Academic literature on the topic 'Nourrissons – Développement'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Nourrissons – Développement.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Journal articles on the topic "Nourrissons – Développement"
Bigras, Nathalie, Andrée Pomerleau, and Gérard Malcuit. "Expérience cumulée de garde, caractéristiques de l’environnement social et développement de nourrissons qui fréquentent une garderie au cours de leur première année de vie." Revue de l'Université de Moncton 35, no. 2 (March 15, 2005): 7–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.7202/010642ar.
Full textClément, Annie, and Julie Gosselin. "Comparaison des caractéristiques comportementales de l'état d'alerte spontané et de l'état libéré chez le nouveau-né à terme." Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy 67, no. 3 (April 2000): 110–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000841740006700304.
Full textMinot, Océane. "Une qualité de sommeil réduite chez les nourrissons est associée au co-sommeil et au stress parental pendant la pandémie de COVID-19." Cortica 1, no. 2 (September 20, 2022): 425–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.26034/cortica.2022.3347.
Full textHubin-Gayte, Mylène. "Le développement de l'attachement des nourrissons irritables : une revue." Devenir 16, no. 3 (2004): 199. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dev.043.0199.
Full textUnger, Sharon L., Tanis R. Fenton, Radha Jetty, Jeff N. Critch, and Deborah L. O’connor. "Les besoins en fer jusqu’à l’âge de deux ans." Paediatrics & Child Health 24, no. 8 (December 2019): 556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/pch/pxz149.
Full textBouhdjila, Abderrachid. "Martial deficiency in children under five years of age in Batna." Batna Journal of Medical Sciences (BJMS) 7, no. 2 (November 9, 2020): 129–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.48087/bjmsoa.2020.7214.
Full textBillon-Denis, Emmanuelle, and Jean-Nicolas Tournier. "COVID-19 et vaccination : une dérégulation globale." médecine/sciences 36, no. 11 (November 2020): 1034–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/medsci/2020203.
Full textPOUCHAIN, D., B. TUDREJ, G. LE ROUX, and R. BOUSSAGEON. "Développement de Beyfortus® (nirsévimab) dans la bronchiolite à VRS." EXERCER 35, no. 203 (May 1, 2024): 219–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.56746/exercer.2024.203.219.
Full textBarbu-Roth, Marianne, Evelyne Soyez-Papiernik, and Marie-Victorine Dumuids-Vernet. "Stimuler la motricité sur le Crawliskate." Enfance N° 4, no. 4 (December 1, 2023): 339–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/enf2.234.0339.
Full textNeault, Isabelle, Louise Cossette, Kim Houle, and Jean Bégin. "Le développement de la régulation des émotions chez des nourrissons de mères adolescents." Enfance N° 2, no. 2 (June 1, 2015): 179–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/enf1.152.0179.
Full textDissertations / Theses on the topic "Nourrissons – Développement"
Lhote, Myriam. "Le développement de la mémoire haptique de la naissance à quatre mois." Paris 5, 2000. http://www.theses.fr/2000PA05H092.
Full textSutter-Dallay, Anne-Laure. "Impact des symptômes dépressifs maternels postnataux précoces sur le développement cognitif et moteur du nourrisson." Bordeaux 2, 2006. http://www.theses.fr/2006BOR21345.
Full textPostnatal depression, very frequent during the postpartum period, is likely to influence child development ; few studies investigated the specific impact of early postnatal depressive symptoms (PNDS). To assess i) the impact of obstetrical complications (OC's) and of behavioral characteristics of the new-born on PNDS at 6 weeks ii) whether 6 week PNDS predict impaired child development independently from later maternal depressive symptoms iii) direct and indirect effects of these factors on child development at 6 months and 2 years. 598 dyads were followed up over 2 years, with repeated measures of maternal PNDS and child development. The children of mothers with PNDS at 6 w. Were more likely to present with poor cognitive outcome, association partly explained by the existence of maternal depressive symptoms over the follow-up. OC's and new-born behavioral characteristics didn't change these results. Causal pathways analyses show effects of new-born behavioral characteristics and of anxious disorders during pregnancy on child's development. Child developmental outcome might be influenced by both antenatal and postnatal maternal factors as well as by early infants’ factors
Boulanger, Stéphanie. "Le risque socioéconomique et le développement cognitif de l'enfant : contribution de l'environnement familial, de la sensibilité maternelle et des facteurs biologiques." Thesis, Université Laval, 2012. http://www.theses.ulaval.ca/2012/28853/28853.pdf.
Full textBrion, Françoise. "Le devenir de l'enfant prématuré : effets de la sur-stimulation précoce." Lille 3, 1985. http://www.theses.fr/1985LIL30008.
Full textCristo, Borrero Martha. "El desarrollo cognitivo hasta 12 meses del niño prematuro y el programa madre canguro : comparación con niños a término." Master's thesis, Université Laval, 2001. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/51718.
Full textBertrand, Samuel. "Incidence du développement de la "conscience parentale de l'Autre-bébé" durant la grossesse sur la qualité des intéractions précoces (0-3 mois)." Paris 5, 2008. http://www.theses.fr/2008PA05H031.
Full textResearches on early interactions and the baby question more and more intersubjectivity and subject definitions of which have become deficient in psychology. After having discovered a foundation of these in phenomenology, the hypothesis is made that the quality of early interactions (0-3 months) is correlated with the development of "consciousness of the Other one-as a baby", appearing in prenatal parental representations and underlain by foeto-parental relation, reflexive function and narrativity of parents, during pregnancy. This hypothesis was tested on 13 primiparous couples, followed frm the third month of pregnancy of the infant. From a qualitative approach and a desciptive methodology, the results show that early interactions (dyadics and triatics) are all the more appropriate as the parental consciousness of baby is developed. This "consciousness of baby" is formed all the more early during pregnancy that the parents have "integrated/balanced representations. The quality of these prenatal parental representaitons is linked to the harmonious development in the one hand of foeto-parental relation, in which the analogical aperception phenomenologic occupies a central place, on the other hand of narrativity and reflexive function of parents, which allow, or not, a growing incarnation of the subject in a position of narrator distanced from its narrative, in which, the other and the self exist, together. Finally, this research shows the necessity to consider parents-child realtion and its evolution, at the intrapsychic and the dyadic in the primary triangle
Damon, Fabrice. "Développement des préférences pour la familiarité chez le nourrisson." Thesis, Université Grenoble Alpes (ComUE), 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015GREAS033/document.
Full textThe purpose of this work is to examine of the development of face category formation using infants’ visual preferences. We investigated the mechanisms leading to differential face preferences by integrating them in the theoretical framework developed by Valentine (1991), the face-space. We proposed that the way perceptual experience shape the structure of the face-space is a determinant of infants’ face preferences. We postulated that faces close to the central tendency of the face-space (i.e., prototype) will be preferred. We first reported a bias to look more toward adult faces than infant faces from birth to 6 month of age (Studies 1 and 2). Adult faces correspond to a frequently encountered category while infant faces represent a less frequently encountered category. We also showed a downturn of this familiarity bias as infants grow older (Study 3). The preferences showed by younger infants might be linked to a form of false recognition of the caregivers’ faces, due to the massive exposure to these faces. This pattern of preferences was not found in 3-to 12-month-olds presented with child and infant faces (Studies 4 and 5). Conversely, infants showed a tendency to prefer the less familiar faces, depending on their perceptual experience. We then studied 9- and 12-month-olds’ abilities to form categories of faces differing by age, i.e., adult, child, and infant faces, (Study 6). Twelve-month-olds formed discrete categories of adult and infant faces in one hand, and of child and infants faces on the other hand. Nine month-olds showed an asymmetric pattern of behavior, forming categories of child faces that exclude a new infant face, and categories of infant faces that include a new child face. All these infants being exposed to infant faces via nursery, the asymmetry might stem from the influence of the knowledge of this category of faces. In the last study (Study 7), we tried to provide more direct evidences of the link between face preferences and the distance from the prototype in two different populations: 12-month-old human infants, and 3-month-old macaque infants (Macaca mulatta). Preferences for faces close to the prototype in both species suggest a common mechanism leading to face preferences
Champs, de Saint Léger Christophe de. "Etude épidémiologique des enfants nés en 1983 dans le Puy-de-Dôme : leur devenir à deux ans." Clermont-Ferrand 1, 1987. http://www.theses.fr/1987CLF11036.
Full textRose, Isabelle. "Les effets de la contingence lors d'une tâche d'habituation visuelle chez les nourissons de 3 et 6 mois." Doctoral thesis, Université Laval, 2006. http://hdl.handle.net/20.500.11794/18309.
Full textMoreno, Martin. "Aux sources des manifestations de la vie psychique : Processus de psychisation et observation directe en psychanalyse." Sorbonne Paris Cité, 2015. http://www.theses.fr/2015USPCC076.
Full textBooks on the topic "Nourrissons – Développement"
Vauclair, Jacques. Développement du jeune enfant: Motricité, perception, cognition. Paris: Belin, 2004.
Find full textMontagner, Hubert. L' Enfant acteur de son développement. Paris: Stock-Laurence Pernoud, 1993.
Find full textF, Lieberman Alicia, Wieder Serena, Fenichel Emily, and Centre National pour les bébés, les petits enfants et leurs familles., eds. Classification diagnostique de 0 à 3 ans: Études de cas : guide pour l'utilisation de la classification diagnostique des troubles de la santé mentale et du développement de la première et de la petite enfance. Genève: Éditions Médecine et Hygiène, 2000.
Find full textPascal, Mallet, ed. Psychologie du développement: Enfance et adolescence. Paris: Belin, 2003.
Find full textDufoyer, Jean-Pierre. La naissance et le développement de la personnalité dans la première année de la vie. 4th ed. Paris: Presses universitaires de France, 1987.
Find full textDesmond, Morris. Baby: A portrait of the amazing first two years of life. London: Hamlyn, 2008.
Find full textLe monde interpersonnel du nourrisson: Une perspective psychanalytique et développementale. Paris: Presses universitaires de France, 1989.
Find full textMoro, Christiane. L'objet et la construction de son usage chez le bebe: Une approche semiotique du developpement preverbal. Bern: Lang, 2005.
Find full textBook chapters on the topic "Nourrissons – Développement"
Poinso, François. "3. Contributions des Unités mère-bébé à la connaissance du développement précoce des nourrissons." In Orages à l'aube de la vie, 41–56. Érès, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/eres.poins.2009.01.0041.
Full textFalk, Judit. "Mettre les nourrissons sur le ventre : nécessité ou obstacle dans le processus de développement ?" In Observer le jeune enfant en lieu d'accueil, 163–200. Érès, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/eres.caffa.2019.01.0163.
Full textNadel, Jacqueline. "Chapitre 11. L'humain version bébé." In Le développement du nourrisson, 347–70. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0347.
Full textRochat, Philippe. "Chapitre 12. Connaissance de soi." In Le développement du nourrisson, 371–86. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0371.
Full textLamour, Martine. "Chapitre 15. Stratégies adaptatives précoces : une approche interactive de la psychopathologie du nourrisson." In Le développement du nourrisson, 455–502. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0455.
Full textBertoncini, Josiane, and Thierry Nazzi. "Chapitre 13. Développement précoce de la perception de la parole." In Le développement du nourrisson, 387–424. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0387.
Full textRangon, Claire-Marie, Silvia Fortes, and Pierre Gressens. "Chapitre 2. Le développement précoce du cerveau." In Le développement du nourrisson, 25–55. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0025.
Full textPêcheux, Marie-Germaine. "Chapitre 14. Une approche socio-constructiviste du développement cognitif du jeune enfant." In Le développement du nourrisson, 425–53. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0425.
Full textMellier, Daniel. "Chapitre 10. Les bébés nés prématurément : quels risques pour le développement psychologique ?" In Le développement du nourrisson, 309–45. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0309.
Full textLécuyer, Roger. "Chapitre 9. La représentation va pouvoir commencer." In Le développement du nourrisson, 271–308. Dunod, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.3917/dunod.lecuy.2004.02.0271.
Full text