To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Perceptual interactions.

Journal articles on the topic 'Perceptual interactions'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Perceptual interactions.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Basirat, Anahita, Jean-Luc Schwartz, and Marc Sato. "Perceptuo-motor interactions in the perceptual organization of speech: evidence from the verbal transformation effect." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367, no. 1591 (2012): 965–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0374.

Full text
Abstract:
The verbal transformation effect (VTE) refers to perceptual switches while listening to a speech sound repeated rapidly and continuously. It is a specific case of perceptual multistability providing a rich paradigm for studying the processes underlying the perceptual organization of speech. While the VTE has been mainly considered as a purely auditory effect, this paper presents a review of recent behavioural and neuroimaging studies investigating the role of perceptuo-motor interactions in the effect. Behavioural data show that articulatory constraints and visual information from the speaker'
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kramer, Peter, Ivilin Stoianov, Carlo Umiltà, and Marco Zorzi. "Interactions between perceptual and numerical space." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18, no. 4 (2011): 722–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0104-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Yarrow, Kielan, Patrick Haggard, and John C. Rothwell. "Vibrotactile–Auditory Interactions are Post-Perceptual." Perception 37, no. 7 (2008): 1114–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5824.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Cook, R., C. Aichelburg, P. Shah, and A. Johnston. "Perceptual interactions between dynamic facial features." Journal of Vision 14, no. 10 (2014): 564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/14.10.564.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Polat, Uri, and Dov Sagi. "The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions." Vision Research 34, no. 1 (1994): 73–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(94)90258-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Reed, Catherine L. "Perceptual Dependence for Shape and Texture during Haptic Processing." Perception 23, no. 3 (1994): 349–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p230349.

Full text
Abstract:
Perceptual dependence—the existence of perceptual interactions between the component dimensions of the same stimulus—was investigated for shape and texture during haptic processing. The haptic system combines tactual and kinesthetic information. Previous research has demonstrated that haptic exploration influences the extent to which object properties are integrated. Conditions designed to promote and impede the integration of shape and texture were compared. Perceptual independence was assessed by the use of a speeded-classification paradigm and quantitative tests developed by Ashby and Maddo
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Holly, Jan E. "Vestibular coriolis effect differences modeled with three-dimensional linear-angular interactions." Journal of Vestibular Research 14, no. 6 (2004): 443–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.3233/ves-2004-14603.

Full text
Abstract:
The vestibular coriolis (or "cross-coupling") effect is traditionally explained by cross-coupled angular vectors, which, however, do not explain the differences in perceptual disturbance under different acceleration conditions. For example, during head roll tilt in a rotating chair, the magnitude of perceptual disturbance is affected by a number of factors, including acceleration or deceleration of the chair rotation or a zero-g environment. Therefore, it has been suggested that linear-angular interactions play a role. The present research investigated whether these perceptual differences and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Rockwell, Patricia, David B. Buller, and Judee K. Burgoon. "Measurement of deceptive voices: Comparing acoustic and perceptual data." Applied Psycholinguistics 18, no. 4 (1997): 471–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716400010948.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACTThis study compared vocal features of deception that can be measured by acoustic equipment with vocal features of deception that can be measured perceptually by human coders. As deception researchers have traditionally measured vocal behavior with either acoustic or perceptual methods (but not both), it is uncertain what correspondence, if any, exists between these methods. This study attempted to determine the degree of this correspondence. Deceptive interactions from an earlier study (Burgoon, Buller, Ebesu, & Rockwell, 1994) were used to conduct a detailed analysis of the vocal
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Conrad, Verena, Marco Pino Vitello, and Uta Noppeney. "Interactions between apparent motion rivalry in vision and touch." Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012): 26–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/187847612x646497.

Full text
Abstract:
Introduction: In multistable perception, the brain alternates between several perceptual explanations of ambiguous sensory signals. Recent studies have demonstrated crossmodal interactions between ambiguous and unambiguous signals. However it is currently unknown whether multiple bistable processes can interact across the senses (Conrad et al., 2010; Pressnitzer and Hupe, 2006). Using the apparent motion quartet in vision and touch, this study investigated whether bistable perceptual processes for vision and touch are independent or influence each other when powerful cues of congruency are pro
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Tenenbaum, Joshua B., and William T. Freeman. "Separating Style and Content with Bilinear Models." Neural Computation 12, no. 6 (2000): 1247–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089976600300015349.

Full text
Abstract:
Perceptual systems routinely separate “content” from “style,” classifying familiar words spoken in an unfamiliar accent, identifying a font or handwriting style across letters, or recognizing a familiar face or object seen under unfamiliar viewing conditions. Yet a general and tractable computational model of this ability to untangle the underlying factors of perceptual observations remains elusive (Hofstadter, 1985). Existing factor models (Mardia, Kent, & Bibby, 1979; Hinton & Zemel, 1994; Ghahramani, 1995; Bell & Sejnowski, 1995; Hinton, Dayan, Frey, & Neal, 1995; Dayan, Hin
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Bernstein, Ira H., Victor Bissonnette, and Kenneth R. Welch. "Perceptual and response interactions in semantic priming." Perception & Psychophysics 48, no. 6 (1990): 525–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03211598.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

VALDEZ, A. B., and E. L. AMAZEEN. "Sensory and perceptual interactions in weight perception." Perception & Psychophysics 70, no. 4 (2008): 647–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pp.70.4.647.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Krumhansl, Carol L., and Paul Iverson. "Perceptual interactions between musical pitch and timbre." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18, no. 3 (1992): 739–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.739.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Auvray, Malika, Charles Lenay, and John Stewart. "Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment." New Ideas in Psychology 27, no. 1 (2009): 32–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Poletto, C. J., and C. L. Van Doren. "Perceptual interactions between electrocutaneous loudness and pitch." IEEE Transactions on Rehabilitation Engineering 3, no. 4 (1995): 334–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/86.481973.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Polat, Uri. "Functional architecture of long-range perceptual interactions." Spatial Vision 12, no. 2 (1999): 143–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156856899x00094.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Laing, D. G. "Perceptual odour interactions and objective mixture analysis." Food Quality and Preference 5, no. 1-2 (1994): 75–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0950-3293(94)90010-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Stanford, Terrence R., and Emilio Salinas. "Urgent Decision Making: Resolving Visuomotor Interactions at High Temporal Resolution." Annual Review of Vision Science 7, no. 1 (2021): 323–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-100419-103842.

Full text
Abstract:
Measuring when exactly perceptual decisions are made is crucial for defining how the activation of specific neurons contributes to behavior. However, in traditional, nonurgent visuomotor tasks, the uncertainty of this temporal measurement is very large. This is a problem not only for delimiting the capacity of perception, but also for correctly interpreting the functional roles ascribed to choice-related neuronal responses. In this article, we review psychophysical, neurophysiological, and modeling work based on urgent visuomotor tasks in which this temporal uncertainty can be effectively over
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Ferreira, Vicente, Arancha de-la-Fuente-Blanco, and María-Pilar Sáenz-Navajas. "A New Classification of Perceptual Interactions between Odorants to Interpret Complex Aroma Systems. Application to Model Wine Aroma." Foods 10, no. 7 (2021): 1627. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/foods10071627.

Full text
Abstract:
Although perceptual interactions are usually mentioned and blamed for the difficulties in understanding the relationship between odorant composition and aromatic sensory properties, they are poorly defined and categorised. Furthermore, old classifications refer mainly to effects on the odour intensity of the mixture of dissimilar non-blending odours and do not consider odour blending, which is one of the most relevant and influential perceptual interactions. Beginning with the results from classical studies about odour interaction, a new and simple systematic is proposed in which odour interac
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Parks, Nathan A., Matthew R. Hilimire, and Paul M. Corballis. "Steady-state Signatures of Visual Perceptual Load, Multimodal Distractor Filtering, and Neural Competition." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, no. 5 (2011): 1113–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21460.

Full text
Abstract:
The perceptual load theory of attention posits that attentional selection occurs early in processing when a task is perceptually demanding but occurs late in processing otherwise. We used a frequency-tagged steady-state evoked potential paradigm to investigate the modality specificity of perceptual load-induced distractor filtering and the nature of neural-competitive interactions between task and distractor stimuli. EEG data were recorded while participants monitored a stream of stimuli occurring in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) for the appearance of previously assigned targets. Per
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Aso, Kenji, Takashi Hanakawa, Toshihiko Aso, and Hidenao Fukuyama. "Cerebro-cerebellar Interactions Underlying Temporal Information Processing." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 12 (2010): 2913–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21429.

Full text
Abstract:
The neural basis of temporal information processing remains unclear, but it is proposed that the cerebellum plays an important role through its internal clock or feed-forward computation functions. In this study, fMRI was used to investigate the brain networks engaged in perceptual and motor aspects of subsecond temporal processing without accompanying coprocessing of spatial information. Direct comparison between perceptual and motor aspects of time processing was made with a categorical-design analysis. The right lateral cerebellum (lobule VI) was active during a time discrimination task, wh
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Torralbo, Ana, and Diane M. Beck. "Perceptual-Load-Induced Selection as a Result of Local Competitive Interactions in Visual Cortex." Psychological Science 19, no. 10 (2008): 1045–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02197.x.

Full text
Abstract:
A growing literature suggests that the degree to which distracting information can be ignored depends on the perceptual load of the task, or the extent to which the task exhausts perceptual capacity. However, there is currently no a priori definition of what constitutes high or low perceptual load. We propose that interactions among cells in visual cortex that represent nearby stimuli determine the perceptual load of a task, and that manipulations designed to modulate these competitive spatial interactions should modulate distractor processing. We found that either spatially separating the tas
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Vogt, Stefan, and Heiko Hecht. "Multi-level sensorimotor interactions." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, no. 5 (2001): 906–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01480101.

Full text
Abstract:
We share the authors' general approach to the study of perception and action, but rather than singling out a particular level of “late perceptual” and “early motor” processing for sensorimotor interactions, we argue that these can arise at multiple levels during action preparation and execution. Recent data on action-perception transfer are used to illustrate this perspective.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Estelle, Stephen, Kenzo Uhlig, Leonardo Zapata-Fonseca, et al. "An open-source perceptual crossing device for investigating brain dynamics during human interaction." PLOS ONE 19, no. 6 (2024): e0305283. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0305283.

Full text
Abstract:
The Perceptual Crossing Device (PCD) introduced in this report is an updated tool designed to facilitate the exploration of brain activity during human interaction with seamless real time integration with EEG equipment. It incorporates haptic and auditory feedback mechanisms, enabling interactions between two users within a virtual environment. Through a unique circular motion interface that enables intuitive virtual interactions, users can experience the presence of their counterpart via tactile or auditory cues. This paper highlights the key characteristics of the PCD, aiming to validate its
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Kramer, Peter, Ivilin Stoianov, Carlo Umiltà, and Marco Zorzi. "Erratum to: Interactions between perceptual and numerical space." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18, no. 5 (2011): 1029. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0149-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Wu, Bing, Roberta L. Klatzky, and Ralph L. Hollis. "Force, Torque, and Stiffness: Interactions in Perceptual Discrimination." IEEE Transactions on Haptics 4, no. 3 (2011): 221–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/toh.2011.3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Klorfeld-Auslender, Shira, and Nitzan Censor. "Visual-oculomotor interactions facilitate consolidation of perceptual learning." Journal of Vision 19, no. 6 (2019): 11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/19.6.11.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Motoyoshi, I. "The role of spatial interactions in perceptual synchrony." Journal of Vision 4, no. 5 (2004): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/4.5.1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Delwiche, Jeannine. "The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived flavor." Food Quality and Preference 15, no. 2 (2004): 137–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0950-3293(03)00041-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Frijters, Jan E. R., and Hendrik N. J. Schifferstein. "Perceptual interactions in mixtures containing bitter tasting substances." Physiology & Behavior 56, no. 6 (1994): 1243–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(94)90372-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Li, Qiong, and Ying-Yi Hong. "Intergroup Perceptual Accuracy Predicts Real-Life Intergroup Interactions." Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 4, no. 4 (2001): 341–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430201004004004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Landry, M., and J. Ristic. "The influence of attentional interactions on perceptual processing." Journal of Vision 12, no. 9 (2012): 673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.9.673.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Saito, Joseph M., Philip Li, and Timothy F. Brady. "Decision-making modulates perceptual interference beyond sensory interactions." Journal of Vision 25, no. 9 (2025): 1879. https://doi.org/10.1167/jov.25.9.1879.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Zhou, Youmei, Kevin Thwaites, and Daixin Dai. "Gender Differences of the Elderly on the Social and Perceptual Dimensional Interaction in Neighbourhood Park: A Case Study of Beijing." SPACE International Journal of Conference Proceedings 2, no. 1 (2022): 10–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.51596/sijocp.v2i1.35.

Full text
Abstract:
Neighbourhood parks are an essential activity venue for contemporary older adults and potentially impact their health. Most research has focused on the physical and visual dimensions of the park, with some studies demonstrating differences in the visual preferences of gender. Still, there is a lack of detailed research on the social and perceptual dimensions of male and female older people and a lack of research on psycho-emotional satisfaction and mechanisms to alleviate the psychological isolation of older people of different genders. In this study, a mixed research method was conducted to i
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

van der Smagt, Maarten J., Christian Wehrhahn, and Thomas D. Albright. "Contextual Masking of Oriented Lines: Interactions Between Surface Segmentation Cues." Journal of Neurophysiology 94, no. 1 (2005): 576–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00366.2004.

Full text
Abstract:
The ability of human observers to detect and discriminate a single feature of a visual image deteriorates markedly when the targeted feature is surrounded by others of a similar kind. This perceptual masking is mirrored by the suppressive effects of surround stimulation on the responses of neurons in primary visual cortex (area V1). Both perceptual and neuronal masking effects are partially relieved, however, if the targeted image feature is distinguished from surrounding features along some dimension, such as contour orientation. Masking relief is likely to play an important role in perceptua
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Raja, Vicente. "From metaphor to theory: the role of resonance in perceptual learning." Adaptive Behavior 27, no. 6 (2019): 405–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1059712319854350.

Full text
Abstract:
Unlike dominant cognitivist theories that take perceptual learning to be a process of enriching sensory stimulation with previous knowledge, ecological psychologists take it to be an enhancement in the detection of already rich perceptual information. The difference between beginners and experts is that the latter detect better information to support their task goals. While the study of perceptual learning in terms of perceptual information and perceiver–environment interactions is common in the ecological literature, ecological psychology still lacks a story regarding the way perceptual infor
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Clark, Logan D., and Sara L. Riggs. "Investigating the Use of Movement Kinematics to Assess Perceptual Ambiguity in Virtual Reality." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 63, no. 1 (2019): 2318–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1071181319631156.

Full text
Abstract:
With the emergence of 3D direct-selection interfaces in virtual reality (VR) displays, specialized metrics may be needed to assess the efficiency of these complex interactions. Kinematic movement trajectory analysis, an established technique in experimental psychology, may provide a useful framework for quantifying meaningful patterns of user interaction using the 3D position data recorded by most VR displays. We explored this possibility by investigating the effectiveness of a kinematic approach for identifying ambiguous interface elements in VR by detecting movements initiated toward incorre
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Serniclaes, Willy. "On the invariance of speech percepts." ZAS Papers in Linguistics 40 (January 1, 2005): 177–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.21248/zaspil.40.2005.265.

Full text
Abstract:
A fundamental question in the study of speech is about the invariance of the ultimate percepts, or features. The present paper gives an overview of the noninvariance problem and offers some hints towards a solution. Examination of various data on place and voicing perception suggests the following points. Features correspond to natural boundaries between sounds, which are included in the infant's predispositions for speech perception. Adult percepts arise from couplings and contextual interactions between features. Both couplings and interactions contribute to invariance. But this is at the ex
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

McMains, Stephanie A., and Sabine Kastner. "Defining the Units of Competition: Influences of Perceptual Organization on Competitive Interactions in Human Visual Cortex." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, no. 11 (2010): 2417–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21391.

Full text
Abstract:
Multiple stimuli that are present simultaneously in the visual field compete for neural representation. At the same time, however, multiple stimuli in cluttered scenes also undergo perceptual organization according to certain rules originally defined by the Gestalt psychologists such as similarity or proximity, thereby segmenting scenes into candidate objects. How can these two seemingly orthogonal neural processes that occur early in the visual processing stream be reconciled? One possibility is that competition occurs among perceptual groups rather than at the level of elements within a grou
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Thellman, Sam, and Tom Ziemke. "The Perceptual Belief Problem." ACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction 10, no. 3 (2021): 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/3461781.

Full text
Abstract:
The explainability of robotic systems depends on people’s ability to reliably attribute perceptual beliefs to robots, i.e., what robots know (or believe) about objects and events in the world based on their perception. However, the perceptual systems of robots are not necessarily well understood by the majority of people interacting with them. In this article, we explain why this is a significant, difficult, and unique problem in social robotics. The inability to judge what a robot knows (and does not know) about the physical environment it shares with people gives rise to a host of communicat
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Fudin, Robert. "Re-Examination of Evidence Questions Christman's (1989) Report of Moderate Experimental Support for the Visual Spatial Frequency Hypothesis." Perceptual and Motor Skills 80, no. 3 (1995): 955–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.80.3.955.

Full text
Abstract:
The visual spatial frequency hypothesis contends that perceptual characteristics of stimulus arrays can affect the magnitude and direction of hemispheric asymmetries in laterality experiments. In a 1989 literature review, Christman reported that 45 of 79 experimental comparisons yielded significant interactions for side of hemispheric advantage x perceptual characteristic which supported the visual spatial frequency hypothesis, a level of support he characterized as moderate. Re-examination of those 45 outcomes shows that in 20 of them either a significant interaction for side of hemispheric a
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Curran, William, Colin W. G. Clifford, and Christopher P. Benton. "The hierarchy of directional interactions in visual motion processing." Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 276, no. 1655 (2008): 263–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1065.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known that context influences our perception of visual motion direction. For example, spatial and temporal context manipulations can be used to induce two well-known motion illusions: direction repulsion and the direction after-effect (DAE). Both result in inaccurate perception of direction when a moving pattern is either superimposed on (direction repulsion), or presented following adaptation to (DAE), another pattern moving in a different direction. Remarkable similarities in tuning characteristics suggest that common processes underlie the two illusions. What is not clear, howeve
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Meese, Tim S., and Mark A. Georgeson. "Spatial Filter Combination in Human Pattern Vision: Channel Interactions Revealed by Adaptation." Perception 25, no. 3 (1996): 255–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p250255.

Full text
Abstract:
Above threshold, two superimposed sinusoidal gratings of the same spatial frequency (eg 1 cycle deg−1), of equal moderate contrast (eg C1 = C2 = 6%), and with orientations of ±45°, usually look like a compound structure containing vertical and horizontal edges (ie a blurred checkerboard). These feature orientations are very different from the dominant filter orientations in a wavelet-type (eg simple-cell) transform of the stimulus, and so present a serious challenge to conventional models of orientation coding based on labelled linear filters. Previous experiments on perceived structure in sta
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Green, Patrick R., and Frank E. Pollick. "Recognising actions." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, no. 1 (2002): 106–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02330024.

Full text
Abstract:
The ability to recognise the actions of conspecifics from displays of biological motion is an essential perceptual capacity. Physiological and psychological evidence suggest that the visual processing of biological motion involves close interaction between the dorsal and ventral systems. Norman's strong emphasis on the functional differences between these systems may impede understanding of their interactions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Wu, Chaozhong, Wenhui Chu, Hui Zhang, and Türker Özkan. "Interactions between Driving Skills on Aggressive Driving: Study among Chinese Drivers." Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board 2672, no. 31 (2018): 10–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361198118755683.

Full text
Abstract:
Aggressive driving has attracted significant attention recently with the increase in related road traffic collisions occurring in China. This study aims to investigate the effect of driving skills on aggressive driving behaviors and traffic accidents to find implications for traffic safety improvement in China. A total of 735 Chinese drivers were recruited to complete a self-reported survey including demographic information, the translated Driver Skill Inventory (DSI), and Driver Aggression Indicator Scale (DAIS). Exploratory factor analysis was first conducted to investigate the factor struct
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Vulchanova, Mila, Valentin Vulchanov, Isabella Fritz, and Evelyn A. Milburn. "Language and perception: Introduction to the Special Issue “Speakers and Listeners in the Visual World”." Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science 3, no. 2 (2019): 103–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s41809-019-00047-z.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Language and perception are two central cognitive systems. Until relatively recently, however, the interaction between them has been examined only partially and not from an over-arching theoretical perspective. Yet it has become clear that linguistic and perceptual interactions are essential to understanding both typical and atypical human behaviour. In this editorial, we examine the link between language and perception across three domains. First, we present a brief review of work investigating the importance of perceptual features, particularly shape bias, when learning names for no
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Crommett, Lexi E., Deeksha Madala, and Jeffrey M. Yau. "Multisensory perceptual interactions between higher-order temporal frequency signals." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 148, no. 7 (2019): 1124–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000513.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Viviani, Paolo, and Natale Stucchi. "Biological movements look uniform: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18, no. 3 (1992): 603–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.603.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Murray, Scott O., Paul Schrater, and Daniel Kersten. "Perceptual grouping and the interactions between visual cortical areas." Neural Networks 17, no. 5-6 (2004): 695–705. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2004.03.010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Wexler, Bruce E., Gary Schwartz, Stephen Warrenburg, Mark Servis, and Irene Tarlatzis. "Effects of emotion on perceptual asymmetry: Interactions with personality." Neuropsychologia 24, no. 5 (1986): 699–710. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(86)90009-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!