To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Perception. 0.

Journal articles on the topic 'Perception. 0'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Perception. 0.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

González-Zapata, Laura I., Sandra L. Restrepo-Mesa, Juan C. Aristizabal, Estela Skapino, Tatiana S. Collese, Leticia B. Azzaretti, Walter V. Nascimento-Junior, et al. "Reliability and validity of body weight and body image perception in children and adolescents from the South American Youth/Child Cardiovascular and Environmental (SAYCARE) Study." Public Health Nutrition 22, no. 06 (February 13, 2019): 988–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1368980018004020.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractObjectiveTo assess the reliability and validity of body weight (BW) and body image (BI) perception reported by parents (in children) and by adolescents in a South American population.DesignCross-sectional study. BW perception was evaluated by the question, ‘Do you think you/your child are/is: severely wasted, wasted, normal weight, overweight, obese?’ BI perception was evaluated using the Gardner scale. To evaluate reliability, BW and BI perceptions were reported twice, two weeks apart. To evaluate validity, the BW and BI perceptions were compared with WHO BMI Z-scores. Kappa and Kendall’s tau-c coefficients were obtained.SettingPublic and private schools and high schools from six countries of South America (Argentina, Peru, Colombia, Uruguay, Chile, Brazil).ParticipantsChildren aged 3–10 years (n 635) and adolescents aged 11–17 years (n 400).ResultsReliability of BW perception was fair in children’s parents (κ=0·337) and substantial in adolescents (κ=0·709). Validity of BW perception was slight in children’s parents (κ=0·176) and fair in adolescents (κ=0·268). When evaluating BI, most children were perceived by parents as having lower weight. Reliability of BI perception was slight in children’s parents (κ=0·124) and moderate in adolescents (κ=0·599). Validity of BI perception was poor in children’s parents (κ=−0·018) and slight in adolescents (κ=0·023).ConclusionsReliability of BW and BI perceptions was higher in adolescents than in children’s parents. Validity of BW perception was good among the parents of the children and adolescents with underweight and normal weight.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Schwartz, Jean-Luc. "The 0∕0 problem in the Fuzzy-Logical Model of Perception." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120, no. 4 (October 2006): 1795–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.2258814.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Bucher, Tamara, and Michael Siegrist. "Children's and parents' health perception of different soft drinks." British Journal of Nutrition 113, no. 3 (January 23, 2015): 526–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0007114514004073.

Full text
Abstract:
Beverages are among the first independent product choices that school-aged children will make and unhealthy choices can be a threat to children's health. The present study investigated which beverage attributes shape adults' and children's health perceptions. For this purpose, 100 children (fifty-two boys; mean age 8·8 (sd 1·1) years) and their parents were invited to independently perform a beverage-sorting task. Participants were asked to place twenty commonly consumed soft drinks in a line ranging from ‘unhealthy’ to ‘healthy’. The sorting data were analysed using multidimensional scaling with property fitting and hierarchical clustering. Sugar content (βparents= − 0·78, βchildren= − 0·68; P< 0·001), artificial sweeteners (βparents= − 0·68, βchildren= − 0·66; P< 0·001), fruit content (βparents= 0·33, βchildren= 0·36; P< 0·05) and caffeine content (βparents= − 0·45, βchildren= − 0·46; P< 0·01) were found to be the predictors of parents' and children's health perceptions. Parents' and children's estimates were strongly related (rs 0·70 (sd 0·15)); both groups classified the beverages into similar clusters. However, compared with their parents, children perceived beverages such as fruit juices and grapefruit soda to be healthier. In conclusion, parents' and children's health perceptions were strongly related based on the same relevant attributes for evaluation. However, fruit content was considered a more important criterion by children, which might lead to differences in the health perception between children and their parents. Low fruit content and the belief of beverages being ‘natural’ could positively bias perceptions. Therefore, certain soft drinks such as squashes or fruit lemonades are problematic, and the consumer's awareness of their low nutritional quality should be raised.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Hasler, William L. "Augmented visceral perception." Current Treatment Options in Gastroenterology 4, no. 4 (August 2001): 339–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11938-001-0060-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Clifton, W. Scott. "Murdochian Moral Perception." Journal of Value Inquiry 47, no. 3 (July 9, 2013): 207–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10790-013-9382-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Rajsic, Jason, Harendri Perera, and Jay Pratt. "Learned value and object perception: Accelerated perception or biased decisions?" Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 79, no. 2 (November 28, 2016): 603–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1242-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

van Ansem, Wilke JC, Carola TM Schrijvers, Gerda Rodenburg, and Dike van de Mheen. "Is there an association between the home food environment, the local food shopping environment and children's fruit and vegetable intake? Results from the Dutch INPACT study." Public Health Nutrition 16, no. 7 (August 8, 2012): 1206–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1368980012003461.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractObjectiveTo examine: (i) the association between home availability of fruit and vegetables and children's fruit and vegetable intake; (ii) the association between parental perception of the local food shopping environment and the home availability of fruit and vegetables; and (iii) whether the home availability of fruit and vegetables mediates the association between parental perception of the local food environment and children's fruit and vegetable consumption.DesignCross-sectional study.SettingA total of ninety-one primary schools in the Netherlands.SubjectsIn total 1501 primary caregivers completed a questionnaire to measure children's fruit and vegetable consumption, home availability of fruit and vegetables, parental perceptions of the local food shopping environment (price, quality and availability), the child's socio-economic status, the child's ethnicity and maternal height and weight.ResultsThe home availability of fruit and vegetables was positively associated with children's fruit and vegetable intake (P < 0·01 and P < 0·001, respectively). Negative parental perceptions of the local food shopping environment were associated with less fruit available at home (P < 0·05, P < 0·01 and P < 0·05 for price, quality and availability of fruit, respectively). No significant associations were found between parental perception of the local food shopping environment and children's fruit and vegetable consumption. We found no evidence that home availability of fruit and vegetables mediates the association between parental perception of the local food environment and children's fruit and vegetable intake.ConclusionsInterventions focusing on improving the home availability of fruit and vegetables may help to increase children's fruit and vegetable consumption. However, more data are required on factors influencing the home availability of fruit and vegetables.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Shi, Jinlian, and Jing Sun. "Meta-Understanding of Environmental Perception in Tourism: Implications for China’s Tourist Attractions." Sustainability 12, no. 4 (February 22, 2020): 1658. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/su12041658.

Full text
Abstract:
Tourism is a global force in economic growth. To provide policy suggestions for advancing the tourism industry, we adopt a primary indicator, environmental perception, to examine tourism development. We conduct a nationwide meta-analysis to collect the environmental perceptions of residents and tourists in China, i.e., the satisfaction of an attraction’s environment. We analyze the collected information about the environmental perceptions, score the information, either negative (0) or positive (1), and sort these scores according to four socioeconomic classes (administrative units, attraction rating, ticket price, and attraction type). Our results show that residents’ and tourists’ degrees of satisfaction with environmental perception vary significantly among different classes and that unsatisfactory environmental perception indicates potential problems in the environment, products, and services provided by tourist attractions. Accordingly, we propose suggestions to address unsatisfactory environmental perceptions in each class, aiming to improve the degrees of satisfaction with environmental perception and to promote sustainability in tourism development.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

ZHANG, J., A. E. WHILE, and I. J. NORMAN. "Seasonal influenza vaccination knowledge, risk perception, health beliefs and vaccination behaviours of nurses." Epidemiology and Infection 140, no. 9 (November 18, 2011): 1569–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0950268811002214.

Full text
Abstract:
SUMMARYThe relationship between knowledge, risk perceptions, health belief towards seasonal influenza and vaccination and the vaccination behaviours of nurses was explored. Qualified nurses attending continuing professional education courses at a large London university between 18 April and 18 October 2010 were surveyed (522/672; response rate 77·7%). Of these, 82·6% worked in hospitals; 37·0% reported receiving seasonal influenza vaccination in the previous season and 44·9% reported never being vaccinated during the last 5 years. All respondents were categorized using two-step cluster analyses into never, occasionally, and continuously vaccinated groups. Nurses vaccinated the season before had higher scores of knowledge and risk perception compared to the unvaccinated (P<0·001). Nurses never vaccinated had the lowest scores of knowledge and risk perception compared to other groups (P<0·001). Nurses' seasonal influenza vaccination behaviours are complex. Knowledge and risk perception predict uptake of vaccination in nurses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Nordin, Steven, Anna-Sara Claeson, Maria Andersson, Louise Sommar, Jakob Andrée, Klas Lundqvist, and Linus Andersson. "Impact of Health-Risk Perception on Odor Perception and Cognitive Performance." Chemosensory Perception 6, no. 4 (July 10, 2013): 190–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12078-013-9153-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Theophanous, Christos. "Light Perception: A Sestina." Journal of General Internal Medicine 35, no. 12 (September 28, 2020): 3715. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11606-020-06223-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Batailler, Pierre, Patrice François, Van Mô Dang, Elodie Sellier, Jean-Philippe Vittoz, Arnaud Seigneurin, and Jose Labarere. "Trends in patient perception of hospital care quality." International Journal of Health Care Quality Assurance 27, no. 5 (June 3, 2014): 414–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ijhcqa-02-2013-0014.

Full text
Abstract:
Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to investigate trends in patient hospital quality perceptions between 1999 and 2010. Design/methodology/approach – Original data from 11 cross-sectional surveys carried out in a French single university hospital were analyzed. Based on responses to a 29-item survey instrument, overall and subscale perception scores (range 0-10) were computed covering six key hospital care quality dimensions. Findings – Of 16,516 surveyed patients, 10,704 (64.8 percent) participated in the study. The median overall patient perception score decreased from 7.86 (25th-75th percentiles, 6.67-8.85) in 1999 to 7.82 (25th-75th percentiles, 6.67-8.74) in 2010 (p for trend <0.001). A decreasing trend was observed for the living arrangement subscale score (from 7.78 in 1999 to 7.50 in 2010, p for trend <0.001). Food service and room comfort perceptions deteriorated over the study period while patients increasingly reported better explanations before being examined. Practical implications – Patient perception scores may disguise divergent judgments on different care aspect while individual items highlight specific areas with room for improvement. Originality/value – Despite growing pressure on healthcare expenditure, this single-center study showed only modest reduction in patients’ hospital-care perceptions in the 2000s.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Bertoncini, J., and L. Cabrera. "La perception de la parole de 0 à 24 mois." Archives de Pédiatrie 21, no. 10 (October 2014): 1153–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arcped.2014.05.003.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Mwangome, Martha K., Gregory Fegan, Andrew M. Prentice, and James A. Berkley. "Maternal perception of malnutrition among infants using verbal and pictorial methods in Kenya." Public Health Nutrition 18, no. 5 (May 27, 2014): 869–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1368980014001074.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractObjectiveTo compare mothers’ perceptions of their own infants’ nutritional status with anthropometric indicators of undernutrition.DesignA qualitative study and cross-sectional quantitative survey. The qualitative study involved developing tools to assess mother’s perception. Two methods of verbal description and a pictorial scale were developed. The quantitative survey involved measuring maternal perception and comparing it with the anthropometric measures of weight-for-age Z-score (WAZ) and mid-upper arm circumference-for-age Z-score (MUACZ).SettingA rural community setting in Kenya.SubjectsSeventy-four infants aged between 4 and 6 months, and their mothers, living in rural Kenya were enrolled.ResultsUsing verbal description, the positive and negative likelihood ratios were 3·57 (95 % CI 1·44, 9·98) and 0·69 (95 % CI 0·50, 0·96) respectively for MUACZ<−2; and 4·60 (95 % CI 1·60, 13·3) and 0·67 (95 % CI 0·49, 0·92) respectively for WAZ<−2. Using the pictorial scale, the positive and negative likelihood ratios were 8·30 (95 % CI 1·91, 36·3) and 0·69 (95 % CI 0·52, 0·93) respectively for MUACZ<−2; and 4·31 (95 % CI 1·22, 15·0) and 0·78 (95 % CI 0·61, 1·00) respectively for WAZ<−2.ConclusionsIn a rural community, mothers better identify undernutrition in their infants using a pictorial scale than verbal description. However, neither can replace formal anthropometric assessment. Objective anthropometric tools should be validated for identification of severe acute malnutrition among infants aged less than 6 months.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Min, Jungwon, Vivian HC Wang, Hong Xue, Jie Mi, and Youfa Wang. "Maternal perception of child overweight status and its association with weight-related parenting practices, their children’s health behaviours and weight change in China." Public Health Nutrition 20, no. 12 (June 6, 2017): 2096–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1368980017001033.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractObjectiveChildhood obesity has increased rapidly in China, but understanding is limited on how parents perceive their child’s weight status and how this perception affects weight-related parenting practices. We examined maternal perception of her child’s weight status and its association with demographics, subsequent weight-related parenting practices, the child’s health behaviours and weight change.Design/Setting/SubjectsMaternal perception of child’s weight status and health behaviours from the China Health and Nutrition Surveys were assessed at baseline and in follow-up surveys for 816 children aged 6–18 years during 2004–2011. Associations were tested using mixed models.ResultsOverall, maternal and child perceptions of the child’s weight status were fairly consistent (κw=0·56), 63·8 % of mothers had correct perception. While 9·6 % of mothers perceived their child as overweight, 10·9 % of children did so, and 13·6 % of children were indeed overweight. Compared with mothers who viewed their children as normal weight, mothers who thought their children were overweight were more likely to encourage their children to increase their physical activity (OR; 95 % CI: 1·8; 1·0, 3·3) and to diet (4·3; 2·3, 7·8). Children perceived as overweight by their mothers were more likely to have insufficient physical activity (2·8; 1·6, 4·7) and gain more weight during follow-up (BMI Z-score, β (se): 1·0 (0·1); P<0·01) than children perceived by their mothers as normal weight.ConclusionsIn China, mothers who perceive their child as overweight are more likely to encourage their child to exercise and modify their diet for weight management, but this encouragement does not seem to improve the child’s health behaviours and weight status.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Specht, Karsten, C. Paul Stracke, and Jürgen Reul. "Laterality of auditory perception." NeuroImage 13, no. 6 (June 2001): 942. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(01)92284-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Dudenhöffer, Kathrin, and Leonie Hause. "Sound Perception of Electric Vehicles." ATZ worldwide eMagazine 114, no. 3 (February 24, 2012): 46–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1365/s38311-012-0158-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Przybyszewski, Krzysztof, and Tadeusz Tyszka. "Emotional Factors in Currency Perception." Journal of Consumer Policy 30, no. 4 (October 23, 2007): 355–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10603-007-9047-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Yoshimura, Yuji, Shan He, Gary Hack, Takehiko Nagakura, and Carlo Ratti. "Quantifying Memories: Mapping Urban Perception." Mobile Networks and Applications 25, no. 4 (May 23, 2020): 1275–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11036-020-01536-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Banissy, Michael J., Vincent Walsh, and Jamie Ward. "Enhanced sensory perception in synaesthesia." Experimental Brain Research 196, no. 4 (June 17, 2009): 565–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-009-1888-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Shaffer, Dennis M., and Eric McManama. "Remote haptic perception of slanted surfaces shows the same scale expansion as visual perception." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 77, no. 3 (December 17, 2014): 948–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-014-0814-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Pavlova, M. A. "Biological Motion Perception: From Inversion to Upright Display Orientation." Perception 25, no. 1_suppl (August 1996): 118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v96l0203.

Full text
Abstract:
How does biological motion perception change with display orientation? As previously shown, display inversion (180°) completely prevents veridical perception of biological motion. However, with upright orientation (0°), observers are able to recover the invariant structure through biological motion despite reverse transformation (showing the film backwards) or changing the presentation rate (Pavlova, 1995 Perception24 Supplement, 112). In the present experiments, observers saw the biological motion pattern at various display deviations, from inverted to upright orientation (180°, 150°, 120°, 90°, 60°, 30°, 0°), in the right or left hemifield, on a circular screen monitor. The display consisted of an array of 11 dots on the main joints of an invisible walker moving as if on a treadmill. While viewing (60 s), observers pressed a key each time their perception changed from one stable percept to another (eg when the direction of apparent rotation of the pattern reversed). The perceived multistability (the number of key-presses) increased as orientation was varied from inverted to 90°, and then decreased between 90° and upright. The recognition of walking figure improved abruptly with changing orientation: at deviations of 60° and 30° most observer reported seeing the walking figure spontaneously, yet the pattern was seen as multistable. The findings imply the relative power of constraints (such as orientation) in perception of biological motion that is discussed in relation to the KSD principle in event perception [Runeson, 1994, in Perceiving Events and Objects Eds Jansson, Epstein, Bergström (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum) pp 383 – 405].
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Truijens, Sophie E. M., Hennie A. Wijnen, Antoinette M. Pommer, S. Guid Oei, and Victor J. M. Pop. "Development of the Childbirth Perception Scale (CPS): perception of delivery and the first postpartum week." Archives of Women's Mental Health 17, no. 5 (March 25, 2014): 411–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00737-014-0420-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Lindeberg, Sofie, Belinda M. Craig, and Ottmar V. Lipp. "2:0 for the good guys: Character information influences emotion perception." Emotion 19, no. 8 (December 2019): 1495–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000530.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Clifford, Colin. "Motion perception: tipping the microbalance?" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, no. 2 (February 2002): 66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)01874-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

VanRullen, Rufin, and Christof Koch. "Is perception discrete or continuous?" Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, no. 5 (May 2003): 207–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00095-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Denoyelle, F., and S. Marlin. "Surdités de perception d'origine génétique." EMC - Oto-rhino-laryngologie 1, no. 1 (January 2006): 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0246-0351(05)35891-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Mello-Thoms, Claudia, Stanley Dunn, Calvin F. Nodine, Harold L. Kundel, and Susan P. Weinstein. "The Perception of Breast Cancer." Academic Radiology 9, no. 9 (September 2002): 1004–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1076-6332(03)80475-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Shabana, Noor, Valérie Cornilleau Pérès, Andrew Carkeet, and Paul T. K. Chew. "Motion Perception in Glaucoma Patients." Survey of Ophthalmology 48, no. 1 (January 2003): 92–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0039-6257(02)00401-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Puce, A. "S38-1 Face perception: Overview." Clinical Neurophysiology 121 (October 2010): S56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1388-2457(10)60235-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Hurlbert, Anya C. "Visual Perception: Knowing is seeing." Current Biology 4, no. 5 (May 1994): 423–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00092-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Brown, Bryan J. H. "Personal perception and community speculation." Annals of Tourism Research 12, no. 3 (January 1985): 355–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-7383(85)90004-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Parsons, K. C., and M. J. Griffin. "Whole-body vibration perception thresholds." Journal of Sound and Vibration 121, no. 2 (March 1988): 237–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0022-460x(88)80027-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Cohen, Leslie B., Leslie J. Rundell, Cara H. Cashon, and Barbara A. Spellman. "Infants' perception of causal chains." Infant Behavior and Development 21 (April 1998): 347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(98)91560-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Miller, Albert. "On Perception, Perspicuity, and Precision." Chest 115, no. 1 (January 1999): 305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0012-3692(15)38132-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Cleveland, William S., and Robert McGill. "An experiment in graphical perception." International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 25, no. 5 (November 1986): 491–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0020-7373(86)80019-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Pitcairn, T. K. "The perception of facial expression." Ethology and Sociobiology 10, no. 5 (July 1989): 397. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(89)90046-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Engen, Trygg. "Perception of odor and irritation." Environment International 12, no. 1-4 (January 1986): 177–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0160-4120(86)90028-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Cheyette, Samuel J., and Steven T. Piantadosi. "A unified account of numerosity perception." Nature Human Behaviour 4, no. 12 (September 14, 2020): 1265–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-00946-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Ribeiro, Rodrigo. "The Role of Experience in Perception." Human Studies 37, no. 4 (October 10, 2014): 559–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10746-014-9318-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Gentaz, Edouard, Gabriel Baud-Bovy, and Marion Luyat. "The haptic perception of spatial orientations." Experimental Brain Research 187, no. 3 (April 30, 2008): 331–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1382-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Bernardo, Marco V., António M. G. Pinheiro, Paulo T. Fiadeiro, and Manuela Pereira. "Quality perception of specific chromatic impairments." Multimedia Tools and Applications 79, no. 27-28 (March 30, 2020): 19831–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11042-020-08766-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Darling, Warren G., and Robert Bartelt. "Kinesthetic perception of visually specified axes." Experimental Brain Research 149, no. 1 (January 11, 2003): 40–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-002-1333-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Fuermaier, Anselm B. M., Philippa Hüpen, Stefanie M. De Vries, Morgana Müller, Francien M. Kok, Janneke Koerts, Joost Heutink, Lara Tucha, Manfred Gerlach, and Oliver Tucha. "Perception in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder." ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders 10, no. 1 (April 11, 2017): 21–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12402-017-0230-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Horrocks, Emma, Jay Iyer, Althea Askern, Nadine Becuzzi, Venkat N. Vangaveti, and Ajay Rane. "Individual male perception of female genitalia." International Urogynecology Journal 27, no. 2 (September 9, 2015): 307–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00192-015-2836-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Talavera, K., Y. Ninomiya, C. Winkel, T. Voets, and B. Nilius. "Influence of temperature on taste perception." Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences 64, no. 4 (December 18, 2006): 377–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00018-006-6384-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Rönnberg, Niklas. "Sonification supports perception of brightness contrast." Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces 13, no. 4 (July 18, 2019): 373–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12193-019-00311-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

McBrayer, Justin P. "A limited defense of moral perception." Philosophical Studies 149, no. 3 (March 7, 2009): 305–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11098-009-9363-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Lyons, Jack. "Precis of perception and basic beliefs." Philosophical Studies 153, no. 3 (September 1, 2010): 443–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11098-010-9606-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Baruah, Satyabrat Malla Bujar, Deepsikha Nandi, Plabita Gogoi, and Soumik Roy. "Primate vision: a single layer perception." Neural Computing and Applications 33, no. 18 (March 16, 2021): 11765–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00521-021-05868-0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography