2001.9.3.7996.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

May, Arne. "The need for continued care after sponsor closure." Lancet Neurology 19, no. 3 (2020): 205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1474-4422(20)30023-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Mattila, Anna S., and Sunmee Choi. "Societal Norms, Need for Closure, and Service Recovery." Journal of International Consumer Marketing 24, no. 5 (2012): 356–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08961530.2012.741479.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Neuman, B. C. "The need for closure in large distributed systems." ACM SIGOPS Operating Systems Review 23, no. 4 (1989): 28–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/70730.70735.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Fox, Shaul, and Yael ElRaz-Shapira. "Perceived group variability and dispositional need for closure." Current Psychology 24, no. 4 (2005): 218–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-005-1024-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Choi, Jong An, Minkyung Koo, Incheol Choi, and Seigyoung Auh. "Need for cognitive closure and information search strategy." Psychology and Marketing 25, no. 11 (2008): 1027–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mar.20253.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Safipour Afshar, Mojtaba, Omid Pourheidari, Bakr Al-Gamrh, and Asghar Afshar Jahanshahi. "Audit management, need for closure and detection of misstatements." Journal of Accounting in Emerging Economies 9, no. 2 (2019): 237–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/jaee-08-2018-0092.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to study whether diverting auditors to erroneous accounts leads to higher effectiveness and detection of errors. Also, this paper investigates the effect of the need for cognitive closure of auditors on audit effectiveness and detection of errors in the presence of audit management. Design/methodology/approach The authors used a financial statement containing a diverting statement and several errors for measuring audit management and used a survey to measure auditors’ need for closure. Research sample consisted of 79 independent auditors having above three years of audit experience. The set of financial statement and questionnaire (measuring the need for closure of auditors) was given to auditors and they had enough time to fill them. Findings Results show that diverting auditors to accounts containing error does not lead to higher effectiveness and detection of errors. Also, auditors need for closure character does not affect their effectiveness and detection of errors in the financial statements. Practical implications Diverting auditors to erroneous accounts leads to higher detection of earning management. With this regard, the results increase the awareness of auditors that diverting auditors away from important errors to easy-to-detect erroneous accounts leads to their belief of achieving the audit objectives by detecting phony errors and misstatements. In other words, the results alert auditors of managers’ techniques of audit management. Originality/value This study contributes to the literature on audit management and need for cognitive closure of auditors in Iran’s audit environment and introduces these concepts to this environment. The paper will be of value to Association of Iranian Certified Public accountants to include stricter measure in appraisal of audit firms’ quality and educate its participants about audit management and mediating effect of the need for closure of auditors on the detection of errors and misstatements in financial statements.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Didenko, A. V., O. K. Alenina, and A. V. Oglezneva. "Need for Cognitive Closure in Patients with Social Phobia." Клиническая и специальная психология 11, no. 4 (2022): 70–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.17759/cpse.2022110403.

Full text
Abstract:
<p style="text-align: justify;">A comparative study of the relationship between the need for cognitive closure, social anxiety, and cognitive strategies for regulating emotions in social phobia was carried out. We examined 135 people (57.8% female; Mage=32.8±11.3 years) with a diagnosis of "Social phobia" (F40.1, ICD-10), combined with other mental disorders (mean disorder duration — 13.1±8.1 years). The comparison group was represented by 100 people without mental disorders (53% female; Mage=35.2±6.5 years). Tools used: Need for Closure Scale by A. Kruglanski and D. Webster adapted by M.I. Yasin; Cognitive Emotional Regulation Questionnaire by N. Garnefski et al., adapted by E.I. Rasskazova et al.; Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale in the adaptation of I.V. Grigorieva and S.N. Enikolopov. It has been established that the clinical manifestations of social phobia are characterized by a high need for cognitive closure and the used dysfunctional cognitive strategies for regulating the emotional sphere are reflected in the inability to achieve cognitive closure, the impossibility of reducing the level of social anxiety, which increases the motivation for social avoidance.</p>
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Chirumbolo, Antonio, Stefano Livi, Lucia Mannetti, Antonio Pierro, and Arie W. Kruglanski. "Effects of need for closure on creativity in small group interactions." European Journal of Personality 18, no. 4 (2004): 265–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.518.

Full text
Abstract:
Three experiments investigated the consequences of the epistemic motivation toward closure on the emergence of creative interactions in small groups. In the first study, need for closure was manipulated via time pressure. Results showed that in groups under high need for closure (i.e. under time pressure) the percentage of creative acts during group discussion was reduced. The second study replicated this result using an individual differences operationalization of the need for closure. In the third study, groups composed of individuals high (versus low) in need for closure performed less creatively, and exhibited less ideational fluidity during group interaction. Moreover, it was demonstrated that conformity pressure mediates the negative relationship between dispositional need for closure and group creativity. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Leone, Christopher, Harry M. Wallace, and Kevin Modglin. "The Need for Closure and the Need for Structure: Interrelationships, Correlates, and Outcomes." Journal of Psychology 133, no. 5 (1999): 553–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223989909599762.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Zimin, LIU, SHI Kan, WAN Jin, and CHEN Chen. "Analysis and outlook of need for cognitive closure research." Advances in Psychological Science 26, no. 4 (2018): 688. http://dx.doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1042.2018.00688.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Gribbins, Theta, and Brian Vandenberg. "Religious Fundamentalism, the Need for Cognitive Closure, and Helping." International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 21, no. 2 (2011): 106–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2011.556999.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Taris, Toon W. "Dispositional Need for Cognitive Closure and Self-Enhancing Beliefs." Journal of Social Psychology 140, no. 1 (2000): 35–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224540009600444.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Acar-Burkay, Sinem, Bob M. Fennis, and Luk Warlop. "Trusting others: The polarization effect of need for closure." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107, no. 4 (2014): 719–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0037022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Pierro, Antonio, Lucia Mannetti, Eraldo De Grada, Stefano Livi, and Arie W. Kruglanski. "Autocracy Bias in Informal Groups Under Need for Closure." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29, no. 3 (2003): 405–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203251191.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Orehek, Edward, Shira Fishman, Mark Dechesne, et al. "Need for Closure and the Social Response to Terrorism." Basic and Applied Social Psychology 32, no. 4 (2010): 279–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2010.519196.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Wyrick, Deidre L., Andrew P. Bozeman, Samuel D. Smith, et al. "Persistent gastrocutaneous fistula: Factors affecting the need for closure." Journal of Pediatric Surgery 48, no. 12 (2013): 2506–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2013.06.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Chirumbolo, Antonio, and Luigi Leone. "Individual differences in need for closure and voting behaviour." Personality and Individual Differences 44, no. 5 (2008): 1279–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Palmer, Ian. "We need to protect hospitals from closure by politicians." Nursing Standard 28, no. 27 (2014): 34–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.7748/ns2014.03.28.27.34.s45.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

De Zavala, Agnieszka Golec, Aleksandra Cislak, and Elzbieta Wesolowska. "Political Conservatism, Need for Cognitive Closure, and Intergroup Hostility." Political Psychology 31, no. 4 (2010): 521–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00767.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Maduro, Daniela Côrtes. "CHARACTERS DON’T NEED CLOSURE: A AVENTURA IMPRESSA E DIGITAL." Revista de Estudos Literários 4 (January 20, 2016): 377–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.14195/2183-847x_4_17.

Full text
Abstract:
“Quem teve a sorte de nascer personagem pode rir até da morte. As personagens não morrem”, escreveu Luigi Pirandello em Seis personagens à procura de um autor (1921). Mesmo que estas sejam executadas pelo autor podem permanecer camufladas entre as letras até serem despertadas. A sua morte é apenas ensaiada e nunca é sinónimo de despedida. Porém, não é apenas o acto de leitura que estende a mão às personagens e que as resgata do esquecimento. Muitas vezes elas são reanimadas por outros média ou por outras formas de expressão. Elas comprovam que um livro não tem um carácter ensimesmado, nem está circunscrito ao seu peso e medida. Ao extrapolarem a fronteira de papel e ao instalarem-se noutros formatos, elas permitem igualmente concluir que o livro mantém um diálogo contínuo com outras formas de representação.
 As personagens não temem a ambiguidade. Tanto embarcam numa sequência linear de eventos, como caminham sobre terreno irregular e enfrentam trilhos bifurcados. A noção de texto como um todo tem vindo a ser contrariada pela literatura impressa e pela literatura electrónica através das suas personagens. A construção de uma narrativa rizomática, a transgressão da barreira entre planos ficcionais, bem como a alteração do contrato entre autor e leitor antecederam a emergência do romance pós-modernista. Na literatura eletrónica é possível assistir a uma extensão desse processo com a adopção do computador como instrumento expressivo. Graças à sua capacidade de armazenamento de informação e velocidade de processamento, bem como à divisão entre interface e memória, é possível criar personagens dinâmicas que trocam autonomamente de posições ou identidade. Isto significa que o texto pode revelar um comportamento emergente e imprevisível. Este artigo pretende abordar o conceito de desfecho através da invocação de algumas personagens que embarcaram numa aventura impressa ou digital.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Erimurti, Kuntari. "SPESIFIKASI MODEL PENGUKURAN FORMATIF UNTUK MENGUJI PENGARUH NEED FOR CLOSURE KONSUMEN ONLINE PADA LEVEL." Jurnal Ekonomi dan Bisnis 10, no. 1 (2009): 200. http://dx.doi.org/10.30659/ekobis.10.1.200-208.

Full text
Abstract:
Need for closure is a new construct in consumer behavior and marketing management research.This construct is considerably robust to measure consumer’s epistemic motivation.Epistemic motivation is a desire based action to obtain relevant knowledge to make judgmentor buying decision. Previous study measures need for closure construct with reflective measurementmodel specification. Conceptual analysis shows that need for closure construct isa formative model due to the equality of its dimensions. This study shows strong evidencethat formative measurement model specification is more robust than reflective model to testthe model of influence of need for closure of online consumer and flow level in the internetenvironmentKeywords : Need For Closure, Flow, Formative, Reflective, Online Consumer
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Durant, Matthew, Xheni Mucelli, and Linda S. Huang. "Meiotic Cytokinesis in Saccharomyces cerevisiae: Spores That Just Need Closure." Journal of Fungi 10, no. 2 (2024): 132. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jof10020132.

Full text
Abstract:
In the budding yeast Saccharomyces cerevisiae, sporulation occurs during starvation of a diploid cell and results in the formation of four haploid spores forming within the mother cell ascus. Meiosis divides the genetic material that is encapsulated by the prospore membrane that grows to surround the haploid nuclei; this membrane will eventually become the plasma membrane of the haploid spore. Cellularization of the spores occurs when the prospore membrane closes to capture the haploid nucleus along with some cytoplasmic material from the mother cell, and thus, closure of the prospore membrane is the meiotic cytokinetic event. This cytokinetic event involves the removal of the leading-edge protein complex, a complex of proteins that localizes to the leading edge of the growing prospore membrane. The development and closure of the prospore membrane must be coordinated with other meiotic exit events such as spindle disassembly. Timing of the closure of the prospore membrane depends on the meiotic exit pathway, which utilizes Cdc15, a Hippo-like kinase, and Sps1, an STE20 family GCKIII kinase, acting in parallel to the E3 ligase Ama1-APC/C. This review describes the sporulation process and focuses on the development of the prospore membrane and the regulation of prospore membrane closure.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Roets, Arne, and Alain Van Hiel. "Separating Ability From Need: Clarifying the Dimensional Structure of the Need for Closure Scale." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33, no. 2 (2007): 266–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167206294744.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Chirumbolo, Antonio, and Alessandra Areni. "Job insecurity influence on job performance and mental health: Testing the moderating effect of the need for closure." Economic and Industrial Democracy 31, no. 2 (2010): 195–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143831x09358368.

Full text
Abstract:
The moderating effect of the need for closure in the relationship between job insecurity, job performance and mental health was investigated. The need for closure refers to a motivated need for certainty, intolerance of ambiguity and preference for predictability. It was argued that the need for closure may function as a psychological moderator in dealing with job insecurity. Participants comprised 287 workers, who were administered a self-reported questionnaire. Results confirmed the negative relationship between job insecurity, performance and mental health. The need for closure was positively related to job performance and unrelated to mental health. More interestingly, the need for closure exhibited multifaceted patterns of interactions with the different components of job insecurity. Higher need for closure revealed a buffering effect in conditions of higher quantitative job insecurity. In this case, individuals high (vs low) in the need for closure reported better job performance and mental health. Conversely, when qualitative job insecurity was higher, individuals high (vs low) in the need for closure reported an impaired job performance and mental health.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Wang, Biying, Abel Beng Heng Ang, and Alex Mitchell. ""I need to play three times before I kind of understand"." Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction 5, CHI PLAY (2021): 1–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3474684.

Full text
Abstract:
Replay of interactive stories is an essential characteristic of the medium, enabling players to both see whether their choices impact the narrative, and work towards an understanding of the story and underlying system. Despite the centrality of replay to the form, little work has been done to investigate what motivates people to replay interactive stories, or when and why they stop replaying. In this paper, we report on the results of an exploratory qualitative observational study of 12 participants (aged 22 to 28) replaying the visual novel The Shadows that Run Alongside Our Car, a short work that presents a post-apocalyptic survival story from the perspective of two different playable characters and involves four dialogue choices leading to one of three possible endings. Unlike previous models of rereading, our observations suggest that players' reasons for deciding whether or not to replay change fluidly as their sense of closure changes, and players are deciding based not just on whether they have reached closure, but also on their estimate of the likelihood of seeing something new or moving closer to closure versus the effort required to replay. This suggests a need to rethink earlier, more simplistic models of rereading in interactive stories.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Onwumere, Juliana, Elizabeth Kuipers, Paul Bebbington, et al. "The need for closure in caregivers of people with psychosis." Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale 19, no. 2 (2010): 159–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1121189x00000865.

Full text
Abstract:
SummaryAim – The aim of the study was to determine how carer need for closure relates to expressed emotion. It also examined the links between carer need for closure and patient functioning including patient need for closure. Methods – In a cross-sectional study, 70 caregivers of patients with psychosis completed the Need for Closure Scale (NFCS), the Camberwell Family Interview (CFI) and measures of distress, burden, coping and social network. The NFCS was assessed in terms of its two primary dimensions: a need for simple structure (NFSS) and Decisiveness. Patients also completed measures of psychotic symptoms and affect, and in 50 matched caregiver patient dyads, direct comparisons were undertaken between caregiver and patient NFCS scores. Results – No links were found between caregiver NFC and EE in this predominately low EE sample. More decisive carers had higher levels of self esteem, were less distressed, and resorted less to avoidant coping. The need for simple structure was greater in carers who lacked a confidante. As predicted, patients reported significantly higher NFSS and lower Decisiveness scores than carers, but no relationship was observed between caregiver NFC and patient symptoms of psychosis. Conclusions – Carers reporting confident decision making were also more likely to report adaptive functioning in terms of having lower levels of avoidant coping and distress, and higher levels of self esteem. The results suggest that this style of thinking might be a helpful way of coping with some of the difficulties involved in caring for someone with psychosis.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Kossowska, Małgorzata, Katarzyna Jaśko, and Yoram Bar-Tal. "Need for closure and cognitive structuring among younger and older adults." Polish Psychological Bulletin 43, no. 1 (2012): 40–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/v10059-012-0005-6.

Full text
Abstract:
Need for closure and cognitive structuring among younger and older adults The paper reported two correlational studies. The aim of the Study 1 was to examine the hypothesis that age moderates the relationship between need for closure (NFC) and cognitive structuring. Results of the study revealed that aging with increased need for closure was associated with better recognition of irrelevant information than schema-relevant items, in testing hypotheses about the target person. These findings are interpreted as demonstrating the age-associated failure of cognitive abilities (i.e., low efficacy at fulfilling the need for closure), reducing tendency to behave according to the level of epistemic motivation. The results of Study 2 demonstrated that older participants are characterized by higher NFC but by lower EFNC than young participants. These results are consistent with the conclusion that the negative relationships between NFC and cognitive structuring demonstrated by the older participants in Study 1 can be attributed to their lower level of EFNC.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Lillie, Sarah E., Steven S. Fu, Angela E. Fabbrini, et al. "Does need for cognitive closure explain individual differences in lung cancer screening? A brief report." Journal of Health Psychology 25, no. 8 (2018): 1109–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1359105317750253.

Full text
Abstract:
The need for cognitive closure describes the extent to which a person, faced with a decision, prefers any answer in lieu of continued uncertainty. This construct may be relevant in lung cancer screening, which can both reduce and increase uncertainty. We examined whether individual differences in need for cognitive closure are associated with Veterans’ completion of lung cancer screening using a self-administered survey ( N = 361). We also assessed whether need for cognitive closure moderates an association between screening completion and lung cancer risk perception. Contrary to our main hypothesis, high need for cognitive closure Veterans were not more likely to complete lung cancer screening and need for cognitive closure did not have a moderating role.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Paakkari, Leena, Didier Jourdan, Jo Inchley, and Minna Torppa. "The Impact of School Closure on Adolescents’ Wellbeing, and Steps towards to a New Normal: The Need for an Assessment Tool Update?" Adolescents 1, no. 3 (2021): 360–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/adolescents1030027.

Full text
Abstract:
Close to 200 countries have implemented school closures to decrease the spread of the COVID-19 coronavirus. Though the closures have seemed necessary, their effects on the wellbeing of children and adolescents have raised serious concerns. To truly understand the impact of such disruption on young people’s wellbeing, and their views on how to move towards a new normal, we must adopt different approaches to gather the data to secure children’s and adolescents’ rights to be heard in the issues that concern their lives. Current ways to examine the impacts of school closure have been dominated by gathering information concerning the children and adolescents, using mainly existing wellbeing indicators and related questionnaire surveys. Although such sources of information are important, they provide limited understanding of how children and adolescents have experienced school closures, especially if they have been produced using measures developed purely by adults. There is a need for information produced by children and adolescents themselves, which may require going beyond existing and pre-COVID theoretical wellbeing frameworks. By capturing information produced by children and adolescents, we can more effectively guide the development and evaluation of public health policies and identify solutions to mitigate the negative impacts of school closure, or to acknowledge the possible positive effects, and respond accordingly.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Pietrzak, Janina, Łukasz Jochemczyk, Piotr Serbin, and Magdalena Kuśka. "The influence of need for closure on expectations about and outcomes of negotiations." Polish Psychological Bulletin 45, no. 3 (2014): 286–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.2478/ppb-2014-0035.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Need for closure is a construct that describes a motivational tendency to quickly select and prioritize information in the environment. Such tendencies can affect the process of negotiations, and so the quality of their outcome. The rigidity that accompanies high need for closure can lead to less openness to proposals that benefit one’s partner, and to solutions that are less optimal. We conducted a study in which 34 pairs of individuals negotiated. Pairs were matched in terms of need for closure (high vs. low) and gender. We found that need for closure affected subjective evaluations of certain aspects of the negotiation process. Participants with low need for closure were more likely to indicate that they and their partners sought win-win solutions during the negotiation. This led to a greater sense of process fairness for the negotiation. These results can be taken into consideration when teaching negotiations, and when planning real-life negotiations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Oh, Se-Hyung, and Ki-Hyon Kim. "The Effects of Need for Cognitive Closure on Negotiation Behaviors." Korean Academic Association of Business Administration 34, no. 2 (2021): 325–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.18032/kaaba.2021.34.2.325.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Stalder, Daniel R. "Need for Closure, the Big Five, and Public Self-Consciousness." Journal of Social Psychology 147, no. 1 (2007): 91–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/socp.147.1.91-94.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Czupryna, Marcin, Elżbieta Kubińska, and Łukasz Markiewicz. "On the Need for Cognitive Closure and Judgmental Trend Forecasting." Argumenta Oeconomica Cracoviensia, no. 17 (2017): 21–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.15678/aoc.2017.1702.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!