To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Peak experience.

Journal articles on the topic 'Peak experience'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Peak experience.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Scott, Daniel G., and Jessica Evans. "Peak experience project." International Journal of Children's Spirituality 15, no. 2 (2010): 143–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1364436x.2010.497648.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Hollander, Daniel B., and Edmund O. Acevedo. "Successful English Channel Swimming: The Peak Experience." Sport Psychologist 14, no. 1 (2000): 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/tsp.14.1.1.

Full text
Abstract:
The unique experience of swimming the English Channel is a test of mind and body to overcome multiple challenges. The purpose of this study was to examine psychological characteristics and reflective meanings of these swimmers. Eight English Channel swimmers were interviewed. Inductive and deductive analyses compared interviews and interpretations with the coinvestigator and swimmers. Themes included the cognitive orientations of mental toughness, while cognitive strategies included goal setting, Compartmentalization of time, and/or swimming distance, and self-regulation. Descriptions of the experience of the swim included an ease of swimming at the beginning, more aversive experiences in the middle, and a paradoxical euphoria and letdown upon completion of the swim. Several swimmers noted the perception of increased occupational effectiveness, self-confidence, and an awareness of unlimited potential. Whereas, other swimmers noted a more competitive post event focus. Reflective experiences supported Maslow’s notion that the meaning associated with a peak experience augments daily life.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Solberg, Ragnhild Torvanger, and Nicola Dibben. "Peak Experiences with Electronic Dance Music." Music Perception 36, no. 4 (2019): 371–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2019.36.4.371.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper investigates the role of musical features in shaping peak-pleasurable experiences of electronic dance music (EDM). Typically, large structural and dynamic changes occur in an EDM track, which can be referred to as the break routine, consisting of breakdown, build-up, and drop. Twenty-four participants listened to four EDM excerpts featuring break routines, and one excerpt without a break routine. Measures were taken of skin conductance, self-reported affect, and embodied aspects of subjective experience, and incidence of pleasant bodily sensations. Participants reported intense affective experience with EDM despite being removed from the club context, and attributed this experience to the drop in particular. They described these experiences as energizing and uplifting, and pointed to an embodied, kinaesthetic experience of the music. Drop sections of the music were associated with significantly higher skin conductance response than other sections of the break routine. Analysis confirms correlation between specific acoustic and musical features and peak-response as observed with other music genres, and also identifies novel musical characteristics particular to EDM associated with peak experience. This shows that pleasurable peak experience with EDM is related to specific musical features, and has embodied spatial and kinaesthetic experiential qualities even when listened to without dancing and away from the club context.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Robinson, Ray. "Peak Experience for Professional Development." Music Educators Journal 77, no. 7 (1991): 35–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3398170.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Cruz, Laurie D. "Chautauqua '87 - A Peak Experience." AORN Journal 46, no. 4 (1987): 742–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0001-2092(07)66502-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Farmer, Rod. "Peak Experience in a Barnyard." Journal of Humanistic Psychology 37, no. 2 (1997): 38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00221678970372007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Yeagle, Ellen H., Gayle Privette, and Frances Y. Dunham. "Highest Happiness: An Analysis of Artists' Peak Experience." Psychological Reports 65, no. 2 (1989): 523–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1989.65.2.523.

Full text
Abstract:
Descriptions of peak experiences collected from 29 exhibiting artists were compared with parallel data from a comparison group of 123 men and women enrolled in university courses in social sciences. Each person narratively described a personal incident of highest happiness, then rated a series of descriptions on Likert-type scales. A multivariate analysis of variance indicated no significant differences in the two groups' descriptions of peak experience, and subsequent t tests showed no significant differences on any item. Responses from both groups provide a constellation of experiential correlates of the peak experience consistent with major theoretical descriptions of subjective characteristics of the event. Demographic differences between the groups, triggers, and experiential characteristics of peak experiences were examined. Differences on demographic variables and triggers and the absence of significant differences in the descriptions of peak experience suggest one approach that would bolster Maslow's position of the universality of the peak experience.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Privette, Gayle, and David Sherry. "Reliability and Readability of Questionnaire: Peak Performance and Peak Experience." Psychological Reports 58, no. 2 (1986): 491–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1986.58.2.491.

Full text
Abstract:
The Questionnaire: Peak Performance and Peak Experience is based on both research and theoretical content found through a comparative analysis of literature germane to peak performance, peak experience, and flow. The Dale-Chall Readability Formula indicated that readability of the questionnaire is at the upper limits of the 9th and 10th grades. A Pearson product-moment correlation, based on 33 paired trials, was used to estimate over-all reliability (.70) and reliability of 42 individual test items. Scores were from 21 women and 12 men, aged 20 to 49 yr.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Corbin, Charles B., and Robert P. Pangrazi. "Physical Activity Pyramid Rebuffs Peak Experience." ACSM'S Health & Fitness Journal 2, no. 1 (1998): 12???17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/00135124-199801000-00007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Ryan, Chris, Birgit Trauer, Jenny Kave, Arti Sharma, and Sanjay Sharma. "Backpackers—What is the Peak Experience?" Tourism Recreation Research 28, no. 3 (2003): 93–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02508281.2003.11081421.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

조영주. "Peak Performance Experience in Korea Dance." Korean Journal of Dance Studies 57, no. 6 (2015): 107–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.16877/kjds.57.6.201511.107.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Just, David R., Ozge Sigirci, and Brian Wansink. "Peak-end pizza: prices delay evaluations of quality." Journal of Product & Brand Management 24, no. 7 (2015): 770–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/jpbm-01-2015-0802.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this paper is to determine if the level of payment required for consumption changed the relationship between a consumer’s overall evaluation of a hedonic consumption experience and the evaluation of first, middle, last piece and peak consumption experiences. Design/methodology/approach Diners at an all-you-can-eat restaurant were either charged $4 or $8 for an Italian lunch buffet. Their taste, satisfaction and enjoyment evaluation of each piece of pizza they had was taken along with other measures of behavior and self-perceptions. Using regression analysis, we examine the relationship between these single event evaluations and their overall evaluations of the experience. Findings For the diners who paid $4 for their buffet, overall taste, satisfaction and enjoyment evaluation depend on the taste of the last piece of the pizza and the peak taste consistent with prior findings. For those paying $8 for the buffet, the first piece of pizza is more important in predicting the overall taste, satisfaction and enjoyment ratings. Practical implications Consumers do not evaluate their meal experience based on every moment of their experience. Rather, just a few moments appear to impact overall evaluation. Firms that sell access to a series of experiences, such as an all-you-can-eat buffet, should focus on leading customers to the best experience first particularly when prices may be considered moderate to high. Originality/value In this paper, we seek to unravel the relationship between price paid and the peak-end heuristic by examining the importance of peak and end experiences under two different pricing regimes. Our study also indicates that the peak-end rule may depend on specific contextual factors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Purnell, Oli. "Towards peak experience - exploring the influence of wilderness on conscious awareness." BJPsych Open 7, S1 (2021): S281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2021.748.

Full text
Abstract:
AimsThis study examines what effect wilderness has on our conscious awareness, and by extension of that meta-cognition; our physical, mental and spiritual health. It reviews available scientific and artistic literature and integrates this with interviews in order to generate original grounded theory. It was found that intensity of wilderness experience varied proportionally with four mediators; Challenge, Immersion, Beauty and Time. With these maximised, experiences broadly within four themes occurred; Increased Awareness, Confidence, Perspective and Connectedness. When sufficiently intense, these four experiences amalgamated to elicit what Maslow described as 'Peak Experience'. As such, this thesis unexpectedly provides a pragmatic recipie towards peak experience, and a map of one's potential psychological journey, in the context of wilderness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

McDonald, Matthew G., Stephen Wearing, and Jess Ponting. "The nature of peak experience in wilderness." Humanistic Psychologist 37, no. 4 (2009): 370–85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08873260701828912.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Stettner, Arianthe C. "A Peak Experience in the Rocky Mountains." Tourism Recreation Research 16, no. 2 (1991): 60–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02508281.1991.11014629.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Privette, Gayle, Kwang-Kuo Hwang, and Charles M. Bundrick. "Cross-Cultural Measurement of Experience: Taiwanese and Americans' Peak Performance, Peak Experience, Misery, Failure, Sport, and Average Events." Perceptual and Motor Skills 84, no. 3_suppl (1997): 1459–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1459.

Full text
Abstract:
To compare Taiwanese and Americans on selected experiential personality dimensions, the Experience Questionnaire was translated and tested with 27 Taiwanese in an American university. Descriptions by 129 Taiwanese of peak performance, peak experience, misery, failure, sport, and average events were compared with those made by 123 Americans. Analysis of variance with repeated measures of factors indicated that both samples uniformly characterized processes of peak performance as full focus with clarity of self in process. The Taiwanese considered failure more significant than the Americans who denied clarity of self in misery and failure and more generally endorsed peak experience than the Taiwanese. The study extends the credibility of experience: experiential events can simultaneously have cross-cultural generality and inner processes that are culturally sensitive.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Naor, Lia, and Ofra Mayseless. "How Personal Transformation Occurs Following a Single Peak Experience in Nature: A Phenomenological Account." Journal of Humanistic Psychology 60, no. 6 (2017): 865–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022167817714692.

Full text
Abstract:
This study focuses on a particular form of positive transformation taking place in the wilderness, defined here as peak transformative experience. A large number of studies have been conducted on the negative transformative effect of a single traumatic event, while very little research has focused on positive transformational events. We addressed this lacuna by studying a unique case of quick positive transformation, taking place in nature. This study goes beyond the common description and outcome of the peak experience by focusing specifically on the process of personal transformation. Applying a phenomenological approach, 15 participants aged 28 to 70 years, who identified as having had such an experience, were interviewed. Analysis of these interviews revealed the “essence” of the peak experience in nature which led to the rapid transformation. This essence involved the recognition of formerly unknown aspects of self, projected onto nature and experienced in an embodied way, evoking an insight into a meaningful personal issue. Choosing to own these newly discovered aspects and integrate them resulted in rapid personal transformation. The findings are discussed, underscoring the centrality of nature in this process, the importance of free choice, and the potential for harnessing positive transformative peak experiences in nature for human development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Privette, Gayle, and Charles M. Bundrick. "Measurement of Experience: Construct and Content Validity of the Experience Questionnaire." Perceptual and Motor Skills 65, no. 1 (1987): 315–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1987.65.1.315.

Full text
Abstract:
The Experience Questionnaire, a research instrument measuring experiential data, was derived from literature on peak performance and peak experience, with constructs extended to comprise negative extremes of performance and feeling. The efficacy of the Experience Questionnaire is tested by addressing accessibility and salience of experiential data and independence of construct events. Data on construct events (peak performance, peak experience, flow, average events, misery, and failure) were gathered from 123 adults. Since over-all event effects were significant on repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance, discriminant analysis and analysis of variance with repeated measures were used. Construct independence and salience of experiential data are supported by correct classification of 97.7% of construct events and differences among construct events on factor and item scores that provide clear, differential descriptions. Peak performance is characterized by full focus and self in clear process. The role of other people is unimportant. Peak experience is noted for fulfillment, significance, spirituality, and the importance of other people. Play, other people, and outer structure, but not spirituality, are endorsed in flow. Average events have outer structure, but full focus is a negative correlate. Misery is characterized by spirituality and significance, but other people, playfulness, and self in clear process are denied. Failure is characterized by spirituality but lacks fulfillment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Gordon, Ronald D. "Dimensions of Peak Communication Experiences: An Exploratory Study." Psychological Reports 57, no. 3 (1985): 824–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1985.57.3.824.

Full text
Abstract:
A brief inventory of characteristics of “peak communication experiences” was developed from Maslow's model of the generalized peak experience. Data from 74 subjects were submitted to principal components analysis with varimax rotation, yielding six factors which accounted for 66.1% of the total variance, as follows: Loving Acceptance (25.9%), Openminded Insight (10.2%), Spontaneity (9.2%), Pleasant Fear (80%), Absorption (7.0%), and Self-detachment (5 8%). Analyses of variance showed that women rated their peak communication experiences higher than men on Loving Acceptance and Spontaneity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Ebersole, Peter, and Russell C. Baumer. "Self-Reports of Peak and Nadir Experience Effects." Perceptual and Motor Skills 66, no. 3 (1988): 950. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1988.66.3.950.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

DeMares, Ryan. "Human Peak Experience Triggered by Encounters with Cetaceans." Anthrozoös 13, no. 2 (2000): 89–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.2752/089279300786999914.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Croucher, Richard, Shaun Tyson, and Alan Wild. "‘Peak’ Employers’ Organizations: International Attempts at Transferring Experience." Economic and Industrial Democracy 27, no. 3 (2006): 463–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143831x06065964.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Shcherbakov, Pavel, Fabrizio Dabbene, and Boris Polyak. "Solutions of Stable Difference Equations Probably Experience Peak." IFAC-PapersOnLine 53, no. 2 (2020): 4762–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ifacol.2020.12.1001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Boggs, Peter B. "Monitoring peak flow using control charts: Comments from experience." Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology 106, no. 6 (2000): 1205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1067/mai.2000.111149.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Siebert, Al. "My transforming peak experience was diagnosed as paranoid schizophrenia." New Directions for Mental Health Services 2000, no. 88 (2000): 103–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/yd.23320008811.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

MacKnee, Chuck. "Peak Sexual and Spiritual Experience: Exploring the Mystical Relationship." Theology & Sexuality 1996, no. 5 (1996): 97–115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/135583589600300507.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Hoffman, Edward, Susan Kaneshiro, and William C. Compton. "Peak-Experiences Among Americans in Midlife." Journal of Humanistic Psychology 52, no. 4 (2012): 479–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022167811433851.

Full text
Abstract:
Though much attention, professionally and in popular American culture, has been accorded to midlife angst and turmoil, no published research has been conducted on peak-experiences during this major lifespan period. In this study, 153 people aged 40 to 65 years in the United States responded to a questionnaire asking them to describe a recent joyful experience and its subsequent impact on their outlook on life. They were also asked to rate it numerically on a 5-point Likert-type scale. Their qualitative reports were subsequently coded into 13 categories. Peaks involving interpersonal joy were reported significantly most frequently, more than all other categories combined. These were followed in frequency by those comprising respectively external achievement and personal growth. Small percentages involved religious peaks in either institutional or noninstitutional settings, recovery from illness/accident, nature, skill mastery, philosophical musing, developmental landmark, homecoming, nostalgia, and serenity. The implications for better understanding and optimizing midlife functioning are discussed, and future research avenues are highlighted.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Purnell, Oli. "Exploring the influence of wilderness on conscious awareness." BJPsych Open 7, S1 (2021): S281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjo.2021.749.

Full text
Abstract:
AimsThis study examined what effect wilderness has on our conscious awareness, and by extension of that meta-cognition; our physical, mental and spiritual health.BackgroundMethodIt reviews available scientific and artistic literature and integrates this with interviews in order to generate original grounded theory.ResultIt was found that intensity of wilderness experience varied proportionally with four mediators; Challenge, Immersion, Beauty and Time. With these maximised, experiences broadly within four themes occurred; Increased Awareness, Confidence, Perspective and Connectedness. When sufficiently intense, these four experiences amalgamated to elicit what Maslow described as 'Peak Experience'.ConclusionAs such, this thesis unexpectedly provides a pragmatic recipie towards peak experience, and a map of one's potential psychological journey, in the context of wilderness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Saleh, Ezzat M., Ahmed A. Abdullwahab, and Mohammed M. Kammal. "Age and sex incidence of hypopharyngeal tumours in Upper Egypt: Assuit University experience." Journal of Laryngology & Otology 109, no. 8 (1995): 737–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0022215100131184.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThree hundred and seventeen patients with hypopharyngeal tumours presented at Assuit University Hospital and were studied retrospectively with regard to sex and age incidence. Postericoid tumours form the majority (50.1 per cent) followed by pyriform fossa tumours (26.5 per cent) and finally posterior pharyngeal wall tumours (23.4 per cent). Men form the majority (211 out of 317, i.e. 69.7 per cent). The age incidence shows two peaks at 31–35 years and 56–60 years. The first peak is formed mainly of women while the second peak is formed mainly of men.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Villinger, Karoline, Deborah R. Wahl, Harald T. Schupp, and Britta Renner. "Memorable meals: The memory-experience gap in day-to-day experiences." PLOS ONE 16, no. 3 (2021): e0249190. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0249190.

Full text
Abstract:
Research shows that retrospective memory is often more extreme than in-the-moment experiences. While investigations into this phenomenon have mostly focused on distinct, one-time experiences, we examined it with respect to recurring day-to-day experiences in the eating domain, focusing on variables of the snapshot model—i.e., the most intense and the final experience. We used a smartphone-based Ecological Momentary Assessment to assess the food intake and eating happiness of 103 participants (82.52% female, Mage = 21.97 years) over eight days, and then calculated their best (positive peak), worst (negative peak) and final experiences. Remembered eating happiness was assessed immediately after the study (immediate recall) and after four weeks (delayed recall). A significant memory-experience gap was revealed at immediate recall (d = .53). Remembered eating happiness was predicted by the worst eating experience (β = .41, p < .001), but not by the best or final eating experience. Analyzing changes over time did not show a significant memory-experience gap at delayed recall, but did reveal a similar influence of the worst eating experience (β = .39, p < .001). Findings indicate that, in the domain of eating, retrospective memory is mainly influenced by negative experiences. Overall, the results indicate that the snapshot model is a valid conceptualization to explain recall of both outstanding and day-to-day experiences.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Rich, Grant Jewell. "Phone Jams: Improvisation and Peak Experience in Phone Sex Workers." Anthropology of Consciousness 9, no. 4 (1998): 82–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/ac.1998.9.4.82.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Privette, Gayle. "Experience as a Component of Personality Theory." Psychological Reports 56, no. 1 (1985): 263–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1985.56.1.263.

Full text
Abstract:
Experience, an internal state or process, has salience, as does behavior, as a basic data category of personality. Two key experiential phenomena, which exemplify experience as a data category, were identified and operationally defined by a model. Peak experience and peak performance were taken to be prototypes of feeling and performance. Possible relations between these phenomena and ramifications for selected behavioral and personality variables were suggested.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Islamy, Athoillah. "DIALECTIC MOTIVATION, BEHAVIOR AND SPIRITUAL PEAK EXPERIENCE IN THE PERSPECTIVE OF ISLAMIC PSYCHOLOGY." Alfuad: Jurnal Sosial Keagamaan 3, no. 2 (2019): 35. http://dx.doi.org/10.31958/jsk.v3i2.1759.

Full text
Abstract:
This study aims to find the correlation of motivation, behavior and spiritual peak experience in the view of Islamic psychology. This type of research, namely qualitative research in the form of literature study. The primary data source of this research, namely the theory of hierarchy of human basic needs from Abraham Maslow. Meanwhile, secondary research data sources, namely various other relevant western motivational theories. The theory is used as a knife of analysis, namely the theory of motivation of the human soul from al-Ghazali which includes dimensions of heart, reason and lust. This study concludes that in the perspective of Islamic psychology there is a strong relationship between motivation and the behavior of every human being in the process of achieving spiritual peak experiences. The relationship of motivation and human behavior in the perspective of Islamic psychology that is more likely to emphasize religious motives (spiritual), both in psychological and physiological conditions of humans. motivation and behavior in Islam are very closely related to various stages in the process towards the ultimate spiritual human experience.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Hwang, Kum Hee, and Shick Won Choi. "Understanding of Tourism Experience to Applications of Peak-End Rule: A Sample of Ha-Dong Wild Tea Cultural Festival." Journal of Agricultural Extension & Community Development 20, no. 2 (2013): 485–509. http://dx.doi.org/10.12653/jecd.2013.20.2.0485.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

TERUYA, Taro. "The community and the difference of Mushin, Peak Experience, and Flow." Journal of the Philosophy of Sport and Physical Education 30, no. 1 (2008): 47–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.9772/jpspe1979.30.47.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Mouton, Angela R., and Monica N. Montijo. "Love, passion, and peak experience: A qualitative study on six continents." Journal of Positive Psychology 12, no. 3 (2016): 263–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1225117.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Rybnikov, A. I., A. S. Osyka, I. S. Malashenko, L. B. Getsov, A. A. Tchizhik, and S. A. Leontiev. "Coated blade operation experience with a peak load power gas turbine." Surface and Coatings Technology 68-69 (December 1994): 38–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0257-8972(94)90135-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Raettig, Tim, and Ulrich Weger. "Learning as a Shared Peak Experience: Interactive Flow in Higher Education." International Journal of Applied Positive Psychology 2, no. 1-3 (2018): 39–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s41042-018-0011-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Garcia, Ander, Maria Teresa Linaza, Aitor Gutierrez, and Endika Garcia. "Gamified mobile experiences: smart technologies for tourism destinations." Tourism Review 74, no. 1 (2019): 30–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/tr-08-2017-0131.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose The purpose of this paper is twofold: to present gamified mobile experiences as valid tools for DMOs to enrich the experience of tourists, and to present the benefits provided to DMOs by analytics tools integrated on gamified mobile experiences. Design/methodology/approach Staff from three DMOs have generated a gamified mobile experience using a custom authoring tool designed and developed to fulfil their requirements. This gamified experience has targeted families with children visiting Basque Country during off-peak season. The experience has been validated over a period of seven weeks within a pilot project promoted by the local tourist information offices of the DMOs. Data directly provided by tourists and data gathered from analytic tools integrated on the gamified mobile experience have been analysed to fulfil the research objectives presented on the paper. Findings Both DMOs and tourists can benefit from gamified mobile experiences. The integration of analytics tools to gain insights into the behaviour of tourists can be a relevant information source for DMOs. Research limitations/implications The pilot project has targeted a niche tourism market, families with children visiting Basque Country, and has been running during off-peak season. Further studies focusing on other tourist types and different tourism season and destination types will be required to strengthen the validation of the research objectives presented on this paper. Practical implications The paper promotes both the development of gamified mobile experiences and the inclusion of analytics tools for DMOs to obtain relevant information about tourists and the mobile experiences. Originality/value A gamified mobile experience is generated by DMOs, validated on the basis of experience of real tourists. The analytics tools inside the gamified mobile experience provide DMOs with relevant information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Nocentini, Annalaura, Benedetta Emanuela Palladino, and Ersilia Menesini. "Adolescents’ Stress Reactions in Response to COVID-19 Pandemic at the Peak of the Outbreak in Italy." Clinical Psychological Science 9, no. 3 (2021): 507–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702621995761.

Full text
Abstract:
In this study, we evaluated the variation on stress reactions in Italian adolescents during the peak of the COVID-19 pandemic as related to national diffusion of the outbreak, demographic characteristics, and personal experiences related to COVID-19. An online survey was disseminated from April 1 to April 5, 2020. The sample was composed of 5,295 Italian adolescents (75.2% females; mean age = 16.67 years, SD = 1.43), 28.9% of whom presented moderate to high stress reactions during the peak of the pandemic. Older adolescents and females were more likely to be affected by the pandemic. The impact was similar regardless of the varying presence of COVID-19, whether in Lombardy or in regions less affected. Personal experiences with COVID-19 were all associated with stress reactions (direct experience, indirect experience via a friend, or an acquaintance infected). The global nature of the stressor influenced a collective psychological reaction not affected by the specific geographical diffusion of the pandemic.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Kim, Yeong-Sook and 왕은자. "A Phenomenological Study on the Career Development Experience of Office Workers in 30-40s Focused on Peak Experience and Frustration Experience." Korea Journal of Counseling 21, no. 1 (2020): 149–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.15703/kjc.21.1.202002.149.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Baumgartner, Hans, Mita Sujan, and Dan Padgett. "Patterns of Affective Reactions to Advertisements: The Integration of Moment-to-Moment Responses into Overall Judgments." Journal of Marketing Research 34, no. 2 (1997): 219–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002224379703400203.

Full text
Abstract:
The authors develop several hypotheses regarding the integration of moment-to-moment emotional responses into overall ad judgments, using the psychological literature dealing with people's preferences for sequences of hedonic outcomes, and they conduct three studies to test these predictions. The results of Study 1 indicate that consumers’ global assessments of extended affective episodes elicited by advertisements are dominated by the peak emotional experience and the final moment of the series and also are correlated with the pace at which momentary affective reactions improve over time. Ad duration is related only weakly to overall ad judgments, though longer advertisements have an advantage as long as they build toward a peak emotional experience. In Study 2, the authors replicate these findings under more realistic viewing conditions and demonstrate that the results cannot be attributed solely to memory artifacts that are based on recency. Study 3 implicates adaptation as a possible explanation for the preference for delayed peaks and high ends and further explains the weak effects of ad duration by showing experimentally that longer advertisements can both enhance and depress ad judgments depending on how duration affects the peak emotional experience and the final moment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Fleck, S. J., and L. S. Dean. "Resistance-training experience and the pressor response during resistance exercise." Journal of Applied Physiology 63, no. 1 (1987): 116–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1987.63.1.116.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects previous resistance-training experience has on the cardiovascular responses to resistance-training exercises. To investigate this, the intra-arterial blood pressure response of four body builders (BB), six novice weight-trained individuals (NT), and six sedentary controls (SC) were monitored during performance of one-arm overhead presses and one-leg knee extensions. One repetition at the maximal weight possible (1 RM) and sets to voluntary fatigue at 90, 80, 70, and 50% of 1 RM were performed. Across groups, the BB demonstrated a significantly (P less than 0.05) lower peak and a lesser magnitude of response (changes from rest to peak) for systolic and diastolic blood pressures than the the NT and SC groups during both exercises. The BB also demonstrated significantly lower values across groups for peak heart rate and magnitude of heart rate response during arm presses. During knee extensions across groups, peak heart rate but not magnitude of the heart rate response was significantly lower in the BB. The results indicate that previous weight-training experience reduces the pressor response to dynamic resistance exercises.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Ryan, Jennifer D., Sandra N. Moses, Melanie L. Ostreicher, et al. "Seeing Sounds and Hearing Sights: The Influence of Prior Learning on Current Perception." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20, no. 6 (2008): 1030–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20075.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known that previous perceptual experiences alter subsequent perception, but the details of the neural underpinnings of this general phenomenon are still sketchy. Here, we ask whether previous experiences with an item (such as seeing a person's face) leads to the alteration of the neural correlates related to processing of the item as such, or whether it creates additional associative connections between such substrates and those activated during prior experience. To address this question, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to identify neural changes accompanying subjects' viewing of unfamiliar versus famous faces and hearing the names of unfamiliar versus famous names. We were interested in the nature of the involvement of auditory brain regions in the viewing of faces, and in the involvement of visual regions in the hearing of names. Evoked responses from MEG recordings for the names and faces conditions were localized to auditory and visual cortices, respectively. Unsurprisingly, peak activation strength of evoked responses was larger for famous versus nonfamous names within the superior temporal gyrus (STG), and was similar for famous and nonfamous faces in the occipital cortex. More relevant to the issue of experience on perception, peak activation strength in the STG was larger for viewed famous versus nonfamous faces, and peak activation within the occipital cortex was larger for heard famous versus nonfamous names. Critically, these experience-related responses were present within 150–250 msec of stimulus onset. These findings support the hypothesis that prior experiences may influence processing of faces and names such that perception encompasses more than what is imparted on the senses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Scheibehenne, Benjamin, and Géraldine Coppin. "How does the peak-end rule smell? Tracing hedonic experience with odours." Cognition and Emotion 34, no. 4 (2019): 713–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2019.1675599.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Schmitt, Natalie Crohn. "Ecstasy and Peak-Experience: W. B. Yeats, Marghanita Laski, and Abraham Maslow." Comparative Drama 28, no. 2 (1994): 167–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/cdr.1994.0012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Satoda, Kozo, Eiji Takahashi, Takeo Onishi, et al. "Passive Method for Estimating Available Throughput for Autonomous Off-Peak Data Transfer." Wireless Communications and Mobile Computing 2020 (February 17, 2020): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2020/3502394.

Full text
Abstract:
Large demands for mobile traffic subject base stations to frequent short-term and sharp peak loads. Recent analysis of data traffic on commercial mobile networks reported that the traffic peaks can be reduced by an average of 40% without compromising the quality of experience provided to the end user, if a peak load can be shifted for at most 20 s. To reduce peak traffic, we previously proposed a method for off-peak data transfer, with which user equipment (UE) autonomously delays receiving data, and a peak load on a base station can be shifted. In terms of off-peak transfer of data, a significant problem is determining how each UE estimates available throughput. In this paper we propose a method of passively estimating available throughput of each UE. We evaluated the effectiveness of the proposed method through experiments on experimental and commercial LTE networks. The results indicate that our method obtains more than a 0.7 correlation between actual available throughput and estimated throughput.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Norberg, Joakim, Andreas Engström, Viktor Kjellén, and Jan Carlsson. "On the Hunt." Society & Animals 28, no. 3 (2017): 233–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685306-12341458.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The aim of the present study was to examine hunters’ experiences of leisure hunting, with a focus on the human-nonhuman animal relationship aspect of the activity. Interviews with twelve Swedish hunters were conducted and analyzed with an inductive thematic approach. The analysis showed that hunting led to an experience of completeness. This experience was complex, encompassing positive ingredients such as flow, peak experience, and transcendence, but there were also negative emotions such as guilt, disgust, and shame. The study showed that the hunters seemed to be aware of these feelings and that killing an animal was thus an ambivalent experience, involving the contrasting feelings of euphoria and guilt.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Fernandes, John, Kevin Lamb, and Craig Twist. "Exercise-Induced Muscle Damage and Recovery in Young and Middle-Aged Males with Different Resistance Training Experience." Sports 7, no. 6 (2019): 132. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/sports7060132.

Full text
Abstract:
This study compared the time course of recovery after a squatting exercise in trained young (YG; n = 9; age 22.3 ± 1.7 years) and trained (MT; n = 9; 39.9 ± 6.2 years) and untrained (MU; n = 9; age 44.4 ± 6.3 years) middle-aged males. Before and at 24 and 72 h after 10 × 10 squats at 60% one-repetition maximum (1RM), participants provided measurements of perceived muscle soreness (VAS), creatine kinase (CK), maximal voluntary contraction (MVC), voluntary activation (VA), and resting doublet force of the knee extensors and squatting peak power at 20% and 80% 1RM. When compared to the YG males, the MT experienced likely and very likely moderate decrements in MVC, resting doublet force, and peak power at 20% and 80% 1RM accompanied by unclear differences in VAS, CK, and VA after the squatting exercise. MU males, compared to MT, experienced greater alterations in peak power at 20% and 80% 1RM and VAS. Alterations in CK, MVC, VA, and resting doublet force were unclear at all time-points between the middle-aged groups. Middle-aged males experienced greater symptoms of muscle damage and an impaired recovery profile than young resistance trained males. Moreover, regardless of resistance training experience, middle-aged males are subject to similar symptoms after muscle-damaging lower-body exercise.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Jansen, Stefanie, Manuela Boor, Moritz F. Meyer, et al. "Influence of repetitive diving in freshwater on pressure equalization and Eustachian tube function in recreational scuba divers." Diving and Hyperbaric Medicine Journal 47, no. 4 (2017): 223–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.28920/dhm47.4.223-227.

Full text
Abstract:
Introduction: We investigated the effect of repetitive pressure exposure during freshwater dives on Eustachian tube function and the middle ear, assessed by the Eustachian tube function test (ETFT). Methods: This prospective observational cohort study included 23 divers over three consecutive days of diving in freshwater lakes in Nordhausen, Germany. Participants underwent otoscopy and ETFT before the first dive, between each dive and after the last dive. ETFT included regular tympanometry (R-tymp), tympanometry after Valsalva (V-tymp) and after swallowing (S-tymp). The peak pressure difference between the R-tymp and the V-tymp (R-VdP) defined effectiveness of pressure equalization after Valsalva manoeuvres. We evaluated the change in compliance and peak pressure and correlated the results to the otoscopic findings and diving experience. Results: Twenty-three divers performed 144 dives. Middle ear barotrauma was assessed using the Edmonds modification of the TEED scoring system. In the ETFT, the R-tymp peak pressure displayed a negative shift from day one to three (P = 0.001) and differed significantly between the experience groups (P = 0.01). R-VdP did not change significantly on any of the three days of diving (all P > 0.05). Participants without MEBt showed significantly lower R-tymp values than did those with barotrauma (P = 0.019). Conclusion: Repetitive pressure exposure during three consecutive days of freshwater diving led to a negative shift of the peak pressure in the middle ear. Less experienced divers showed significantly higher middle ear peak pressure and higher pressure differences after equalization manoeuvres. Higher middle ear peak pressure was also associated with a higher prevalence of barotrauma.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography