To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Perception. Space perception.

Journal articles on the topic 'Perception. Space perception'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Perception. Space perception.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Sun, Jun. "Strengthen the "Non-Visual Perception Experience" in the Design of Public Space." Applied Mechanics and Materials 174-177 (May 2012): 3083–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.4028/www.scientific.net/amm.174-177.3083.

Full text
Abstract:
Since human beings are living in the dynamic environment which requires multi-perceptional experiences, multiple perceptions prevail in every aspect of people’s life. In this article, the writer is concerned with the problems revealed in the design of public space environment, and the important role non-visual perceptional experience plays in the relationship between human being and environment. In the procession of their design, it is necessary for the designers to pay attention to the users' requirements on the non-visual perceptional experience. Making use of several cases of major city public spaces as example, the writer conducted careful survey into the current situation of the actual practice of non-visual perception experience and validates its essential function.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Herbudy Putra, Alexander Dhea. "PERSEPSI PEMANFAATAN LAHAN FASILITAS UMUM DAN LAHAN TERBUKA SEBAGAI RUANG INTERAKSI ANTAR WARGA KOMPLEK PERUMAHAN KASUS STUDI : KOMPLEK PERUMAHAN PURI TIMOHO ASRI 2 DI YOGYAKARTA." Jurnal Arsitektur KOMPOSISI 10, no. 6 (May 1, 2017): 383. http://dx.doi.org/10.24002/jars.v10i6.1097.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract: People’s perception in developing residental housing area could be different between one another. Their perception determine how life is going to be. This is a form of people’s response that driving people to make perception. This study aims to identify people’s perceptions about public spaces and unbuilt land as interaction space, created by residents. The method used is the observation of people’s behavior in utilizing open spaces and undeveloped land in Puri Timoho Asri 2 residential, in Yogyakarta. Interview was conducted in order to gain an understanding of people’s perceptions of open spaces and undeveloped land. The analysis used theory of people’s perception of open public spaces and undeveloped land. The analysis used theory of perception processes. The results showed that perception of the interaction space was developed by people to improve social interaction activities.Keywords: interaction space, process of perception, residential open spaceAbstrak: Persepsi warga masyarakat dalam mengembangkan komplek perumahannya bisa berbeda antar perumahan yang satu dengan yang lainnya. Persepsi warga menentukan bagaimana kehidupan dalam perumahan tersebut nantinya. Hal ini merupakan bentuk tanggapan warga terhadap faktor-faktor pendorong warga untuk berpresepsi.Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengidentifikasi persepsi mengenai pemanfaatan lahan fasum dan lahan yang belum terbangun sebagai ruang interaksi dalam komplek perumahan yang dilakukan secara swadaya oleh warga. Metode yang digunakan adalah obeservasi perilaku warga perumahan dalam memanfaatkan lahan terbuka fasilitas umum (fasum) dan lahan yang belum terbangun sebagai ruang interaksi warga di komplek Puri Timoho Asri 2, Yogyakarta, dan wawancara dengan penghuni agar memperoleh pemahaman persepsi warga. Analisis dilakukan dengan menggunakan teori dari proses persepsi. Hasil yang diperoleh adalah mengetahui persepsi terhadap ruang interaksi yang di kembangkan oleh warga guna meningkatan kegiatan interaksi sosial.Kata Kunci : ruang interaksi, proses persepsi, lahan fasum perumahan
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Sedlmeier, Andreas, and Sebastian Feld. "Learning indoor space perception." Journal of Location Based Services 12, no. 3-4 (October 2, 2018): 179–214. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17489725.2018.1539255.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Legrain, Valéry, Louise Manfron, Marynn Garcia, and Lieve Filbrich. "Does Body Perception Shape Visuospatial Perception?" Perception 47, no. 5 (March 15, 2018): 507–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006618763269.

Full text
Abstract:
How we perceive our body is shaped by sensory experiences with our surrounding environment, as witnessed by poor performance in tasks during which participants judge with their hands crossed the temporal order between two somatosensory stimuli, one applied on each hand. This suggests that somatosensory stimuli are not only processed according to a somatotopic representation but also a spatiotopic representation of the body. We investigated whether the perception of stimuli occurring in external space, such as visual stimuli, can also be influenced by the body posture and somatosensory stimuli. Participants performed temporal order judgements on pairs of visual stimuli, one in each side of space, with their hands uncrossed or crossed. In Experiment 1, participants’ hands were placed either near or far from the visual stimuli. In Experiment 2, the visual stimuli were preceded, either by 60 ms or 360 ms, by tactile stimuli applied on the hands placed near the visual stimuli. Manipulating the time interval was intended to activate either a somatotopic or a spatiotopic representation of somatic inputs. We did not obtain any evidence for an influence of body posture on visual temporal order judgment, suggesting that body perception is less relevant for processing extrabody stimuli than the reverse.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Sewell, Daniel K. "Latent space models for network perception data." Network Science 7, no. 2 (April 15, 2019): 160–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/nws.2019.1.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractSocial networks, wherein the edges represent nonbehavioral relations such as friendship, power, and influence, can be difficult to measure and model. A powerful tool to address this is cognitive social structures (Krackhardt, D. (1987). Cognitive social structures. Social Networks, 9(2), 109–134.), where the perception of the entire network is elicited from each actor. We provide a formal statistical framework to analyze informants’ perceptions of the network, implementing a latent space network model that can estimate, e.g., homophilic effects while accounting for informant error. Our model allows researchers to better understand why respondents’ perceptions differ. We also describe how to construct a meaningful single aggregated network that ameliorates potential respondent error. The proposed method provides a visualization method, an estimate of the informants’ biases and variances, and we describe a method for sidestepping forced-choice designs.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Wexler, Mark. "Voluntary Head Movement and Allocentric Perception of Space." Psychological Science 14, no. 4 (July 2003): 340–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.14491.

Full text
Abstract:
Although visual input is egocentric, at least some visual perceptions and representations are allocentric, that is, independent of the observer's vantage point or motion. Three experiments investigated the visual perception of three-dimensional object motion during voluntary and involuntary motion in human subjects. The results show that the motor command contributes to the objective perception of space: Observers are more likely to apply, consciously and unconsciously, spatial criteria relative to an allocentric frame of reference when they are executing voluntary head movements than while they are undergoing similar involuntary displacements (which lead to a more egocentric bias). Furthermore, details of the motor command are crucial to spatial vision, as allocentric bias decreases or disappears when self-motion and motor command do not match.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Mitrokhin, A., P. Sutor, C. Fermüller, and Y. Aloimonos. "Learning sensorimotor control with neuromorphic sensors: Toward hyperdimensional active perception." Science Robotics 4, no. 30 (May 15, 2019): eaaw6736. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aaw6736.

Full text
Abstract:
The hallmark of modern robotics is the ability to directly fuse the platform’s perception with its motoric ability—the concept often referred to as “active perception.” Nevertheless, we find that action and perception are often kept in separated spaces, which is a consequence of traditional vision being frame based and only existing in the moment and motion being a continuous entity. This bridge is crossed by the dynamic vision sensor (DVS), a neuromorphic camera that can see the motion. We propose a method of encoding actions and perceptions together into a single space that is meaningful, semantically informed, and consistent by using hyperdimensional binary vectors (HBVs). We used DVS for visual perception and showed that the visual component can be bound with the system velocity to enable dynamic world perception, which creates an opportunity for real-time navigation and obstacle avoidance. Actions performed by an agent are directly bound to the perceptions experienced to form its own “memory.” Furthermore, because HBVs can encode entire histories of actions and perceptions—from atomic to arbitrary sequences—as constant-sized vectors, autoassociative memory was combined with deep learning paradigms for controls. We demonstrate these properties on a quadcopter drone ego-motion inference task and the MVSEC (multivehicle stereo event camera) dataset.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

UCHIDA, HIROMI, HIROSHI KURATA, YOSHINORI OGAWA, and KYOZO YONEMOTO. "PERSONAL SPACE PERCEPTION IN HUMANS." Japanese Journal of Physical Fitness and Sports Medicine 35, no. 1 (1986): 22–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.7600/jspfsm1949.35.22.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Cormack, Lawrence K. "Visual Space Perception: A Primer." Optometry and Vision Science 76, no. 7 (July 1999): 442–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199907000-00017.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Hatada, Toyohiko. "Visual Characteristics on Space Perception." JAPANESE ORTHOPTIC JOURNAL 20 (1992): 15–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.4263/jorthoptic.20.15.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Kelly, Jonathan W., Melissa Burton, Brice Pollock, Eduardo Rubio, Michael Curtis, Julio De La Cruz, Stephen Gilbert, and Eliot Winer. "Space perception in virtual environments." ACM Transactions on Applied Perception 10, no. 4 (October 2013): 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2536764.2536765.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Корина, Наталья. "Space Perception and Language Categorization." Journal of Linguistics/Jazykovedný casopis 67, no. 3 (December 1, 2016): 233–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/jazcas-2017-0010.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The study brings a brief view at the problem of cognitive association between the space perception and the categorization reflected in natural language from the linguistic point of view that is very close connected to the linguistic worldview and possesses the ethno-cultural specifics. The analysis is focused on the differences of the vertical and horizontal space models’ cognitive dominants in two relative Slavic languages – Russian and Slovak, and on the possible reasons of its existence. The study is devoted to the prominent Slovak philologist Viktor Krupa.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Chittka, Lars, and Axel Brockmann. "Perception Space—The Final Frontier." PLoS Biology 3, no. 4 (April 12, 2005): e137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030137.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Clifton, Rachel K., Eve E. Perris, and Andre Bullinger. "Infants' perception of auditory space." Developmental Psychology 27, no. 2 (1991): 187–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.187.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Postma, Albert. "Space: from perception to action." Acta Psychologica 118, no. 1-2 (January 2005): 1–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.10.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Grush, Rick, and Alison Springle. "Agency, perception, space and subjectivity." Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 18, no. 5 (July 2, 2018): 799–818. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9582-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Uno, Kuniichi. "War of Perception, Perception of Time." Deleuze and Guattari Studies 12, no. 2 (May 2018): 252–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.3366/dlgs.2018.0307.

Full text
Abstract:
For Gilles Deleuze's two essays ‘Causes and Reasons of Desert Islands’ and ‘Michel Tournier and the World Without Others’, the crucial question is what the perception is, what its fundamental conditions are. A desert island can be a place to experiment on this question. The types of perception are described in many critical works about the history of art and aesthetical reflections by artists. So I will try to retrace some types of perception especially linked to the ‘haptic’, the importance of which was rediscovered by Deleuze. The ‘haptic’ proposes a type of perception not linked to space, but to time in its aspects of genesis. And something incorporeal has to intervene in a very original stage of perception and of perception of time. Thus we will be able to capture some links between the fundamental aspects of perception and time in its ‘out of joint’ aspects (Aion).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Aulia, Febria Rizky, and Luky Dwiantoro. "Literature Review: Peran Kepemimpinan Transformasional dalam Meningkatkan Persepsi Perawat terhadap Handover." Jurnal Manajemen Kesehatan Indonesia 8, no. 3 (December 31, 2020): 178–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.14710/jmki.8.3.2020.178-183.

Full text
Abstract:
Nurses' perceptions of the implementation of handovers is various. The wrong perception of the implementation of handovers can affect the communication process that threatens patient satisfaction and safety. The head of space as a transformational leader has an important role in influencing nurses' perceptions in the implementation of handovers. The method used in this paper is study literature from several journal articles. The author analyses and synthesizes to describe the role of transformational leadership in increasing nurses' perceptions of the implementation of the handover from the literature study that has been done, the four elements of transformational leadership have a positive role in influencing factors affecting nurses' perceptions. The more positive the impact of transformational leadership on perception factors, the more will have a positive impact on nurses' perceptions. Through the application of transformational leadership, nurses' perceptions and the quality of handovers can improve. It can be concluded that transformational leadership has a positive role in increasing the perception of nurses. This transformational leadership style can be used as a solution and is highly recommended for use by managers / heads of space as one solution to improve nurses' perceptions
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Ji, Haohao, Linbo Qing, Longmei Han, Zhengyong Wang, Yongqiang Cheng, and Yonghong Peng. "A New Data-Enabled Intelligence Framework for Evaluating Urban Space Perception." ISPRS International Journal of Geo-Information 10, no. 6 (June 9, 2021): 400. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijgi10060400.

Full text
Abstract:
The urban environment has a great impact on the wellbeing of citizens and it is of great significance to understand how citizens perceive and evaluate places in a large scale urban region and to provide scientific evidence to support human-centered urban planning with a better urban environment. Existing studies for assessing urban perception have primarily relied on low efficiency methods, which also result in low evaluation accuracy. Furthermore, there lacks a sophisticated understanding on how to correlate the urban perception with the built environment and other socio-economic data, which limits their applications in supporting urban planning. In this study, a new data-enabled intelligence framework for evaluating human perceptions of urban space is proposed. Specifically, a novel classification-then-regression strategy based on a deep convolutional neural network and a random-forest algorithm is proposed. The proposed approach has been applied to evaluate the perceptions of Beijing and Chengdu against six perceptual criteria. Meanwhile, multi-source data were employed to investigate the associations between human perceptions and the indicators for the built environment and socio-economic data including visual elements, facility attributes and socio-economic indicators. Experimental results show that the proposed framework can effectively evaluate urban perceptions. The associations between urban perceptions and the visual elements, facility attributes and a socio-economic dimension have also been identified, which can provide substantial inputs to guide the urban planning for a better urban space.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Pokharel, Ramjee Prasad. "Visitors’ Perception and their Experience of Public Open Space of Bhadrakali Premises in Pokhara, Kaski, Nepal." Janapriya Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies 9, no. 1 (December 31, 2020): 159–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.3126/jjis.v9i1.35285.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study is to examine the users’ perception towards the existing management of public open space (POS) because urban public open space is the significant part of urban landscape. POSs are indispensable as it provides wellbeing, beautify the city and enhance quality of urban life so this case study is related to users’ perception about Bhadrakali premises as POS in Pokhara metropolitan city. Exploratory and prediction research design is followed and users’ experiences are analyzed on the basis of different groups of visitors inside the study area. It focuses on visitor’s perception towards attributes like condition of open space and the situation of infrastructure facilities there. The perception of visitor is scanned through filled up closed questionnaire form to express their perception with eight independent parameters such as accessibility, natural attraction, cultural heritage, safety, sanitation, parking facilities, internal layout and surrounding situation in the study area. The mapping process have been done with the help of ArcGIS. The statistical test revealed that there is a strong statistically significant association between the level of perception with the age, gender, caste, occupation and education status of visitors. The relationship between visitors and existing environment of Bhadrakali site play a significant role to understand visitor’s perception which supports in improving places according to their requirement. It is assumed that, positive perceptional indicators refer the popularity and well sophistication of public open space while negative indicators of visitors provide feedback for further space development and proper management of public open spaces.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Wicaksono, Mohammad Aditya Ajie, Nur Intan Simangunsong, and Benny Benyamin Suharto. "Pengaruh Jarak terhadap Persepsi Sehat Penghuni Perumahan Kecamatan Tebet Jakarta Selatan." Jurnal Lanskap Indonesia 13, no. 1 (April 1, 2021): 13–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.29244/jli.v13i1.33321.

Full text
Abstract:
The poor environmental conditions in Jakarta indicate that housing residents are not healthy, therefor the need for green open spaces that are close to housing is very important. The purpose of this study is to determine the effect of green open space on the health perceptions of housing residents. The research method used is descriptive quantitative with primary data collection stages; limiting the distance of the research location area within a radius of 1-3 km; perform multiple linear regression analysis and cross-tabulation. The results of this study indicate that the negative coefficient is not unidirectional; the adolescent and elderly age group had a good healthy perception, reaches 42.7%; male gender showed a good healthy perception 35.4%; Housing residents who do not work have a healthy perception of 33.3%. The perception of health decreases with increasing distance and is increasingly visible in residents who live within 1-3 km of green open space. This research serves as input to related agencies that the minimum distance of green open space to housing is 300-1000 m because it is important to provide good health effects for housing residents.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Töppel, Mandy, and Christian Reichel. "Qualitative Methods and Hybrid Maps for Spatial Perception with an Example of Security Perception." Urban Planning 6, no. 1 (February 24, 2021): 105–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.17645/up.v6i1.3614.

Full text
Abstract:
The security/insecurity of our cities has become the subject of public debate in recent years. The individual intuitions about security or insecurity can vary with age, gender, social background, personal constitution and previous positive or negative experiences. They are also constantly (re)produced, as perceptions of space are individual and selective. Noting these variations, materialised factors also play a major role, e.g., recessed house entrances, dense or high hedges, poor orientation options, dark places, etc. Attributing meaning to these materialised factors, real constructs are formed which create positive or negative narratives about certain (urban) spaces, influencing the actual use and design of urban spaces. To investigate the importance attached to certain spaces, qualitative methods are required for examining socio-spatial situations, perceptual processes and attribution. Using different methods in an explorative and in-depth descriptive research phase, such as expert interviews, user observations, surveys on go-alongs, participatory mapping with detailed information on structural and spatial locations, the advantages and disadvantages of method selection are presented. Berlin’s Alexanderplatz was used as a case study area to determine perceptions of security in urban areas. We confirmed that despite variations, certain subjective perceptions concerning visibility, brightness, and audibility are collective. Additionally, hybrid maps are used to explain how subjective perceptions of space, combined with 3D graphics, can alert architects and city planners to uncertainty among users of public space.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Padatu, Alexander, Gun Mardiatmoko, and Ch C. V. Suhendy. "PERSEPSI MASYARAKAT TENTANG PATTIMURA PARK SEBAGAI RUANG TERBUKA HIJAU." JURNAL HUTAN PULAU-PULAU KECIL 2, no. 1 (April 1, 2018): 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.30598/jhppk.2018.2.1.1.

Full text
Abstract:
Understanding or perceptions of the Ambon city community about the existence of Pattimura Park as a public green open space and its benefits are important to dense population of people can perform various activities, recreation and interact actively. This study aims to determine the community perception of the Pattimura Park as a Public green open space. Research method used was accidental sampling method. The analysis, while the community perception using Chi-square analysis. The results showed that the knowledge of Pattimura Park = 84%, the attitudes = 81.33%, the opinions = 84% and the expectation that benefited = 74.67%. Their various activities in the Pattimura Park cause various perceptions of the park.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Deregowski, J. B. "Real space and represented space: Cross-cultural perspectives." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12, no. 1 (March 1989): 51–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00024286.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis paper examines the contribution of cross-cultural studies to our understanding of the perception and representation of space. A cross-cultural survey of the basic difficulties in understanding pictures—ranging from the failure to recognise a picture as a representation to the inability to recognise the object represented in the picture— indicates that similar difficulties occur in pictorial and nonpictorial cultrues. The experimental work on pictorial space derives from two distinct traditions: the study of picture perception in “remote” populations and the study of the perceptual illusions. A comprison of the findings on pictorial space perception with those on real space perceptual illusions. A comparison of findings on pictorial space perception with those on real space perception and perceptual constancy suggersts that cross-cultural differences in the perception of both real and representational space involve two different types of skills: those related exclusively to either real space or representational space, and those related to both. Different cultural groups use different skills to perform the same perceptual tasks.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Rodríguez-Ramos, Luis F. "Virtual Acoustic Space: Space perception for the blind." Proceedings of the International Astronomical Union 5, S260 (January 2009): 556–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1743921311002845.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis R&D project implements a new way of perception of the three-dimensional surrounding space, based exclusively in sounds and thus especially useful for the blind. The innate capability of locating sounds, the externalization of sounds played with headphones and the machine capture of the 3D environment are the technological pillars used for this purpose. They are analysed and a summary of their main requirements are presented. A number of laboratory facilities and portable prototypes are described, together with their main characteristics.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Loomis, Jack M. "Visual space perception: phenomenology and function." Arquivos Brasileiros de Oftalmologia 66, no. 5 (September 2003): 26–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s0004-27492003000600004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

MAEDA, Taro, and Susumu TACHI. "Space Perception Model which Generates Horopter." Transactions of the Society of Instrument and Control Engineers 25, no. 10 (1989): 1111–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.9746/sicetr1965.25.1111.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

VILAPLANA, Alejandra, and Toshimasa YAMANAKA. "Effect of Smell in Space Perception." International Journal of Affective Engineering 14, no. 3 (2015): 175–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.5057/ijae.ijae-d-15-00010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Schellingerhout, R., A. W. Smitsman, and G. P. Van Galen. "Texture information in tactual space perception." Acta Psychologica 99, no. 1 (June 1998): 93–114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0001-6918(98)00004-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Meyers, Victoria. "Space and the Perception of Time." Journal of Architectural Education 53, no. 2 (November 1999): 91–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/104648899564457.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

BRYANT, DAVID J. "Representing Space in Language and Perception." Mind & Language 12, no. 3-4 (May 4, 2007): 239–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.1997.tb00073.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Masson, J. B. "Olfactory searches with limited space perception." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110, no. 28 (June 26, 2013): 11261–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221091110.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Dergowski, J. B., and A. M. Bentley. "Perception of Pictorial Space by Bushmen." International Journal of Psychology 21, no. 1-4 (January 1986): 743–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207598608247619.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Bryant, David J. "Representing Space in Language and Perception." Mind and Language 12, no. 3&4 (September 1997): 239–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00047.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Viguier, Alain, Gilles Clément, and Yves Trotter. "Distance Perception within near Visual Space." Perception 30, no. 1 (January 2001): 115–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3119.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Dong, Bo, Airui Chen, Yuting Zhang, Changchun Li, Tianyang Zhang, and Ming Zhang. "Inaccurate Space Perception Seeing Through Fences." Perception 49, no. 9 (August 16, 2020): 926–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0301006620946525.

Full text
Abstract:
According to the sequential surface integration process hypothesis, the fine near-ground-surface representation and the homogeneous ground surface play a vital role in the representation of the ground surface. When an occluding box or opaque wall is placed between observers and targets, observers underestimate egocentric distance. However, in our daily life, many obstacles are perforated and cover the ground surface and targets simultaneously (e.g., fences). Humans see and observe through fences. The images of these fences and targets, projected onto observers’ retinas, overlap each other. This study aims to explore the effects of perforated obstacles (i.e., fences) on space perception. The results showed that observers underestimated the egocentric distances when there was a fence on the ground surface relative to the no-fence condition, and the effect of widely spaced thick wood fences was larger than that of narrowly spaced thin iron fences. We further demonstrated that this effect was quite robust when the target size had a visual angle of 1°, 2°, or 4° in three virtual reality experiments. This study may add support for the notion that the sequential surface integration process hypothesis is applicable even if the obstacle is perforated and covers the target.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Dima, Diana C., Tyler Tomita, Christopher Honey, and Leyla Isik. "The representational space of action perception." Journal of Vision 20, no. 11 (October 20, 2020): 1161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/jov.20.11.1161.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Ashmead, Daniel H. "Experiential influences on auditory space perception." Infant Behavior and Development 19 (April 1996): 14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(96)90069-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Sakamoto, Shuichi, Wataru Teramoto, Akio Honda, Yôiti Suzuki, and Jiro Gyoba. "Auditory space perception during self-motion." Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 140, no. 4 (October 2016): 2998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.4969289.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Girshick, A. R., D. Vishwanath, and M. S. Banks. "Pictorial space perception and viewing distance." Journal of Vision 4, no. 8 (August 1, 2004): 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/4.8.18.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Vishwanath, D., A. R. Girshick, and M. S. Banks. "Pictorial space perception and oblique viewing." Journal of Vision 4, no. 8 (August 1, 2004): 375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/4.8.375.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Aczél, János, Zoltán Boros, Jürgen Heller, and Che Tat Ng. "Functional Equations in Binocular Space Perception." Journal of Mathematical Psychology 43, no. 1 (March 1999): 71–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmps.1998.1224.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Stewart, Lauren, and Vincent Walsh. "Music Perception: Sounds Lost in Space." Current Biology 17, no. 20 (October 2007): R892—R893. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.012.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Camiz, Paolo. "Can space perception be gender‐dependent?" International Review of Sociology 6, no. 3 (November 1996): 371–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03906701.1996.9971209.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Popple, Ariella. "Reviews: Visual Space Perception: A Primer." Perception 28, no. 6 (June 1999): 797–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p2806rvw.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Basso, Gianpaolo, Paolo Nichelli, Francesca Frassinetti, and Giuseppe di Pellegrino. "Time perception in a neglected space." NeuroReport 7, no. 13 (September 1996): 2111–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199609020-00009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

masson, jean-baptiste. "Olfactory Searches with Limited Space Perception." Biophysical Journal 106, no. 2 (January 2014): 793a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2013.11.4351.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Kellert, Stephen H. "Space perception and the fourth dimension." Man and World 27, no. 2 (April 1994): 161–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf01278961.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Xiaoxia, Gong, Lu Siyu, and Dong Bo. "The Ground Theory of Space Perception." Psychology of China 3, no. 2 (2021): 228–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.35534/pc.0302028.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Zhdanova, S. Yu, and V. S. Kraeva. "PERCEPTION OF HOME AND DORMITORY BY RUSSIAN AND CHINESE STUDENTS." Bulletin of Udmurt University. Series Philosophy. Psychology. Pedagogy 30, no. 2 (June 25, 2020): 162–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.35634/2412-9550-2020-30-2-162-172.

Full text
Abstract:
The article presents a study of perception of living space. The semantic differential method is used in the research. The relevance of the work is determined by studying the perception of different types of living space in the cultural context. The study was conducted on the sample of students from Russia and China. As a result of the study, general and specific features of perception of home and dormitory spaces among Russian and Chinese students were found. The factor structure of perception of living space among Russian and Chinese representatives was revealed. Empirically, the general factor of comfort describing the perception of the living environment among representatives of both cultures was determined. It is established that perception of the living space of Russian students is focused on the controllability of home and the pragmatism of the dormitory. Affinity is an important parameter of perception of home space for Chinese students. The Chinese students highlight the dynamics and resource intensity in perception of dormitory. Students of both groups prefer their home space rather than dormitory. A dormitory space forms a more positive image in the perception of Chinese students, in contrast to the perception of the subjects from Russia.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography