To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Perceptual maps.

Journal articles on the topic 'Perceptual maps'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Perceptual maps.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Longo, Matthew R., and Marie Holmes. "Distorted perceptual face maps." Acta Psychologica 208 (July 2020): 103128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103128.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Lee, Anthony J. T., Fu-Chen Yang, Chao-Hung Chen, Chun-Sheng Wang, and Chih-Yuan Sun. "Mining perceptual maps from consumer reviews." Decision Support Systems 82 (February 2016): 12–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dss.2015.11.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Steenbergen, Peter, Jan R. Buitenweg, Jörg Trojan, and Peter H. Veltink. "Reproducibility of somatosensory spatial perceptual maps." Experimental Brain Research 224, no. 3 (2012): 417–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3321-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Blackwell, Robert E., Richard Harvey, Bastien Y. Queste, and Sophie Fielding. "Colour maps for fisheries acoustic echograms." ICES Journal of Marine Science 77, no. 2 (2019): 826–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsz242.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Echograms are used to visualize fisheries acoustic data, but choice of colour map has a significant effect on appearance. Quantitative echograms should use colour maps, which are colourful (have a perceived variety and intensity of colours), sequential (have monotonic lightness), and perceptually uniform (have consistency of perceived colour contrast over their range). We measure whether colour maps are colourful (M^(3)>0), sequential (rs=±1), and perceptually uniform (ρ = 1) using an approximately perceptually uniform colour space (CIELAB). Whilst all the fisheries acoustic co
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Myga, Kasia A., Klaudia B. Ambroziak, Luigi Tamè, Alessandro Farnè, and Matthew R. Longo. "Whole-hand perceptual maps of joint location." Experimental Brain Research 239, no. 4 (2021): 1235–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-021-06043-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Heady, Ronald B., and Jennifer L. Lucas. "PERMAP: An interactive program for making perceptual maps." Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 29, no. 3 (1997): 450–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03200598.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

MIYAZAWA, Kouki, Ayako SHIROSE, Reiko MAZUKA, and Hideaki KIKUCHI. "Self Organizing Maps as the Perceptual Acquisition Model." Journal of Japan Society for Fuzzy Theory and Intelligent Informatics 26, no. 1 (2014): 510–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.3156/jsoft.26.510.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Kovács, Ilona, and Bela Julesz. "Perceptual sensitivity maps within globally defined visual shapes." Nature 370, no. 6491 (1994): 644–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/370644a0.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Chown, Eric, Lashon B. Booker, and Stephen Kaplan. "Perception, action planning, and cognitive maps." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, no. 5 (2001): 882. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x01240102.

Full text
Abstract:
Perceptual learning mechanisms derived from Hebb's theory of cell assemblies can generate prototypic representations capable of extending the representational power of TEC (Theory of Event Coding) event codes. The extended capability includes categorization that accommodates “family resemblances” and problem solving that uses cognitive maps.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

McGraw, P. V., N. W. Roach, D. R. Badcock, and D. Whitaker. "Size-induced distortions in perceptual maps of visual space." Journal of Vision 12, no. 4 (2012): 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/12.4.8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Barik, Ram Chandra, and Suvamoy Changder. "Perceptual accessible image encryption scheme conjugating multiple chaotic maps." IET Image Processing 14, no. 11 (2020): 2457–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1049/iet-ipr.2019.0527.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Turi, Marco, and David Burr. "Spatiotopic perceptual maps in humans: evidence from motion adaptation." Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, no. 1740 (2012): 3091–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0637.

Full text
Abstract:
How our perceptual experience of the world remains stable and continuous despite the frequent repositioning eye movements remains very much a mystery. One possibility is that our brain actively constructs a spatiotopic representation of the world, which is anchored in external—or at least head-centred—coordinates. In this study, we show that the positional motion aftereffect (the change in apparent position after adaptation to motion) is spatially selective in external rather than retinal coordinates, whereas the classic motion aftereffect (the illusion of motion after prolonged inspection of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Longo, Matthew R. "Sex differences in perceptual hand maps: A meta-analysis." Acta Psychologica 196 (May 2019): 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.03.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Ruiz-Mayor, Antonio, Juan-Carlos Crespo, and Gracian Trivino. "Perceptual ambiguity maps for robot localizability with range perception." Expert Systems with Applications 85 (November 2017): 33–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eswa.2017.05.015.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Longo, Matthew R. "The effects of instrumental action on perceptual hand maps." Experimental Brain Research 236, no. 11 (2018): 3113–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5360-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Gower, John, Patrick J. F. Groenen, Michel Van de Velden, and Karen Vines. "Better perceptual maps: Introducing explanatory icons to facilitate interpretation." Food Quality and Preference 36 (September 2014): 61–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.01.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

IMAIZUMI, Akane, Yumiko ITO, and Norikazu SHIMIZU. "PERCEPTUAL MAPS FOR SHAPE AND COLOR DESIGN OF UNDERGROUND SPACE." Journal of Japan Society of Civil Engineers, Ser. F2 (Underground Space Research) 72, no. 1 (2016): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.2208/jscejusr.72.1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Gamba, P., and A. Mecocci. "Perceptual grouping for symbol chain tracking in digitized topographic maps." Pattern Recognition Letters 20, no. 4 (1999): 355–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0167-8655(99)00003-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Sacktor, T. C. "Signal and Sense: Local and Global Order in Perceptual Maps." Neurology 42, no. 5 (1992): 1131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/wnl.42.5.1131-e.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Bounds, Paulina. "Perceptual regions in Poland: An investigation of Poznań speech perceptions." Journal of Linguistic Geography 3, no. 1 (2015): 34–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/jlg.2015.1.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper, ideas from perceptual dialectology, linguistics of speech, and cognitive science are drawn upon to explain the perceptions of others’ speech. The perceptual map task, based on Preston’s “Draw-a-map methodology,” was collected from 215 respondents in Poland and transformed into result maps. The second step in the analysis of the perceptual maps was to collect all of the labels that were assigned to the perceived speech varieties around the country. Those two facets of the data show how the idea of connecting speech with a specific locality can be observed quantitatively. The resu
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Niccoli, Matteo. "Geophysical tutorial." Leading Edge 33, no. 8 (2014): 910–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1190/tle33080910.1.

Full text
Abstract:
This is the first of a two-part tutorial on color maps. My goal is to share the methods I developed ( Niccoli, 2012 ; Niccoli and Lynch, 2012 ; Niccoli, 2013 ) to evaluate default color maps and choose more perceptual alternatives (Part 1) and to make your own map based on perceptual principles (Part 2). For both parts, there will be an accompanying IPython Notebook with extended examples and analyses and open data. You can find the notebooks and code at github.com/seg/tutorials .
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Hidaka, Souta, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Elena Azañón, and Matthew R. Longo. "Tactile distance adaptation aftereffects do not transfer to perceptual hand maps." Acta Psychologica 208 (July 2020): 103090. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103090.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Kang, Jian, Francesco Aletta, Efstathios Margaritis, and Ming Yang. "A model for implementing soundscape maps in smart cities." Noise Mapping 5, no. 1 (2018): 46–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/noise-2018-0004.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Smart cities are required to engage with local communities by promoting a user-centred approach to deal with urban life issues and ultimately enhance people’s quality of life. Soundscape promotes a similar approach, based on individuals’ perception of acoustic environments. This paper aims to establish a model to implement soundscape maps for the monitoring and management of the acoustic environment and to demonstrate its feasibility. The final objective of the model is to generate visual maps related to perceptual attributes (e.g. ‘calm’, ‘pleasant’), starting from audio recordings o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Varga, Domonkos. "A Combined Full-Reference Image Quality Assessment Method Based on Convolutional Activation Maps." Algorithms 13, no. 12 (2020): 313. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/a13120313.

Full text
Abstract:
The goal of full-reference image quality assessment (FR-IQA) is to predict the perceptual quality of an image as perceived by human observers using its pristine (distortion free) reference counterpart. In this study, we explore a novel, combined approach which predicts the perceptual quality of a distorted image by compiling a feature vector from convolutional activation maps. More specifically, a reference-distorted image pair is run through a pretrained convolutional neural network and the activation maps are compared with a traditional image similarity metric. Subsequently, the resulting fe
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Rambo, William W., William C. Scott, and Antolin M. Llorente. "Selective Attrition of Experimental Subjects: A Methodological Study." Perceptual and Motor Skills 74, no. 1 (1992): 51–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.74.1.51.

Full text
Abstract:
Rather than being randomly drawn from a well-defined population, experimental samples are frequently formed as the result of a number of nonrandom influences, e.g., volunteering and subjects' attrition. The consequences of one such selective factor were examined in a study that required subjects to report for two experimental sessions. The task involved the multidimensional scaling of complex visual targets, drawings of the human face. The first of two sessions provided data reflecting intrarater consistency, interrater agreement, the orthogonality of perceptual dimensions, and the dimensional
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Grafton, Scott T., Joanna Salidis, and Daniel B. Willingham. "Motor Learning of Compatible and Incompatible Visuomotor Maps." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13, no. 2 (2001): 217–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892901564270.

Full text
Abstract:
Brain imaging studies demonstrate increasing activity in limb motor areas during early motor skill learning, consistent with functional reorganization occurring at the motor output level. Nevertheless, behavioral studies reveal that visually guided skills can also be learned with respect to target location or possibly eye movements. The current experiments examined motor learning under compatible and incompatible perceptual/motor conditions to identify brain areas involved in different perceptual-motor transformations. Subjects tracked a continuously moving target with a joystick-controlled cu
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Durgin, Frank H., and Zhi Li. "Perceptual experience as a bridge between the retina and a bicoded cognitive map." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36, no. 5 (2013): 549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x1300037x.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe bicoded cognitive maps described by Jeffery et al. are compared to metric perceptual representations. Systematic biases in perceptual experience of egocentric distance, height, and surface orientation may reflect information processing choices to retain information critical for immediate action (Durgin et al. 2010a). Different information processing goals (route planning vs. immediate action) require different metric information.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Tan, Steven K. S. "Differences between Experienced and Inexperienced Physical Education Teachers’ Augmented Feedback and Interactive Teaching Decisions." Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 15, no. 2 (1996): 151–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.15.2.151.

Full text
Abstract:
This study represents a descriptive analysis of feedback patterns and perceptual maps of experienced and inexperienced teachers. Five experienced elementary physical education teachers and 5 inexperienced teachers participated in the study. Data were collected by videotaping and audiotaping three lessons taught by each teacher. Transcripts of audiotapes were made for all verbal feedback administered by the teachers, and each unit of feedback was coded from the written transcripts using a multidimensional observation system. Following the second and third lessons, patterns in cue perception emp
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Ekhlassi, Amir, and Amirhosein Zahedi. "A Unique Method of Constructing Brand Perceptual Maps by the Text Mining of Multimedia Consumer Reviews." International Journal of Mobile Computing and Multimedia Communications 9, no. 3 (2018): 1–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijmcmc.2018070101.

Full text
Abstract:
Brand perceptual mapping is a visual technique, it displays how a brand is positioned in the mind of customers, as well as in relation to the competitors. With the rapid growth of e-commerce and the abundance of online consumer-generated content, there is no need for marketers to go through market research in order to understand consumers' opinions. Therefore, in this study, the authors propose a unique method which allows the building of a perceptual map automatically by mining consumer opinions from in particular online product reviews. The authors employ opinion mining techniques to extract
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Ren, Hong, Chunyu Zhang, and Ningning Zhang. "Research on EEG-based Graphic User Interface Kansei Design Evaluation." E3S Web of Conferences 179 (2020): 02103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/e3sconf/202017902103.

Full text
Abstract:
Graphical user interface (GUI) is designed as the interaction medium between the user and the interface, and the perceptual experience of GUI design has been paid more and more attention by users. Based on the theory of perceptual engineering (KE), two groups of different visual style interfaces were taken as an example to record the EEG data when users watched two groups of visual interfaces, in order to explore the user’s perceptual imagery and perceptual experience for the visual interface. It aims to meet the user’s perceptual needs and provide an effective evaluation method and design bas
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Lloyd, Robert Earl. "Attention on Maps." Cartographic Perspectives, no. 52 (September 1, 2005): 28–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.14714/cp52.377.

Full text
Abstract:
Wilson’s (1998) notion of consilience among disciplines should be a goal for cartographers. Consilience requires important facts and fact-based theories to apply across disciplines. This paper reviews research on visual attention as an example of a topic shared by information science disciplines. Attention is considered as a competition between neural processes that allow information to be selected and emphasized for perceptual processing. Visual attention has been modeled as a spotlight, zoom lens, gradient, and multiple spotlights. It is argued that visual attention can impact multiple map r
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Santos, Elisa Norberto Ferreira, Gilberto Rodrigues Liska, and Marcelo Angelo Cirillo. "Methodology for constructing perceptual maps incorporating measuring error in sensory acceptance tests." Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods 16, no. 1 (2017): 308–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.22237/jmasm/1493597820.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Mazer, J. A., and J. L. Gallant. "Evidence for perceptual saliency maps in area V4 during freeviewing visual search." Journal of Vision 2, no. 7 (2010): 727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/2.7.727.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Ross, W. D., S. Grossberg, and E. Mingolla. "Visual cortical mechanisms of perceptual grouping: interacting layers, networks, columns, and maps." Neural Networks 13, no. 6 (2000): 571–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0893-6080(00)00040-x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Tversky, Barbara, and Diane J. Schiano. "Perceptual and conceptual factors in distortions in memory for graphs and maps." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118, no. 4 (1989): 387–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.4.387.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Schmalensee, Richard, and Jacques-Frangois Thisse. "Perceptual maps and the optimal location of new products: An integrative essay." International Journal of Research in Marketing 5, no. 4 (1988): 225–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8116(88)90003-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

MacKay, David B., and Cornelia Dröge. "Extentions of probabilistic perceptual maps with implications for competitive positioning and choice." International Journal of Research in Marketing 7, no. 4 (1990): 265–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0167-8116(90)90004-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Baran, Tamer. "Positions of Mobile Phone Brands on University Students' Mind." International Journal of Sustainable Economies Management 5, no. 1 (2016): 18–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.4018/ijsem.2016010102.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this study is determine the perception of Turkish university students of mobile phone brands. To achieve this, brand, brand positioning concepts and brand positioning strategies were mentioned. 417 questionnaires were carried out from university students. The results obtained from the questionnaires have been analysed by multi-dimensional scaling method. Then findings have been explained by using perceptual maps. The results have revealed that Nokia is the highest perception level among university students. Nokia is followed by Samsung, Sony Ericsson, LG and General Mobile (GM) resp
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Shugan, Steven M. "Estimating Brand Positioning Maps Using Supermarket Scanning Data." Journal of Marketing Research 24, no. 1 (1987): 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002224378702400101.

Full text
Abstract:
Hauser and Shugan's DEFENDER model is estimated with data obtained from supermarket scanning devices. As a result of that estimation, per dollar multidimensional brand maps are obtained from aggregate sales and price time series data. The estimation procedure employs least squares regression analysis. The maps enable a researcher or brand manager to (1) derive a map from observed choice behavior, (2) study the direct effect of both a brand's price and a brand's position on that brand's sales, (3) obtain statistical measures of accuracy for the estimated brand coordinates, and (4) integrate the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Wascher, Edmund, and C. Beste. "Spatial Representations as an Emergent Feature of Perceptual Processing." Journal of Psychophysiology 24, no. 3 (2010): 161–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000007.

Full text
Abstract:
Spatial selection of relevant information has been proposed to reflect an emergent feature of stimulus processing within an integrated network of perceptual areas. Stimulus-based and intention-based sources of information might converge in a common stage when spatial maps are generated. This approach appears to be inconsistent with the assumption of distinct mechanisms for stimulus-driven and top-down controlled attention. In two experiments, the common ground of stimulus-driven and intention-based attention was tested by means of event-related potentials (ERPs) in the human EEG. In both exper
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Iacobucci, Dawn, and Doug Grisaffe. "Perceptual maps via enhanced correspondence analysis: representing confidence regions to clarify brand positions." Journal of Marketing Analytics 6, no. 3 (2018): 72–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/s41270-018-0037-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Gao, Yu, Xiaoyu Xiu, Jie Liang, and Weisi Lin. "Fast synthesized and predicted just noticeable distortion maps for perceptual multiview video coding." Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation 24, no. 6 (2013): 700–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvcir.2012.04.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

DeAngelis, Gregory C., and William T. Newsome. "Perceptual “Read-Out” of Conjoined Direction and Disparity Maps in Extrastriate Area MT." PLoS Biology 2, no. 3 (2004): e77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0020077.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Hopfer, Helene, Elliott H. McDowell, Line E. Nielsen, and John E. Hayes. "Preferred beer styles influence both perceptual maps and semantic descriptions of dry hops." Food Quality and Preference 94 (December 2021): 104337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104337.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Wandell, Brian A., Alyssa A. Brewer, and Robert F. Dougherty. "Visual field map clusters in human cortex." Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360, no. 1456 (2005): 693–707. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1628.

Full text
Abstract:
We describe the location and general properties of nine human visual field maps. The cortical location of each map, as well as many examples of the eccentricity and angular representations within these maps, are shown in a series of images that summarize a large set of functional MRI data. The organization and properties of these maps are compared and contrasted with descriptions by other investigators. We hypothesize that the human visual field maps are arranged in several clusters, each comprising a group of maps that share a common foveal representation and semicircular eccentricity map. Th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Bermúdez, José Luis. "Cognitive impenetrability, phenomenology, and nonconceptual content." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22, no. 3 (1999): 367–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x99232025.

Full text
Abstract:
This commentary discusses Pylyshyn's model of perceptual processing in the light of the philosophical distinction between the conceptual and the nonconceptual content of perception. Pylyshyn's processing distinction maps onto an important distinction in the phenomenology of visual perception.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Kuchinke, Lars, Julian Keil, Dennis Edler, Anne-Kathrin Bestgen, and Frank Dickmann. "Grid overlays reduce bias in mental representations of topographic maps." Abstracts of the ICA 1 (July 15, 2019): 1. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/ica-abs-1-196-2019.

Full text
Abstract:
<p><strong>Abstract.</strong> Reading spatial information from topographic maps to form mental representations that guide spatial orientation and navigation is a rather complex cognitive process. Perceptual and knowledge-driven processes interact to support the map reader in building these mental representations. The resulting cognitive maps are not one-to-one mappings of the spatial information and known to be distorted systematically. It is assumed that spatial information is hierarchically organized in these mental models. We are interested in how map design based on cogni
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Bauer, H. U., and R. Der. "Controlling the Magnification Factor of Self-Organizing Feature Maps." Neural Computation 8, no. 4 (1996): 757–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/neco.1996.8.4.757.

Full text
Abstract:
The magnification exponents μ occurring in adaptive map formation algorithms like Kohonen's self-organizing feature map deviate for the information theoretically optimal value μ = 1 as well as from the values that optimize, e.g., the mean square distortion error (μ = 1/3 for one-dimensional maps). At the same time, models for categorical perception such as the "perceptual magnet" effect, which are based on topographic maps, require negative magnification exponents μ < 0. We present an extension of the self-organizing feature map algorithm, which utilizes adaptive local learning step sizes t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Visentin, Marco, Mariachiara Colucci, and Gian Luca Marzocchi. "Brand Measurement Scales and Underlying Cognitive Dimensions." International Journal of Market Research 55, no. 1 (2013): 43–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.2501/ijmr-2013-006.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this exploratory research is to compare a well-known scale, the Aaker brand personality scale, with an empirical scale based on individuals' relevant attributes, in order to analyse why they can lead to similar brand positioning maps. We provide empirical evidence of how a bias can overwrite the ability of a measurement scale to actually measure its underlying construct. In order to do so, we first find that the two sets of attributes – one derived from the brand personality scale, the other reflecting attributes obtained through a focus group – span common cognitive representations
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Elmore, Janelle R., and Hildegarde Heymann. "Perceptual maps of photographs of carbonated beverages created by traditional and free-choice profiling." Food Quality and Preference 10, no. 3 (1999): 219–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0950-3293(99)00028-2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!