Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Neuroethology'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 42 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Neuroethology.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Lisney, Thomas James. "Neuroethology and vision in elasmobranchs /." [St. Lucia, Qld.], 2004. http://www.library.uq.edu.au/pdfserve.php?image=thesisabs/absthe18057.pdf.
Full textPearson, Martin James. "Computational Neuroethology using Programmable Logic devices." Thesis, University of the West of England, Bristol, 2007. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.495459.
Full textHall, Zachary J. "The neuroethology and evolution of nest-building behaviour." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/5542.
Full textDawson, Jeffery Wayne. "The neuroethology of acoustic startle/escape in flying locusts." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 2001. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk3/ftp04/NQ63417.pdf.
Full textAmmagarahalli, Munishamappa Byrappa. "Olfactory neuroethology of the Oriental fruit moth, Grapholita molesta (Busck)." Doctoral thesis, Universitat de Lleida, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/314761.
Full textGrapholita molesta es una de las principales plagas del melocotonero. Los volátiles de planta son una técnica prometedora para atraer G. molesta en condiciones de confusión sexual. En mi tesis he caracterizado la respuesta de las neuronas receptoras olfativas (ORN) a la feromona y volátiles de planta mediante registros de sensila única. Después determiné que el sinergismo entre volátiles de planta y feromona previamente publicado no ocurre a nivel de la ORN. Más adelante he comparado volátiles de planta que en estudios anteriores en China y en Australia habían dado buenos resultados pero en mi caso no hubo respuestas en campo, aunque en el túnel de vuelo sí que aumentaron la respuesta a la feromona. Finalmente he explorado el papel de volátiles de planta y alcoholes en la respuesta a mezclas subóptimas de feromona. Con estos resultados espero haber contribuido al conocimiento de la neuretología olfativa de esta plaga.
Grapholita molesta és una de les principals plagues del presseguer. Els volàtils de planta són una tècnica prometedora per atreure G. molesta en condicions de confusió sexual. En la meva tesi he caracteritzat la resposta de les neurones receptores olfactòries (ORN) a la feromona i volàtils de planta mitjançant registres de sensila única. Després vaig determinar que el sinergismo entre volàtils de planta i feromona prèviament publicat no ocorre a nivell de la ORN. Més endavant he comparat volàtils de planta que en estudis anteriors a Xina i a Austràlia havien dau bons resultats però en el meu cas no va haver-hi respostes en camp, encara que en el túnel de vol sí que van augmentar la resposta a la feromona. Finalment he explorat el paper de volàtils de planta i alcohols en la resposta a mescles subóptimas de feromona. Amb aquests resultats espero haver contribuït al coneixement de la neuretología olfactòria d'aquesta plaga.trolled with sex pheromones (mating disrupton) and insecticide applications. Under mating disruption conditions it is difficult to monitor the pest and to evaluate the control methods. Plant volatiles are a promising technique to attract G. molesta under mating disruption conditions. In addition plant volatiles could attract females, while the sex pheromone only attracts males.
Faulkes, Zen. "Sand crab digging, the neuroethology and evolution of a new behaviour." Thesis, National Library of Canada = Bibliothèque nationale du Canada, 1996. http://www.collectionscanada.ca/obj/s4/f2/dsk3/ftp04/nq21923.pdf.
Full textCliff, David T. "Animate vision in an artificial fly : a study in computational neuroethology." Thesis, University of Sussex, 1991. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.314559.
Full textPérez, Aparicio Alicia. "Sense and Sensitivity: on the biology and neuroethology of two tortricid moths." Doctoral thesis, Universitat de Lleida, 2021. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/673006.
Full textLa confusión sexual mediante feromonas es una táctica de control de plagas que, al impedir el apareamiento entre polillas, ha ayudado enormemente a reducir los daños en cultivos, así como el uso de pesticidas. La necesidad de disminuir los costes asociados a su aplicación ha fomentado el desarrollo de dispensadores automáticos o puffers. Estos puffers liberan una gran cantidad de feromona mientras los insectos están sexualmente activos. Sin embargo, la falta de información sobre la biología y el comportamiento de los insectos y del efecto que algunos factores externos tienen sobre su periodicidad diaria impiden un correcto ajuste de los puffers, lo que puede suponer una disminución de su eficacia en campo. A pesar de que el trampeo automático puede servir para determinar estos periodos de actividad y decidir cuándo liberar feromona, las trampas disponibles comercialmente no confieren una resolución temporal suficiente para bosquejar la actividad de los insectos, que puede estar reducida a unas pocas horas. Para mi tesis hemos diseñado y puesto a prueba una trampa asequible y fácil de construir que me ha permitido determinar el vuelo sexual diario y estacional de Grapholita molesta (Busk) a partir de fotografías tomadas con una gran resolución temporal. Tanto en trampas cebadas con feromona sintética como en trampas cebadas con hembras vírgenes, la mayoría de las capturas tuvieron lugar entre 3 horas antes y una hora después de la puesta de sol, indicando una actividad diurna crepuscular en la especie. El tipo de cebo (septo de feromona o hembra virgen) no afectó el periodo de vuelo diario. Aun así, las bajas temperaturas avanzaron los vuelos, mientras que las temperaturas más altas retrasaban el vuelo hasta horas cercanas a la puesta de sol. Estos resultados destacan la necesidad de programar los puffers para que liberen la feromona a distintas horas a lo largo del año, siguiendo la curva de vuelo de los insectos. Hacerlo aumentaría la eficacia de este sistema en campo, al tiempo que reduciría el coste de su aplicación y evitaría un desperdicio de feromona. Para explicar los mecanismos que subyacen al funcionamiento de la confusión sexual en el control de plagas, la mayoría de los estudios se han centrado en el efecto de grandes niveles de feromona sintética sobre los machos. Las hembras, por otro lado, se han considerado insensibles a su propia feromona. Sin embargo, una revisión reciente indica que hay una alteración notable en el comportamiento de las hembras al ser expuestas a su propia feromona, un fenómeno conocido como “autodetección”. A pesar de ello, no hay pruebas que confirmen que las hembras sean capaces de detectar su propia feromona a nivel de las neuronas olfativas. Mediante técnicas de electrofisiología, he comparado la respuesta de las neuronas olfativas alojadas en sensilla trichodea de machos y hembras a compuestos biológicamente relevantes para G. molesta, una especie en la que se ha descrito autodetección. Un análisis de agrupamiento jerárquico indica un sistema olfativo periférico radicalmente diferente en cada sexo, lo que podría estar relacionado con las diferentes necesidades biológicas de cada uno. Mientras que en los machos no había células que respondieran a su propia feromona de cortejo, cinamato de etilo, la mayoría (63%) respondieron específicamente a los compuestos de feromona sexual mayoritario y minoritario (Z8-12:Ac y E8-12:Ac, respectivamente). La frecuencia de cada uno de estos tipos de células es similar al ratio de mezcla de feromonas de las hembras. En hembras, un 6% de las células respondían a volátiles de planta y un 3% son específicas para la feromona de cortejo de los machos. A pesar de que algunas células eran estimuladas por la feromona sexual de las hembras, estas respuestas eran siempre menores que las de machos y, además, respondían a más compuestos. De hecho, la agrupación jerárquica las incluye en un grupo (91%) de neuronas inespecíficas. La ausencia en hembras de receptores para la feromona sexual y su baja sensibilidad a la misma les impediría detectar variaciones en las ratios de feromona en la naturaleza. Por lo tanto, cualquier alteración del comportamiento tras una exposición a feromona conspecífica en el laboratorio no podría ser atribuida a un reconocimiento de la propia especie. El comportamiento de las hembras sí que puede verse alterado por otras señales más relevantes para sus necesidades biológicas. El olor característico de una planta puede provocar un efecto diferente en hembras que provienen de huéspedes distintos. En mi tesis he analizado respuestas electroantenográficas de machos y hembras adultos de Lobesia botrana (Denis et Schiffermüller) (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) recogidas en fase larvaria de la vid (Vitis vinífera L.) o del torvisco (Daphne gnidium L.) a volátiles específicos y compartidos de los huéspedes, así como a compuestos de feromona sexual. Mis resultados indican que la detección de feromona no difiere entre ambas poblaciones. Además, la detección de volátiles de planta no se ve afectada por el sexo o la planta de desarrollo larvario. Poblaciones polífagas desarrollándose en un huésped concreto parecen retener la capacidad para responder a los volátiles de otros huéspedes. Esta falta de diferencias estadísticas en la diferenciación de compuestos olfativos al nivel de la antena no implica, sin embargo, que los individuos que provienen de cada huésped muestren preferencias similares ante ambos conjuntos de volátiles, dado que las respuestas biológicas dependen en última instancia de la integración cerebral de cada individuo. Aun así, es importante comprender la capacidad de los insectos polífagos para reproducirse o buscar alimentos y refugios alternativos a la hora de determinar los límites topográficos de la confusión sexual. Mi tesis sirve para entender mejor las repercusiones que algunos factores ambientales pueden tener sobre el funcionamiento de una técnica compleja como es la confusión sexual. A pesar de que el uso de pesticidas se ha reducido, aún nos valemos de estos químicos para evitar el descontrol de las plagas, y es necesario conocer las variables que afectan a la eficacia de las alternativas sostenibles.
Pheromone-mediated mating disruption (MD), a pest management tactic that prevents moth mating, has helped reduce insecticide use and crop damage in agriculture. The need to decrease the economic and labor costs associated with its application has encouraged the development of automated dispensers. These puffers disrupt mating behavior of moth pests by releasing pheromone during the time when insects are active. However, the lack of a firm basis on the biology and behavior of moths, and how external factors can alter their periodicity, prevent the proper adjustment of the puffers, and may result in a decrease of their efficacy in the field. Although automated traps can be used to determine these activity periods and adjust puffer pheromone release, commercially automated traps do not offer enough temporal resolution to outline insect activity periods, which usually last one or a few hours. In my thesis, we have designed and tested a cheap and easy to build high temporal resolution image-sensor insect trap. Based on captures in traps lured with synthetic pheromone and virgin females I have determined the daily and seasonal Grapholita molesta male sexual responsiveness. Although the lure did not influence the daily and seasonal sexual responsiveness period of Grapholita molesta (Busk), the flight activity within 24 h circadian rhythm is often influenced by sunset time and temperature. Most captures were registered between 3 hours before and 1 hour after sunset, indicating a diurnal to crepuscular activity of the species. Lure type (septum or female) did not influence the daily time of flight. While low temperatures led to an earlier flight in males, warmer temperatures resulted in flights closer to the sunset time. To reduce the cost of MD and avoid wasting pheromone, puffers should be programmed to spray at a variable time throughout the season, following the curve of activity of the insects. In order to explain the mechanisms of pheromone-mediated MD in pest management, most studies have focused on the effect high levels of synthetic pheromones have on the behavior of males, whereas females were considered unresponsive to their own pheromone. However, a recent review shows that there is substantial behavioral evidence that female moths respond to their own sex pheromone. Nevertheless, the evidence for sex pheromone "autodetection" at the olfactory receptor neuron (ORN) level is limited. By means of electrophysiological methods I compared the responses of ORNs housed in antennal sensilla trichodea to an array of biologically relevant compounds of male and female G. molesta, a species with reported pheromone autodetection. Hierarchical cluster analysis (HCA) indicated a radically different peripheric olfactory system between sexes that could be related to their specific ecological roles. In males no cells responded to their own courtship pheromone ethyl trans-cinnamate, while most (63%) were tuned specifically to the major or minor pheromone compounds (Z8-12:Ac and E8-12:Ac, respectively), their relative abundance being similar to their ratio in the female pheromone. Plant volatile cells were relatively frequent in females (6%) and 3% of the female ORNs were also tuned to the male-produced courtship pheromone. Several female cells were excited by female-produced sex pheromone, but their responses were generally lower than in males, and they responded broadly to the other compounds as well, so the HCA grouped them in a large cluster (91%) of "unspecific" female neurons. The lack of differential sex pheromone receptor neurons in females, and their relatively low sensitivity to sex pheromone would not allow females to detect variations in the pheromone ratios in nature. Thus, the alteration of their behavior after exposure to conspecific pheromone under laboratory conditions does not appear to be species-specific. Female behavior can nonetheless be specifically altered by other cues more relevant to their biological needs. The headspace of different plants may induce a different effect on females stemming from different hosts. I analyzed electroantennogram (EAG) responses of male and female adults of the European grapevine moth Lobesia botrana (Denis et Schiffermüller) (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae) collected as larvae from grapevine (Vitis vinifera L.) and flax-leaved daphne (Daphne gnidium L.) to specific and shared plant volatiles of the two hosts, as well as to sex pheromone compounds. My results indicate that pheromone detection did not differ between the two populations. Furthermore, host-plant volatile detection was not affected by sex or larval host plant. Polyphagous populations developing on a specific host seem therefore able to detect the plant volatiles of alternative hosts. However, lack of statistical differences in odor discrimination at the antennal level does not imply that insects from each host would show similar preference for the two host-specific odor blends, since behavioral responses to plant odors require brain integration of the antennal input of each individual odorant in a blend. Differences in plant preference could still occur if there are no differences at the EAG level. Understanding the potential ability of a polyphagous moth to reproduce or find food and shelter in alternative hosts is important to assess the topographical limitations of MD. My thesis serves to understand a bit better the complex functioning of a technique like MD and the repercussions that some environmental factors can have in its proper functioning. Even though pesticide use has been reduced, we still rely on these chemicals to prevent pest outburst. A more deep and rigorous study of the variables affecting the efficacy environmentally friendly alternatives should be conducted.
Banta, Pamela Ann 1966. "Neuroethology of acquired English and conspecific vocalizations in the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus)." Diss., The University of Arizona, 1998. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/282800.
Full textChilds, Edward William. "The roles of the locust DCMD in collision detection." Thesis, University of Newcastle Upon Tyne, 1999. http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.300191.
Full textMartin, Joshua Pierce. "Encoding the Configuration of a Conspecific Pheromone in the Antennal Lobe of a Moth, Manduca sexta." Diss., The University of Arizona, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/10150/216970.
Full textSkorina, Laura. "Representation of the stationary visual environment in the anterior thalamus of the leopard frog." Diss., Temple University Libraries, 2013. http://cdm16002.contentdm.oclc.org/cdm/ref/collection/p245801coll10/id/231474.
Full textPh.D.
The optic tectum of the leopard frog has long been known to process visual information about prey and looming threats, stimuli characterized by their movement in the visual field. However, atectal frogs can still respond to the stationary visual environment, which therefore constitutes a separate visual subsystem in the frog. The present work seeks to characterize the stationary visual environment module in the leopard frog, beginning with the hypothesis that this module is located in the anterior thalamus, among two retinorecipient neuropil regions known as neuropil of Bellonci (NB) and corpus geniculatum (CG). First, the puzzle of how a stationary frog can see the stationary environment, in the absence of the eye movements necessary for persistence of vision, is resolved, as we show that whole-head movements caused by the frog's respiratory cycles keep the retinal image in motion. Next, the stationary visual environment system is evaluated along behavioral, anatomic, and physiological lines, and connections to other brain areas are elucidated. When the anterior thalamic visual center is disconnected, frogs show behavioral impairments in visually navigating the stationary world. Under electrophysiological probing, neurons in the NB/CG region show response properties consistent with their proposed role in processing information about the stationary visual environment: they respond to light/dark and color information, as well as reverse-engineered "stationary" stimuli (reproducing the movement on the retina of the visual backdrop caused by the frog's breathing movements), and they do not habituate. We show that there is no visuotopic map in the anterior thalamus but rather a nasal-ward constriction in the receptive fields of progressively more caudal cell groups in the NB/CG region. Furthermore, each side of the anterior thalamic visual region receives information from only the contralateral half of the visual field, as defined by the visual midline, resulting from a pattern of partial crossing over of optic nerve fibers that is also seen in the mammalian thalamic visual system, a commonality with unknown evolutionary implications. We show that the anterior thalamic visual region shares reciprocal connections with the same area on the opposite side of the brain, as well as with the posterior thalamus on both sides; there is also an anterograde ipsilateral projection from the NB/CG toward the medulla and presumably pre-motor areas.
Temple University--Theses
Keeley, Brian Lee. "Cognitive science as the computational neuroethology of intelligent behavior : why biological facts are important for explaining intelligent behavior /." Diss., Connect to a 24 p. preview or request complete full text in PDF format. Access restricted to UC campuses, 1997. http://wwwlib.umi.com/cr/ucsd/fullcit?p9804512.
Full textBertsch, David J. "The Effects of Satiety-state Neuromodulation on Predatory Hunting Behaviors and CNS Sensorimotor Processing in the Praying Mantis, Tenodera sinensis." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1631984571296264.
Full textBak-Coleman, Joseph Brightwell. "Comparing the Role of the Lateral Line During Rheotaxis Between a Sedentary and Mobile Species." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1392913490.
Full textKulpa, Matthew Ryan. "The Lateral Line is Necessary for Blind Cavefish Rheotaxis in Non-Uniform Flow." Bowling Green State University / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=bgsu1415191679.
Full textLehmkuhl, Andrew M. II. "Characterizing the mechanoreception of water waves in the leech Hirudo verbana." University of Cincinnati / OhioLINK, 2016. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=ucin1468509670.
Full textHupé, Ginette Jessica. "Electrocommunication in a Species of Weakly Electric Fish Apteronotus Leptorhynchus: Signal Patterning and Behaviour." Thesis, Université d'Ottawa / University of Ottawa, 2012. http://hdl.handle.net/10393/20675.
Full textGuo, Peiyuan. "THE ROLE OF THE CENTRAL COMPLEX IN ADAPTIVE LOCOMOTOR BEHAVIOR IN COCKROACHES." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2014. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1381444649.
Full textCopley, Sean. "An Investigation into the Role of Motion Vision in Manduca sexta Flight." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2019. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1543852712345633.
Full textRodrigues, Marcelo Cairrão Araujo. "Análise neuroetológica e estudo da atividade pró-convulsivante e anticonvulsivante in vivo da peçonha bruta da aranha Parawixia bistriata em ratos: injeção central e periférica." Universidade de São Paulo, 1999. http://www.teses.usp.br/teses/disponiveis/59/59134/tde-13022003-091530/.
Full textSpider venoms have hight affinity and specificity for neuronal receptors, transporters and ion channels, therefore been important tools to characterize mammal and insect nervous system. However, behavioural alterations in mammals caused by injections of spider venoms have not been studied in detail. In this work we describe the rat behavioural alteration caused by central injection of the crude venom of the spider Parawixia bistriata, using a neuroethological methodology. There were seen two types of seizures, named mild and severe. Neuroethological flowcharts showed that in mild seizures, there was a strong statistical correlation (c2) between tremor followed by laying or by laying left, which indicates that the venom, perhaps, is difficulting central coordination of movements. In severe seizures, this effect is enworsed, with the animal falling.This type of seizure are similar to those described by Racine (1972). Since the crude venom of P. bistriata showed a potencial anticonvulsant activity in vitro, we tested if it would indeed inhibit clonic and tonic convulsions induced by pentilenetetrazole (PTZ; 80 mg/kg, i.p.). Boiled crude venom of P. bistriata was i.c.v. injected, and 20 minutes later, animals (n=10) received PTZ. A control group (n=10) received only PTZ. The results were: central injection of the venom abolished clonic and tonic convulsions induced by PTZ, in 60% of the animals. In conclusion, the crude spider venom of P. bistriata, centrally injected, causes central loss of movement coordination, and elicits limbic seizures similar to those described by Racine (1972), but, when boiled and injected in lower doses, it blocks clonic and tonic convulsions induced by PTZ (80 mg/kg).
Tietz, Brian R. "MODELS OF COCKROACH SHELTER SEEKING IMPLEMENTED ON A ROBOTIC TEST PLATFORM." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2012. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1323455715.
Full textRauscher, Michael James. "Induced haltere movements reveal multisensory integration schema in Drosophila." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2021. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1618609909377269.
Full textBertsch, David J. "The effects of satiety-state neuromodulation on predatory hunting behaviors and CNS sensorimotor processing in the praying mantis, Tenodera sinensis." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2008. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1623260572699621.
Full textYarger, Alexandra Mead. "Inertial encoding mechanisms and flight dynamics of dipteran insects." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1585688085360805.
Full textJozet-Alves, Christelle. "Neuroethologie de la cognition spatiale chez la seiche commune (Sepia officinalis)." Paris 13, 2008. http://www.theses.fr/2008PA132014.
Full textThis PhD thesis is related to neuroethology of spatial learning in Sepia officinalis. Firstly, we have shown that cuttlefish are able to solve a spatial task in a T-maze by using either a motor sequence or visual cues. The males, which display a higher level of locomotor activity than females, preferentially rely on visual cues to orient in a T-maze. On the contrary, females preferentially use a motor sequence to solve the same spatial task. Concerning the neural determinisms of spatial learning, lesional studies have shown that acquisition and retention of a spatial task are vertical lobe-dependent. A morphoetric study also indicated that the postembryonic maturation of the vertical lobe and the optic lobes are influenced by rearing conditions. These highly associative structures of the central nervous system of cuttlefish may play a key-role in spatial learning
Michel, Olivier. "Experiences en neuroethologie artificielle evots : une methodologie evolutionniste appliquee en robotique mobile." Nice, 1996. http://www.theses.fr/1996NICE5013.
Full textVarga, Adrienn Gabriella. "The Neural Basis of Head Direction and Spatial Context in the Insect Central Complex." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2017. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1487249074487484.
Full textGill, Jeffrey Paul. "Neural Correlates of Adaptive Responses to Changing Load in Feeding Aplysia." Case Western Reserve University School of Graduate Studies / OhioLINK, 2020. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=case1579795905638273.
Full textZube, Christina. "Neuronal representation and processing of chemosensory communication signals in the ant brain." kostenfrei, 2008. http://www.opus-bayern.de/uni-wuerzburg/volltexte/2008/3038/.
Full text"Neuroethology of Social Attention in Primates." Diss., 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10161/883.
Full textShepherd, Stephen Vincent. "Neuroethology of Social Attention in Primates." Diss., 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/10161/883.
Full textTo solicit the attention or determine the intentions of another, we use our eyes. While many animals appear to use eyes as an important behavioral cue, for humans, these cues are especially critical. The power of the eyes to attract and direct attention shapes human behavior from an early age and likely serves as a foundation for social skill acquisition, ranging from simple, friendly eye contact to complex, spoken language, even to our almost mystical ability to empathize and "see the world through another's eyes". Humans have transformed our environment through our economic alliances and military competitions, and our individual successes and failures depend critically on social skills built on a foundation of shared attention. When these abilities break down, as in autism, pervasive social awkwardness can challenge the close relationship of individuals with their friends, family, and community. Nonetheless, we know almost nothing about the brain mechanisms that have evolved to process social cues and convert them into a rich experience of shared attention. To investigate this process, we explored the ability of human and nonhuman primates to follow the attention of other individuals. First, we characterized natural gaze-following behavior using a novel telemetric device in socially-interacting prosimian primates, and later in monkeys and humans responding to gaze cues in the lab. Finally, we examined the neuronal responses to gaze cues in a macaque posterior parietal area implicated in attention control--the lateral intraparietal area, LIP. Our findings suggest that gaze-following abilities may be widespread in social primates, relying on conserved, homologous brain pathways; and that they may not be informationally-encapsulated reflexes, but rather are densely interwoven with diverse social processes. Indeed, we found gaze cues influenced neurons in LIP, part of the dorsal frontoparietal attention network. Finally, we report that "mirror" neurons in parietal areas may thus play a role not only in representing perceived bodily actions, but also perceived mental states such as observed attention.
Dissertation
Hsieh, Fang Kuei, and 謝方桂. "Computational neuroethology - Developing a virtual worm system and its energy homeostasis mechanism." Thesis, 2016. http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/handle/4ntgff.
Full text國立清華大學
系統神經科學研究所
104
Neuroethology is the study of animal behavior with an emphasis on the underlying neural mechanisms and interactions between subjects and environment. A great approach to address questions in neuroethology that cannot be addressed by experiment and observation has arisen due to the booming development of computational neuroscience. From this, our lab has developed a software tool, known as the Hanitu system. The system is a simulation environment that simulates the behavior and neural activity of user-designed virtual worms in a two-dimensional virtual world. Hanitu was developed around a central idea, which given a challenging environment, how should the nervous system be designed for a virtual worm to forage for foods, avoid toxicants, and reach the ultimate goal – to survive. This thesis is focused on rebuilding the system based on modular design strategy for easy upgrading and maintenance. There are three core components of the Hanitu system as follows: the graphical user interface (GUI), Hanitu (the virtual world), and Flysim (the neural network simulator). There are two main functions added to the latest version of the Hanitu system: a cross-sensory mechanism and an energy homeostasis mechanism to simulate the more realistic behavior of animals. The former implements a phenomenon for a single olfactory receptor neuron that is active from multiple odorants .The latter that simulates the secretion of an internal energy related hormone, ghrelin, in animals and adds the neuropeptide Y (NPY) neuron which is stimulated by Ghrelin into the neural circuit of virtual worm. The results of the simulation illustrate that different behavior arise from the energy homeostasis mechanism.
Koucoulas, Dean. "Investigating Perception Under Dynamic Auditory Conditions in the Acoustic Parasitoid Fly Ormia ochracea." Thesis, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/1807/42994.
Full text"Modulation of Sensing and Sharing Food-Related Information in the Honey Bee." Doctoral diss., 2017. http://hdl.handle.net/2286/R.I.46345.
Full textDissertation/Thesis
Doctoral Dissertation Biology 2017
Adams, Geoffrey Keith. "Foraging for Information in the Prefrontal Cortex." Diss., 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10161/9428.
Full textThe ability to monitor, learn from, and respond to social information is essential for many highly social animals, including humans. Deficits to this capacity are associated with numerous psychopathologies, including autism spectrum disorders, social anxiety disorder, and schizophrenia. To understand the neural mechanisms supporting social information seeking behavior requires understanding this behavior in its natural context, and presenting animals with species-appropriate stimuli that will elicit the behavior in the laboratory. In this dissertation, I describe a novel behavioral paradigm I developed for investigating social information seeking behavior in rhesus macaques in a laboratory setting, with the use of naturalistic videos of freely-behaving conspecifics as stimuli. I recorded neural activity in the orbitofrontal and lateral prefrontal cortex of monkeys as they engaged in this task, and found evidence for a rich but sparse representation of natural behaviors in both areas, particularly in the orbitofrontal cortex. This sparse encoding of conspecifics' behaviors represents the raw material for social information foraging decisions.
Dissertation
Asi, Navdeep Singh. "Coding of Bat-like Auditory Features in the AN2 Interneuron of the Pacific Field Cricket, Teleogryllus oceanicus and its Relation to Decreasing the Conspicuousness of Synthetic Bat Echolocation Calls." Thesis, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/1807/25411.
Full textSmolka, Jochen. "Sampling Visual Space: Topography, colour vision and visually guided predator avoidance in fiddler crabs (Uca vomeris)." Phd thesis, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/1885/7107.
Full textResearch School of Biological Sciences (RSBS, now RSB), and the Australian National University for providing funding through an ANU PhD scholarship; the Australian Department of Education, Employment and Workplace Relations for an International Postgraduate Research Scholarship; the German National Academic Foundation and the Zeiss Foundation for support through a Heinz-Dürr Scholarship; and the Australian Institute of Marine Sciences for providing accommodation and facilities during fieldwork in Queensland.
Groh, Claudia. "Environmental influences on the development of the female honeybee brain Apis mellifera." Doctoral thesis, 2005. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-17388.
Full textOlfaction plays an important role in a variety of behaviors throughout the life of the European honeybee. Caste specific, environmentally induced and aging/experiencedependent differences in olfactory behavior represent a promising model to investigate mechanisms and consequences of phenotypic neuronal plasticity within the olfactory pathway of bees. This study focuses on the two different female phenotypes within the honeybee society, queens and workers. In this study, for the first time, structural plasticity in the honeybee brain was investigated at the synaptic level. Queens develop from fertilized eggs that are genetically not different from those that develop into workers. Adult queens are larger than workers, live much longer, and display different behaviors. Developmental trajectory is mainly determined by nutritional factors during the larval period. Within the subsequent post-capping period, brood incubation is precisely controlled, and pupae are incubated close to 35°C via thermoregulatory activity of adult workers. Behavioral studies suggest that lower rearing temperatures cause deficits in olfactory learning in adult bees. To unravel possible neuronal correlates for thermoregulatory and caste dependent influences on olfactory behavior, I examined structural plasticity of developing as well as mature olfactory synaptic neuropils. Brood cells were reared in incubators and pupal as well as adult brains were dissected for immunofluorescent staining. To label synaptic neuropils, I used an antibody to synapsin and fluophore-conjugated phalloidin which binds to filamentous (F-) actin. During development, neuronal F-actin is expressed in growing neurons, and in the mature nervous system, F-actin is most abundant in presynaptic terminals and dendritic spines. In the adult brains, this double labeling technique enables the quantification of distinct synaptic complexes microglomeruli [MG]) within olfactory and visual input regions of the mushroom bodies (MBs) prominent higher sensory integration centers. Analyses during larval-adult metamorphosis revealed that the ontogenetic plasticity in the female castes is reflected in the development of the brain. Distinct differences among the timing of the formation of primary and secondary olfactory neuropils were also revealed. These differences at different levels of the olfactory pathway in queens and workers correlate with differences in tasks performed by both female castes. In addition to caste specific differences, thermoregulation of sealed brood cells has important consequences on the synaptic organization within the MB calyces of adult workers and queens. Even small differences in rearing temperatures affected the number of MG in the olfactory calyx lip regions. In queens, the highest number of MG in the olfactory lip developed at 1°C below the temperature where the maximum of MG is found in workers (33.5 vs. 34.5°C). Apart from this developmental neuronal plasticity, this study exhibits a striking age-related plasticity of MG throughout the extended life span of queens. Interestingly, MG numbers in the olfactory lip increased with age, but decreased within the adjacent visual collar of the MB calyx. To conclude, developmental and adult plasticity of the synaptic circuitry in the sensory input regions of the MB calyx may underlie caste- and age-specific adaptations and long-term plasticity in behavior
Brandstaetter, Andreas Simon. "Neuronal correlates of nestmate recognition in the carpenter ant, Camponotus floridanus." Doctoral thesis, 2010. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-55963.
Full textKooperation innerhalb sozialer Gruppen ist vorteilhaft und zeigt sich bei sozialen Insekten in seiner am höchsten entwickelten Form. Besonders eusoziale Hymenopteren, wie Ameisen und Honigbienen, zeigen ein Maß an Kooperation, das nur selten von anderen Tierarten erreicht wird. Um eine effektive Verteidigung der Gruppenmitglieder sicher zu stellen, ist die zuverlässige Erkennung von Feinden unerlässlich. Ameisen verwenden schwerflüchtige, koloniespezifische Profile kutikulärer Kohlenwasserstoffe (Kolonieduft) zur Unterscheidung zwischen Gruppenmitgliedern (Nestgenossen) und fremden Arbeiterinnen (Nestfremdlinge). Man geht davon aus, dass die aus einer Vielzahl von Komponenten bestehenden Koloniedüfte zum Zweck der Kolonieerkennung mit einer neuronalen Schablone, welche sich an bisher unbestimmter Stelle im Nerven-system befindet, abgeglichen werden. Dabei führt eine Diskrepanz zwischen Schablone und Kolonieduft zu Aggression. Eine alternative Hypothese besagt, dass ein sensorischer Filter in der Peripherie des Nervensystems die Aufgabe einer neuronalen Schablone übernimmt. Dies würde mittels sensorischer Adaptation zu spezifischer Anosmie gegenüber Nestgenossen-Kolonieduft führen, so dass die Wahrnehmung von Nestgenossen effektiv verhindert wäre. Allerdings sind Koloniedüfte nicht stabil, sondern verändern sich im Lauf der Zeit aufgrund von Umwelteinflüssen. Um dies zu kompensieren, muss das Erkennungssystem fortwährend aktualisiert werden (Schablonenerneuerung). In dieser Arbeit erbringe ich den Nachweis, dass bei Rossameisen (Camponotus floridanus) die Schablonenerneuerung artifiziell durch Modifizierung der sensorischen Erfahrung induziert werden kann (Kapitel 1). Die Ergebnisse der in Kapitel 1 beschriebenen Experimente zeigen, dass die Schablonenerneuerung ein relativ langsamer Prozess ist, der mehrere Stunden in Anspruch nimmt. Dies widerspricht der Hypothese eines sensorischen Filters, welcher auf sensorischer Adaptation beruht. Dieser Befund konnte mittels erster in-vivo Messungen bestätigt werden, mit Hilfe derer die der Schablonenerneuerung zugrunde liegenden neuronalen Prozesse beschrieben wurden (Kapitel 5). Die neurophysiologischen Messungen wurden zu Beginn dieser Studie durch das Fehlen eines adäquaten Mittels zur Präsentation von Koloniedüften erschwert. In einem Verhaltensversuch konnte ich zeigen, dass taktile Interaktionen für die Kolonieerkennung nicht notwendig sind (Kapitel 2). Ich entwickelte eine neuartige Stimulierungsmethode (Dummy-vermittelte Stimulierung) und testete deren Eignung für neurophysiologische Experimente (Kapitel 3). Meine Experimente zeigten, dass die Dummy-vermittelte Stimulierung besonders für die Präsentation von schwerflüchtigen Düften geeignet ist. Die Konzentration von Koloniedüften im Gasraum konnte durch moderates Aufheizen der Dummys weiter gesteigert werden. Dies erlaubte mir, die neuronalen Korrelate von Koloniedüften im peripheren und im zentralen Nervensystem mittels Elektroantennographie bzw. funktionaler Bildgebung (Calcium Imaging) zu messen (Kapitel 4). Nestgenossen- und Nestfremdlings-Koloniedüfte riefen starke neuronale Antworten in den olfaktorischen Rezeptorneuronen der Antenne und in den funktionalen Einheiten des ersten olfaktorischen Neuropils des Ameisengehirns, den Glomeruli des Antennallobus (AL), hervor. Meine Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Ameisen nicht anosmisch gegenüber Nestgenossen-Koloniedüften sind, womit die vorgeschlagene Hypothese eines sensorischen Filters eindeutig für ungültig erklärt werden kann. Mittels fortschrittlicher Zwei-Photonen-Mikroskopie konnte ich die neuronale Repräsentation von Koloniedüften in verschiedenen neuroanatomischen Kompartimenten des AL messen (Kapitel 5). Obgleich die neuronale Aktivität inhomogen verteilt war, konnte ich keine exklusive Repräsentation finden, die auf ein einzelnes AL-Kompartiment beschränkt gewesen wäre. Dieses Ergebnis weist darauf hin, dass Informationen über Koloniedüfte parallel verarbeitet werden und dies erlaubt die Nutzung der Rechenleistung des kompletten AL-Netzwerkes. Im AL waren die Muster glomerulärer Aktivität (räumliche Aktivitätsmuster) variabel, selbst wenn sie durch wiederholte Stimulierung mit dem gleichen Kolonieduft hervorgerufen wurden (Kapitel 4&5). Dieser Befund ist insofern überraschend, als frühere Studien darauf hinwiesen, dass die räumlichen Aktivitätsmuster im AL widerspiegeln, wie ein Duft von einem Tier wahrge¬nommen wird (Duftqualität). Unter natürlichen Bedingungen stellen Düfte, die aus einer Vielzahl von Komponenten bestehen, variable und fluktuierende Stimuli dar. Höchstwahrscheinlich sind Tiere generell mit dem Problem konfrontiert, dass solche Düfte variable neuronale Antworten hervorrufen. Mittels Zwei-Photonen-Mikroskopie konnte ich zeigen, dass die Variabilität in Antwort auf Nestgenossen-Kolonieduft höher war als in Antwort auf Nestfremdlings-Kolonieduft (Kapitel 5). Möglicherweise spiegelt dies jene Plastizität im AL-Netzwerk wider, welche die Schablonenerneuerung ermöglicht. Aufgrund ihrer hohen Variabilität waren die von verschiedenen Koloniedüften hervorgerufenen räumlichen Aktivierungsmuster nicht hinreichend unterschiedlich, um eine Zuordnung von Duft-qualitäten wie ‚Freund‘ oder ‚Feind‘ zu erlauben. Dieser Befund stellt unsere momentane Auffassung in Frage, wie die Duftqualität komplexer, aus vielen Komponenten bestehender Düfte kodiert wird. Höchstwahrscheinlich sind zusätzliche neuronale Parameter, wie z.B. die präzise, zeitliche Koordinierung neuronaler Aktivität, zur Diskriminierung notwendig. Die geringere Variabilität der von Nestfremdlings-Kolonieduft hervorgerufenen Aktivitätsmuster könnte die Erkennung von Nestfremdlingen auf der nächsten Ebene der olfaktorischen Bahn begünstigen. Meine Forschungsarbeit hat das Kolonieerkennungssystem für direkte neurophysiologische Untersuchungen zugänglich gemacht. Meine Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Ameisen ihre eigenen Nest-genossen wahrnehmen können. Die neuronale Repräsentation von Koloniedüften ist über die AL-Kompartimente verteilt, was auf eine parallele Verarbeitung hinweist. Desweiteren könnte die geringere Variabilität der von Nestfremdlings-Kolonieduft hervorgerufenen Aktivitätsmuster die Erkennung von Nestfremdlingen auf der nächsten Ebene der olfaktorischen Bahn begünstigen. Erstaunlicherweise sind die räumlichen Aktivitätsmuster in Antwort auf Koloniedüfte hochvariabel. Die wirft die Frage auf, wie in diesem System die Duftqualität kodiert wird. Der experimentelle Fortschritt, den ich in dieser Doktorarbeit vorstelle, wird nützlich sein, um weitere Erkenntnisse zu gewinnen, wie soziale Insekten Freunde von Feinden unterscheiden. Desweiteren wird meine Arbeit dem Forschungsbereich Insektenolfaktion zuträglich sein, da die Kolonieerkennung bei sozialen Insekten ein hervorragendes Modelsystem darstellt, um die Kodierung von Duftqualität zu erforschen, sowie Langzeitmechanismen, die der Erkennung komplexer, aus vielen Komponenten bestehender Düfte zugrunde liegen
Stieb, Sara Mae. "Synaptic plasticity in visual and olfactory brain centers of the desert ant Cataglyphis." Doctoral thesis, 2011. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-85584.
Full textDesert ants of the genus Cataglyphis have become model systems for the study of insect navigation. An age-related polyethism subdivides their colonies into interior workers and short-lived light-exposed foragers. While foraging in featureless and cluttered terrain over distances up to several hundred meters, the ants are able to precisely return back to their often inconspicuous nest entrance. They accomplish this enormous navigational performance by using a path integration system - including a polarization compass and an odometer - as their main navigational means in addition to landmark-dependent orientation and olfactory cues. C. fortis, being the focus of the present thesis, is endemic to the salt flats of western North Africa, which are completely avoided by other Cataglyphis species. The fact that Cataglyphis ants undergo a behavioral transition associated with drastically changing sensory demands makes these ants particularly interesting for studying synaptic plasticity in visual and olfactory brain centers. This thesis focuses on plastic changes in the mushroom bodies (MBs) - sensory integration centers supposed to be involved in learning and memory presumably including landmark learning - and in synaptic complexes belonging to the lateral accessory lobe (LAL) known to be a relay station in the polarization processing pathway. To investigate structural synaptic plasticity in the MBs of C. fortis, synaptic complexes (microglomeruli, MG) in the visual (collar) and olfactory (lip) input regions of the MB calyx were immunolabeled and their pre- and postsynaptic profiles were quantified. The results show that a volume increase of the MB calyx during behavioral transition is associated with a decrease of MG number - an effect called pruning - in the collar and, less pronounced, in the lip that goes along with dendritic expansion in MB intrinsic Kenyon cells. Light-exposure of dark-reared ants of different age classes revealed similar effects and dark-reared ants age-matched to foragers had MG numbers comparable to those of interior workers. The results indicate that the enormous structural synaptic plasticity of the MB calyx collar is primarily driven by visual experience rather than by an internal program. Ants aged artificially for up to one year expressed a similar plasticity indicating that the system remains flexible over the entire life-span. To investigate whether light-induced synaptic reorganization is reversible, experienced foragers were transferred back to darkness with the result that their MBs exhibit only some reverse-type characteristics, in particular differences in presynaptic synapsin expression. To investigate the structure of large synaptic complexes in the LAL of C. fortis and to detect potential structural changes, pre- and postsynaptic profiles in interior workers and foragers were immunolabeled and quantified by using confocal imaging and 3D-reconstruction. The results show that these complexes consist of postsynaptic processes located in a central region that is surrounded by a cup-like presynaptic profile. Tracer injections identified input and output tracts of the LAL: projection neurons from the anterior optic tubercle build connections with neurons projecting to the central complex. The behavioral transition is associated with an increase by ~13% of synaptic complexes suggesting that the polarization pathway may undergo some sort of calibration process. The structural features of these synaptic contacts indicate that they may serve a fast and reliable signal transmission in the polarization vision pathway. Behavioral analyses of C. fortis in the field revealed that the ants perform exploration runs including pirouette-like turns very close to the nest entrance for a period of up to two days, before they actually start their foraging activity. During these orientation runs the ants gather visual experience and might associate the nest entrance with specific landmarks or get entrained to other visual information like the polarization pattern, and, concomitantly adapt their neuronal circuitries to the upcoming challenges. Moreover, the pirouettes may serve to stimulate and calibrate the neuronal networks involved in the polarization compass pathway. Video recordings and analyses demonstrate that light experience enhanced the ants’ locomotor activity after three days of exposure. The fact that both the light-induced behavioral and neuronal changes in visual brain centers occur in the same time frame suggests that there may be a link between structural synaptic plasticity and the behavioral transition from interior tasks to outdoor foraging. Desert ants of the genus Cataglyphis possess remarkable visual navigation capabilities, but also employ olfactory cues for detecting nest and food sites. Using confocal imaging and 3D-reconstruction, potential adaptations in primary olfactory brain centers were analyzed by comparing the number, size and spatial arrangement of olfactory glomeruli in the antennal lobe of C. fortis, C. albicans, C. bicolor, C. rubra, and C. noda. Workers of all Cataglyphis species have smaller numbers of glomeruli compared to those of more olfactory-guided Formica species - a genus closely related to Cataglyphis - and to those previously found in other olfactory-guided ant species. C. fortis has the lowest number of glomeruli compared to all other species, but possesses a conspicuously enlarged glomerulus that is located close to the antennal nerve entrance. Males of C. fortis have a significantly smaller number of glomeruli compared to female workers and queens and a prominent male-specific macroglomerulus likely to be involved in sex pheromone communication. The behavioral significance of the enlarged glomerulus in female workers remains elusive. The fact that C. fortis inhabits microhabitats that are avoided by all other Cataglyphis species suggests that extreme ecological conditions may not only have resulted in adaptations of visual capabilities, but also in specializations of the olfactory system. The present thesis demonstrates that Cataglyphis is an excellent candidate for studying the neuronal mechanisms underlying navigational features and for studying neuronal plasticity associated with the ant’s lifelong flexibility of individual behavioral repertoires
Schmitt, Franziska. "Neuronal basis of temporal polyethism and sky-compass based navigation in \(Cataglyphis\) desert ants." Doctoral thesis, 2017. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-142049.
Full textWüstenameisen der Gattung Cataglyphis sind weit verbreitet in ariden Gebieten der paläarktischen Ökozone. Die von ihnen bewohnten Habitate reichen von landmarkenreichen Arealen im Mittelmeerraum, zu beinahe landmarkenfreien Wüstengebieten. Aufgrund ihres hochentwickelten Navigationssystems, welches größtenteils auf dem Himmelskompass basiert, wurden sie in den letzten 4 Jahrzehnten extensiv studiert und sind ein einzigartiges Modellsystem für Navigation. Cataglyphis weisen einen alterskorrelierten Polyethismus auf: Innendienstler dienen als Speichertiere für die ersten ∼2 Wochen ihres adulten Lebens (Interior I). Sie gehen daraufhin zu Brutpflege und Nestbau (Interior II) über bis sie nach ∼4 Wochen zu tagaktiver Furagiertätitkeit außerhalb ihres Nestes wechseln. Dieser letzte Übergang dauert ∼2-3 Tage und wird von den Ameisen genutzt, um Lernund Orientierungsläufe durchzuführen. In der vorliegenden Arbeit befasse ich mich vor allem mit zwei Fragen, die ich mit Hilfe von Cataglyphis als Modellorganismus beantworten möchte: 1. Welches sind die zugrunde liegenden Mechanismen des Alterspolyethismus? 2. Was ist die neuronale Grundlage von Navigation, die auf dem Himmelskompass basiert? Neuropeptide sind bedeutende Regulatoren der Physiologie und des Verhaltens von Insekten und als solche vielversprechende Kandidaten im Hinblick auf die Regulation des Alterspolyethismus in Cataglyphis Ameisen. Neuropeptide werden aus größeren Vorläuferproteinen herausgeschnitten und posttranslational stark modifiziert. Daher ist es wichtig, annotierte Peptide auch biochemisch zu identifizieren. Da für Ameisen kaum Peptiddaten zur Verfügung stehen und es zudem keine relevanten genomischen Daten für Cataglyphis gibt, identifizierte ich zunächst das Neuropeptidom adulter Camponotus floridanus (Formicinae) Arbeiterinnen (Manuskript 1). Daraus resultierte das erste Neuropeptidom, das für eine Ameisenart beschrieben wird—39 Neuropeptide aus 18 Peptidfamilien. In einer weiteren Studie identifizierte ich gezielt die Neuropeptidfamilien Allatostatin A (AstA), Allatotropin (AT), das kurze Neuropeptid F (sNPF) und Tachykinin (TK) mittels Massenspektroskopie und untersuchte die Verteilung von AstA, AT und TK im Gehirn mit Hilfe der Immunhistologie (Manuskript 2). Alle drei Peptide sind im Zentralkomplex lokalisiert, dem Gehirnzentrum welches sensorische Eingänge integriert und in einer übergeordneten Rolle Lokomotorverhalten steuert. AstA und TK sind zudem in den visuellen und olfaktorischen Eingangsregionen, sowie den Pilzkörpern, den Zentren für Lernen und Gedächtnisbildung, zu finden. Ein Vergleich der TK-Immunfärbung im Gehirn von 1, 7 und 14 Tage alten im Dunkeln gehaltenen Tieren zeigt, dass sich die Verteilung im Zentralkomplex verändert— dies ist besonders prominent in der 14 Tage alten Gruppe. In Drosophila moduliert TK im Zentralkomplex Lokomotoraktivität. Basierend darauf stelle ich die Hypothese auf, dass TK in der internen Regulierung des Übergangs von Interior I zu Interior II involviert ist, welchen die Tiere im Alter von ∼2 Wochen durchlaufen. Für eine dritte Studie konstruierte ich ein Verhaltenssetup um den Einfluss von Neuropeptiden und Licht auf die beiden Verhaltensmerkmale ’Lokomotoraktivität’ und ’Phototaxis’ zu testen (Manuskript 3). Der Test zeigte, dass Interior I Ameisen weniger aktiv sind als Interior II Ameisen, welche wiederum weniger aktiv sind als Furageure. Zudem sind Interior Ameisen negativ phototaktisch, verglichen mit einer häufiger zu beobachtenden positiven Phototaxis bei Furageuren. Im Test zeigte sich auch, dass der Einfluss von AstA und AT stadiumsspezifisch ist: während das Verhalten von Interior I Tieren nicht offensichtlich beeinflusst wird, werden Furageure durch die Injektion von AT positiv phototaktisch, sowie aktiver und AstA-Injektion führt zu geminderter Lokomotoraktivität. Darüber hinaus testete ich den Lichteinfluss auf beide Verhaltensmerkmale in den Innendienststadien und zeige, dass er Lokomotoraktivität steigert und in einer geminderten negativen Phototaxis resultiert. Beide Innendienststadien sind jedoch weiterhin negativer phototaktisch als Furageure und nur die Lokomotoraktivtät von Interior II Ameisen wird auf das Niveau von Furageuren angehoben. Diese Ergebnisse stützen die Hypothese, dass Neuropeptide und Licht stadiumsspezifisch Verhalten beeinflussen. Der zweite Aspekt dieser Thesis war es, die neuronale Grundlage der Himmelskompassnavigation in Cataglyphis aufzuklären (Manuskript 4). Die neuroanatomische Lokalisation der Himmelskompasssehbahn zeigt, dass die allgemeine Organisation dieser neuronalen Bahn der bei bisher untersuchten anderen Insekten stark ähnelt. Ich habe mich daraufhin auf Riesensynapsen im lateralen Komplex konzentriert, der letzten Verschaltungsstation ehe die Himmelskompassinformation in den Zentralkomplex übertragen wird. Ein Vergleich zwischen der Synapsenzahl in frisch geschlüpfte Ameisen und erfahrenen Furageueren zeigte einen Anstieg der Synapsenzahl von Innendienst zu Furaguer, was aufgabenabhängige synaptische Plastizität in der Himmelskompasssehbahn suggeriert. In einem weiteren Versuch verglich ich die Riesensynapsenzahlen lichtexponierter Tiere und dunkel gehaltener, gealteter Tiere. Dieses Experiment zeigte, dass der Zuwachs an Riesensynapsen durch den Lichteinfluss ausgelöst wird und keinen altersabhängigen Prozess darstellt. Zudem verändert sich die Anzahl der neu gebildeten Riesensynapsen in Abhängigkeit von den spektralen Eigenschaften des Lichts, dem die Ameisen ausgesetzt sind. Zusammengefasst beschrieb ich in dieser Thesis Neuropeptide in C. floridanus und Cataglyphis und lieferte erste Evidenz, dass diese den Alterspolyethismus in Cataglyphis beeinflussen. Zudem zeigte ich, dass das Ausmaß in dem Neuropeptide und Lichtexposition Verhalten beeinflussen können, stadiumsspezifisch ist. Dies suggeriert, dass das System nur unter bestimmten Bedingungen auf externe Einflüsse reagiert. Diese Ergebnisse lieferten erste wichtige Einblicke in die neuronale Grundlage von Alterspolyethismus in Cataglyphis. Zudem charakterisierte ich erstmals das neuronale Substrat der Himmelskompassnavigation in Cataglyphis. Das hohe Maß an synaptischer Plastizität in dieser Sehbahn beim Übergang von Innenzu Außendienst, könnte besondere Relevanz für die initiale Kalibrierung des Kompasssystems haben