To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Neuroconstructivism.

Journal articles on the topic 'Neuroconstructivism'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 47 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Neuroconstructivism.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Campos, Ruth, and María Sotillo. "Constructing minds: The development of mindreading abilities in typical and atypical trajectories." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 336–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08004111.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractFrom our deep interest in the neuroconstructivist framework, we would like to comment on two fundamental aspects of Mareschal et al.'s work: the role of neuroconstructivism for clinical work with people suffering from developmental disorders; and the relation between the process of progressive specialization and the increasing abstraction of representations in development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Westermann, Gert, Denis Mareschal, Mark H. Johnson, Sylvain Sirois, Michael W. Spratling, and Michael S. C. Thomas. "Neuroconstructivism." Developmental Science 10, no. 1 (2007): 75–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Thomas, Michael S. C., Gert Westermann, Denis Mareschal, Mark H. Johnson, Sylvain Sirois, and Michael Spratling. "Studying development in the 21st Century." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 345–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08004202.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractIn this response, we consider four main issues arising from the commentaries to the target article. These include further details of the theory of interactive specialization, the relationship between neuroconstructivism and selectionism, the implications of neuroconstructivism for the notion of representation, and the role of genetics in theories of development. We conclude by stressing the importance of multidisciplinary approaches in the future study of cognitive development and by identifying the directions in which neuroconstructivism can expand in the Twenty-first Century.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Mareschal, Denis. "From NEOconstructivism to NEUROconstructivism." Child Development Perspectives 5, no. 3 (2011): 169–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00185.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Gerrans, Philip. "Nativism, neuroconstructivism, and developmental disorder." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, no. 6 (2002): 757–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02280139.

Full text
Abstract:
Either genetically specified modular cognitive architecture for syntactic processing does not exist (neuroconstructivism), or there is a module but its development is so abnormal in Williams syndrome (WS) that no conclusion can be drawn about its normal architecture (moderate nativism). Radical nativism, which holds that WS is a case of intact syntax, is untenable. Specific Language Impairment and WS create a dilemma that radical nativism cannot accommodate.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Ramus, Franck. "Should neuroconstructivism guide developmental research?" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, no. 3 (2004): 100–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Sirois, Sylvain, Michael Spratling, Michael S. C. Thomas, Gert Westermann, Denis Mareschal, and Mark H. Johnson. "Précis of Neuroconstructivism: How the Brain Constructs Cognition." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 321–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0800407x.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractNeuroconstructivism: How the Brain Constructs Cognition proposes a unifying framework for the study of cognitive development that brings together (1) constructivism (which views development as the progressive elaboration of increasingly complex structures), (2) cognitive neuroscience (which aims to understand the neural mechanisms underlying behavior), and (3) computational modeling (which proposes formal and explicit specifications of information processing). The guiding principle of our approach is context dependence, within and (in contrast to Marr [1982]) between levels of organiza
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. "Preaching to the Converted? From Constructivism to Neuroconstructivism." Child Development Perspectives 3, no. 2 (2009): 99–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00086.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. "Nativism versus neuroconstructivism: Rethinking the study of developmental disorders." Developmental Psychology 45, no. 1 (2009): 56–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014506.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Gerrans, Philip. "Nativism and Neuroconstructivism in the Explanation of Williams Syndrome." Biology & Philosophy 18, no. 1 (2003): 41–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1023396800448.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Lickliter, Robert. "Representing development: models, meaning, and the challenge of complexity." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 342–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08004172.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractNeuroconstructivism (Mareschal et al. 2007a) provides a useful framework for how to integrate research from different levels of analysis to model the multidimensional dynamics of development. However, the authors overlook the topic of meaning, a fundamental feature of cognition and subjective experience and also downplay the nonlinear nature of developmental causality. Neuroconstructivism is overly optimistic on the point of how well current computational models can address the challenge of complexity in developmental science.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Foster, Jonathan, Anke van Eekelen, and Eugen Mattes. "Neuroconstructivism: Evidence for later maturation of prefrontally mediated executive functioning." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 338–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08004135.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe authors of this commentary concur with the viewpoint presented by Mareschal et al. (2007a; 2007b) concerning the relevance of neurological data when theorizing about cognitive development. However, we argue here that Mareschal et al. fail to consider adequately the relevance of reorganizational brain events occurring through adolescence and early adulthood, especially regarding the prefrontal cortex and the ontogeny of executive functioning. In addition, evidence from the lifespan neurodevelopmental literature indicates that increased activity of neural networks may signify less ef
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Bateson, Patrick. "A good approach to neural and behavioural development but would be even better if set in a broader context." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31, no. 3 (2008): 334–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08004093.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractAn attractive feature of Neuroconstructivism, Vol. I: How the Brain Constructs Cognition is its emphasis on the active role of the individual in neural and behavioural development and the importance of the interplay with the environment. Certain aspects of development are omitted, however, such as specializations for the distinctive ecologies of infancy and childhood and the scaffolding-like features of behaviour seen during development. It was also a pity that so little credit was given to many scientists who have contributed to just those aspects of development on which the authors f
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Campos, Ruth, Carmen Nieto, and María Núñez. "Research domain criteria from neuroconstructivism: A developmental view on mental disorders." Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 10, no. 3 (2018): e1491. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1491.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Farina, Mirko. "Three Approaches to Human Cognitive Development: Neo-nativism, Neuroconstructivism, and Dynamic Enskillment." British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 67, no. 2 (2016): 617–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/bjps/axu026.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Richter, Matthias. "Die Konfusion der Identitätstheorie mit dem Neurokonstruktivismus und deterministischen Naturalismus – demonstriert am Ansatz von Michael Pauen." Deutsche Zeitschrift für Philosophie 67, no. 2 (2019): 211–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/dzph-2019-0017.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Representatives of ‘philosophy of mind’ such as Michael Pauen think about the issue of mind and brain as if they were solving a linguistic puzzle. At first glance this approach appears to be particularly objective, but actually it contains misleading simplifications, logical failures and categorical mistakes at crucial points in the train of thought. This article demonstrates that the claim of identity of mind and brain only makes sense if one presupposes neuroconstructivism and strong naturalism already in the very composition of the identity-oriented argument. According to naturalis
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Gerrans, Philip. "La lecture de pensée pour débutants." Articles 33, no. 1 (2006): 125–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.7202/012950ar.

Full text
Abstract:
Résumé Certains psychologues évolutionnistes (PE) adoptent un nativisme fort à propos de la théorie de l’esprit (TDE). Ils soutiennent que le développement de la compétence cognitive spécialisée nécessaire à la TDE requiert une spécification génétique de la trajectoire développementale d’un système cognitif spécialisé. La PE arrive à cette conclusion en se basant sur des arguments conceptuels qui concernent l’inadéquation du neuroconstructivisme tabula rasa (neuroconstructivisme fort) et sur les données empiriques fournies par la psychologie du développement et la neuropsychologie. Je soutiens
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Allen, James R. "Constructivist and Neuroconstructivist Transactional Analysis." Transactional Analysis Journal 39, no. 3 (2009): 181–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/036215370903900302.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Rinaldi, Luca, and Annette Karmiloff-Smith. "Intelligence as a Developing Function: A Neuroconstructivist Approach." Journal of Intelligence 5, no. 2 (2017): 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence5020018.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Thomas, Michael S. C., Frank D. Baughman, and Henri Lehalle. "Neuroconstructivisme : comprendre les trajectoires développementales typiques et atypiques." Enfance N° 3, no. 3 (2014): 205–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/enf1.143.0205.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Thomas, Michael S. C., and Frank D. Baughman. "Neuroconstructivisme : comprendre les trajectoires développementales typiques et atypiques." Enfance 2014, no. 03 (2014): 205–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.4074/s0013754514003036.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Ansari, Daniel, and Annette Karmiloff-Smith. "Atypical trajectories of number development: a neuroconstructivist perspective." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, no. 12 (2002): 511–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)02040-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Klein-Tasman, Bonita P. "Neurodevelopmental Disorders Across the Lifespan: A Neuroconstructivist Approach." Child Neuropsychology 19, no. 5 (2013): 557–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.729029.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. "From constructivism to neuroconstructivism: Did we still fall into the foundationalism/encodingism trap? Commentary on “Stepping off the pendulum: Why only an action-based approach can transcend the nativist–empiricist debate” by J. Allen and M. Bickhard." Cognitive Development 28, no. 2 (2013): 154–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2013.01.007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Westermann, Gert, and Nicolas Ruh. "A neuroconstructivist model of past tense development and processing." Psychological Review 119, no. 3 (2012): 649–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028258.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Quadrelli, Ermanno, and Chiara Turati. "Origins and development of mirroring mechanisms: A neuroconstructivist framework." British Journal of Developmental Psychology 34, no. 1 (2015): 6–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12110.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

KARMILOFF-SMITH, A. "The tortuous route from genes to behavior: A neuroconstructivist approach." Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 6, no. 1 (2006): 9–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/cabn.6.1.9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Oliver, Andrew, Mark H. Johnson, Annette Karmiloff-Smith, and Bruce Pennington. "Deviations in the emergence of representations: a neuroconstructivist framework for analysing developmental disorders." Developmental Science 3, no. 1 (2000): 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00094.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Marcus, David J., and Charles A. Nelson. "Neural Bases and Development of Face Recognition in Autism." CNS Spectrums 6, no. 1 (2001): 36–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1092852900022872.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis paper critically examines the literature on face recognition in autism, including a discussion of the neural correlates of this ability. The authors begin by selectively reviewing the behavioral and cognitive neuroscience research on whether faces are represented by a “special” behavioral and neural system—one distinct from object processing. The authors then offer a neuroconstructivist model that attempts to account for the robust finding that certain regions in the inferior temporal cortex are recruited in the service of face recognition. This is followed by a review of the evid
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Cornish, Kim. "E. K. Farran and A. Karmiloff-Smith: Neurodevelopmental Disorders Across the Lifespan: A Neuroconstructivist Approach." Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 43, no. 3 (2013): 752. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1761-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Viana-Sáenz, Lourdes, Sylvia Sastre-Riba, Maria Luz Urraca-Martínez, and Juan Botella. "Measurement of Executive Functioning and High Intellectual Ability in Childhood: A Comparative Meta-Analysis." Sustainability 12, no. 11 (2020): 4796. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/su12114796.

Full text
Abstract:
From a neuroconstructivist approach and a developmental model of high intellectual ability (HIA), it is argued that the management of intellectual resources through executive functioning (EF) is one of the factors influencing the expression of high potential. The main objective is to determine the effectiveness of measures of executive functioning used comparing schoolchildren with HIA and those of average intelligence. A meta-analysis was carried out on a selection of 17 studies for a total sample of 1518 children with either HIA or an average level of intelligence. Pooled estimates of effect
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Martínez Castilla, Pastora. "La teoría del muestreo temporal aplicada a los trastornos del lenguaje: análisis desde una perspectiva neuroconstructivista." Revista de Neurología 73, no. 11 (2021): 394. http://dx.doi.org/10.33588/rn.7311.2021204.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Lorusso, Maria Luisa, Simona Travellini, Marisa Giorgetti, Paola Negrini, Gianluigi Reni, and Emilia Biffi. "Semi-Immersive Virtual Reality as a Tool to Improve Cognitive and Social Abilities in Preschool Children." Applied Sciences 10, no. 8 (2020): 2948. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/app10082948.

Full text
Abstract:
Virtual reality (VR) creates computer-generated virtual environments where users can experience and interact in a similar way as they would do in real life. VR systems are increasingly being used for rehabilitation goals, mainly with adults, but also with children, extending their application to the educational field. This report concerns a study of the impact of a semi-immersive VR system in a group of 25 children in a kindergarten context. The children were involved in several different games and activity types, specifically developed with the aim of learning specific skills and foster team
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Filippi, Roberto, Dean D’Souza, and Peter Bright. "A developmental approach to bilingual research: The effects of multi-language experience from early infancy to old age." International Journal of Bilingualism 23, no. 5 (2018): 1195–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367006917749061.

Full text
Abstract:
Aims and objectives: In this commentary article we consider the benefits of adopting a neuroconstructivist approach in the study of bilingualism in order to promote empirical and theoretical progress on the fiercely debated issue of whether bilingualism confers genuine cognitive advantages. Significance/implications: Although there is a general consensus that exposure to multilingual environments does not impair cognitive development, there are still doubts on the possible beneficial advantages of bilingualism. Critics argue that the evidence for this advantage might have been confounded by un
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Del Popolo Cristaldi, Fiorella, Lisa Toffoli, Gian Marco Duma, and Giovanni Mento. "Little fast, little slow, should I stay or should I go? Adapting cognitive control to local-global temporal prediction across typical development." PLOS ONE 18, no. 2 (2023): e0281417. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0281417.

Full text
Abstract:
Adaptive cognitive control (CC), the ability to adjust goal-directed behavior according to changing environmental demand, can be instantiated bottom-up by implicit knowledge, including temporal predictability of task-relevant events. In S1-S2 tasks, either local (trial-by-trial hazard expectation) or global (block-by-block expectation) temporal information can induce prediction, allowing for proactive action control. Recent developmental evidence showed that adaptive CC based on global temporal prediction emerges earlier than when it is based on the local one only. However, very little is know
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Carroll, Julia. "Neurodevelopmental Disorders Across the Lifespan: A Neuroconstructivist Approach E.K.Farran & A.Karmiloff-Smith (Eds.) Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2012. pp. 408, £49.95 (hb). ISBN: 978-0-19-959481-8." Child and Adolescent Mental Health 18, no. 4 (2013): 255–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/camh.12047_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Lin, Shu-Kun. "Neurodevelopmental Disorders across the Lifespan: A Neuroconstructivist Approach. Edited by Emily K. Farran and Annette Karmiloff-Smith, Oxford University Press, 2012; 394 pages. Price: £49.99, ISBN 978-0-19-959481-8." Brain Sciences 3, no. 4 (2013): 84–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3010084.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Tokuhama-Espinosa, Tracey, and Cynthia Borja. "Radical neuroconstructivism: a framework to combine the how and what of teaching and learning?" Frontiers in Education 8 (August 1, 2023). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2023.1215510.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent advances in pedagogical research have called attention to the dynamic nature of the teaching and learning process in which the actors mutually influence one another. The understanding of how this works in the brain—the specialized neural networks related to this process—is often limited to neuroscientists but are slowly becoming available to other learning scientists, including teachers. A transdisciplinary approach combining the best information about observable teaching-learning processes from education with newer information from the neurosciences may aid in resolving fundamental que
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Astle, Duncan E., Mark H. Johnson, and Danyal Akarca. "Toward computational neuroconstructivism: a framework for developmental systems neuroscience." Trends in Cognitive Sciences, May 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2023.04.009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Urraca-Martínez, MᵃLuz, and Sylvia Sastre-Riba. "Cognitive Flexibility in Schoolchild Through the Graphic Representation of Movement." Frontiers in Psychology 11 (January 13, 2021). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.624922.

Full text
Abstract:
Neuroconstructivism postulates the progressive complexity of mental representation over the course of cognitive development and the role of the graphic representation of movement in the transformation of mental schemas, cognitive flexibility, and representational complexity. This study aims to: (1) understand children’s resources in the drawing of movement (5–8 years); and (2) verify whether there are differences in the graphic representation of movement as an indicator of cognitive flexibility. The participants were N = 240 children aged 5–8 years; 1,440 drawings were collected representing 2
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Kissné, Réka, Réka Zsámboki, and László Varga. "Természet- és életközeli nevelés a reformpedagógiai törekvések és a neurokonstruktivista tanuláselmélet párhuzamainak tükrében • Nature- and Experience-Based Education in the Light of Theoretical Parallels to Reform Pedagogy and Neuroconstructivism." Magyar Tudomány, March 1, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1556/2065.184.2023.3.2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Campos, Ruth, and Carmen Nieto. "When dyadic interaction is the context: Mimicry behaviors on the origin of imitation." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 40 (2017). http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x16001862.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractKeven & Akins (K&A) redefine some of the neonatal imitation (NI) behaviors as developmental stereotypes. From a neuroconstructivist framework, those early gestures are also far from being considered as imitative behaviors. The cognitive substrate of imitation requires an interactive context to develop. Prior to intentional imitation, the dyad shows mimicry behaviors, which are automatic, but do not fade through development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

"Supplemental Material for A Neuroconstructivist Model of Past Tense Development and Processing." Psychological Review, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028258.supp.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Plusquellec, A., and L. Vandromme. "Point de vue neuroconstructiviste : modèle développemental du trouble du spectre de l’autisme." Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence, September 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurenf.2023.09.003.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Romero-Rivas, Carlos, Sara Rodríguez-Cuadrado, Lucía Sabater, et al. "Beyond the conservative hypothesis: a meta-analysis of lexical-semantic processing in Williams syndrome." Language and Cognition, May 17, 2023, 1–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2023.15.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Williams syndrome (WS) is a rare genetic disorder, characterised at the cognitive level by a phenotypic pattern of relative weaknesses (e.g., visuospatial skills) and strengths (e.g., some linguistic and nonverbal reasoning skills). In this study, we performed a systematic search and meta-analysis on lexical-semantic processing in WS, an area of knowledge in which contradictory results have been obtained. We found 42 studies matching our criteria, and, in total, 78 effect sizes were included in the meta-analysis. Results showed that individuals with WS have worse lexical-semantic skil
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Martínez, Verónica, Vanesa Pérez, María Aránzazu Antón, Manuela Miranda, and Patricio Vergara. "Longitudinal profiles of late phonological development in children with Williams syndrome." Frontiers in Communication 9 (April 16, 2024). http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2024.1386899.

Full text
Abstract:
Williams syndrome (WS) is a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by language skills above what is expected considering non-verbal intelligence. Research on phonological development is scarce, with many studies focusing on grammar in children and adolescents. In one of our previous studies transversally explored the profiles of late phonological development in Spanish-speaking WS children, adolescents, and adults, while our objective is to longitudinally determine these profiles for WS children based on present error indexes in spontaneous speech. Participants were seven WS childre
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Waber, Deborah P., Ellen C. Boiselle, Ayse D. Yakut, Carrie P. Peek, Kristine E. Strand, and Jane H. Bernstein. "Developmental Dyspraxia in Children With Learning Disorders: Four-Year Experience in a Referred Sample." Journal of Child Neurology, October 25, 2020, 088307382096691. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0883073820966913.

Full text
Abstract:
Objective: The term apraxia is clearly understood in adult behavioral neurology, with assessment of gesture central to the diagnosis. In contrast, the concept of “developmental dyspraxia” has been more problematic. In an effort to better specify its potential significance, we describe preliminary practice-based evidence for understanding developmental dyspraxia acquired within a theoretical framework informed by both adult behavioral neurology and a neuroconstructivist appreciation of neurodevelopmental disorders. Specifically, we describe the experience of a diagnostic clinic for children wit
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!