Dissertations / Theses on the topic 'Physiologische Psychologie'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 30 dissertations / theses for your research on the topic 'Physiologische Psychologie.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse dissertations / theses on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Aregger, Lisa. "Der Placeboeffekt: Illusion oder Wirklichkeit? : psychische und physiologische Wirkmechanismen /." Zürich : Hochschule für Angewandte Psychologie, 2007. http://www.zhaw.ch/fileadmin/user_upload/psychologie/Downloads/Bibliothek/Arbeiten/D/d1968.pdf.
Full textJentzsch, Ines. "Sequenzeffekte in Verhalten und hirnelektrischen Potentialen : Komponentenseparation, Dipolquellenanalyse und Modellierung /." Aachen : Shaker, 2001. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=009259613&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.
Full textAlbrecht, Ulrike. "Reizreaktion und Verlangen bei pathologischen Glücksspielern: psychologische und physiologische Parameter." Berlin Logos-Verl, 2006. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?id=2820882&prov=M&dok_var=1&dok_ext=htm.
Full textAuracher, Jan. ""... wie auf den allmächtigen Schlag einer magischen Rute" psychophysiologische Messungen zur Textwirkung." Baden-Baden Dt. Wiss.-Verl, 2006. http://deposit.d-nb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?id=2953848&prov=M&dok_var=1&dok_ext=htm.
Full textRisius, Uda-Mareke [Verfasser]. "Memory accuracy : A 7-Tesla fMRI approach to memory accuracy ; retrieval, monitoring and control processes / Uda-Mareke Risius. Fakultät für Psychologie und Sportwissenschaft - Arbeitseinheit 14 - Physiologische Psychologie." Bielefeld : Universitätsbibliothek Bielefeld, Hochschulschriften, 2011. http://d-nb.info/1015183646/34.
Full textThanhäuser, Jürgen. "Zum Heimfinden nach einer simulierten Bewegung im Raum : eine Untersuchung des visuell-räumlichen Speichers im Arbeitsgedächtnis unter Verwendung psychophysiologischer Indikatoren /." Aachen : Shaker, 2000. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=009144047&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.
Full textGeier, Jens-Stefan. "Psychophysiologische Konzepte und emotionstheoretische Modelle als Grundlage anxioalgolytischer Musikwirkung /." Osnabrück : Univ.-Verl. Rasch, 2001. http://bvbr.bib-bvb.de:8991/F?func=service&doc_library=BVB01&doc_number=009259331&line_number=0001&func_code=DB_RECORDS&service_type=MEDIA.
Full textStrunk, Anne [Verfasser], P. [Gutachter] Joraschky, and H. [Gutachter] Berth. "Die subjektive und physiologische Stressreaktion von Patienten mit einer Sozialen Angststörung. Der Einfluss cyberball-induzierter Ausgrenzung / Anne Strunk ; Gutachter: P. Joraschky, H. Berth." Dresden : Technische Universität Dresden, 2019. http://d-nb.info/1227196571/34.
Full textDoblinger, Susanne [Verfasser], and Sabine [Akademischer Betreuer] Walper. "Kinder aus psychosozial belasteten Familien beim Übergang vom Kindergarten in die Schule : psychische Gesundheit, physiologische Stressbelastung und Übergangsbewältigung / Susanne Doblinger ; Betreuer: Sabine Walper." München : Universitätsbibliothek der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, 2018. http://d-nb.info/1204827605/34.
Full textMay, Jana. "Bewertung individueller Voraussetzungen zur Bewältigung wechselnder Arbeitsanforderungen anhand psychologischer und neuropsychologischer Parameter." Phd thesis, Universität Potsdam, 2004. http://opus.kobv.de/ubp/volltexte/2005/396/.
Full textNach Rubinstein et al. (2001) setzt sich die Reaktionszeit aus der Dauer seriell angeordneter Verarbeitungsstufen zusammen. Im Stufenmodell der exekutiven Kontrolle von Rubinstein et al. (2001) sind Prozesse der ausführenden Kontrolle nur an Wechseltrials beteiligt und können getrennt von den Aufgabenprozessen ablaufen. Mittels der Informationen zu den Reaktionszeiten ist es jedoch nicht möglich zu klären, auf welche kognitiven Verarbeitungsprozesse Reaktionszeitunterschiede unter den jeweiligen experimentellen Bedingungen zurückzuführen sind. Zur Analyse der kognitiven Prozesse wurden in dieser Untersuchung die CNV und P300 herangezogen. Es wurden zwei Altersgruppen (20-30 Jährige und 49-61 Jährige) untersucht. Den Probanden wurden Ziffern präsentiert, die entweder nach dem numerischen Wert oder der Schriftgröße mit dem Hinweisreiz, der Zahl 5, verglichen werden sollten. Die Stimuli wurden nach dem Alternating-Runs-Paradigma dargeboten (Rogers und Monsell, 1995).
Erwartungsgemäß gab es Reaktionszeitunterschiede zwischen alt und jung mit längeren Reaktionszeiten für die älteren Probanden. Altersunterschiede in den Fehlerraten ließen sich nicht nachweisen. Möglicherweise erfolgte die Reaktionsauswahl bei den Älteren überlegter aus als bei den Jüngeren. Dies spiegelte sich in längeren aber fehlerfreien Reaktionen wider. Vermutlich bereiteten jedoch alle Probanden in dem Intervall zwischen Cue und Stimulus das jeweilige Aufgabenset komplett vor. Das könnte auch erklären, warum es bei einem Aufgabenwechsel nicht zu einem Anstieg der Reaktionszeit und der Fehlerrate kam. Entgegen der Erwartung zeigten sich keine Wechselkosten. Teilweise wurden inverse Wechselkosten nachgewiesen. In Bezug auf die Wechselkosten konnte das Stufenmodell der exekutiven Kontrolle (Rubinstein et al., 2001) nicht bestätigt werden. Der explizite Hinweisreiz scheint allerdings Einfluss auf die Wechselkosten zu haben. Verschiedene Erklärungsansätze werden diskutiert.
Die Contingent Negative Variation ist wie erwartet vor einem Aufgabenwechsel größer als vor einer Aufgabenwiederholung. Durch den Hinweisreiz ist eine erhöhte Kapazität vorhanden. Entsprechend den Ergebnissen der CNV kann davon ausgegangen werden, dass ältere Erwachsene stärker von der Vorinformation zu profitieren scheinen als jüngere Erwachsene. Die älteren Erwachsenen beginnen im Gegensatz zu den jüngeren Erwachsenen offenbar eher mit der Vorbereitung. Zeitdruck und Aufgabenwechsel lösen eine stärkere P300 aus. Demzufolge scheinen Zeitdruck und Aufgabenwechsel einen erhöhten Kapazitätsbedarf zu erfordern. Im Sinne des Stufenmodells der exekutiven Kontrolle von Rubinstein et al. (2001) führt die Zielverschiebung bei einem Aufgabenwechsel zu einer größeren P300. Die Resultate der hier dargestellten Untersuchungen verdeutlichen, dass ältere Erwachsene einen höheren zeitlichen Aufwand in den Stufen der einzelnen exekutiven Prozesse benötigen. Dies spricht für die Hypothese der selektiven Verlangsamung. Ältere kompensieren dies durch einen höheren Aufwand in der Vorbereitung, was auf elektrokortikaler Ebene nachweisbar ist, sind aber nicht in der Lage, dies in den Reaktionszeiten umzusetzen.
Die Ergebnisse dieser Untersuchung unterstützen die vereinfachte Annahme von Rubinstein et al. (2001), nach dem die Teilprozesse der Reaktionszeit seriell verarbeitet werden können. Die Resultate lassen allerdings den Schluss zu, dass die Wechselkosten im Hinblick auf die Reaktionszeiten nicht der geeignete Parameter für die Messung der exekutiven Kontrolle sind.
Die vorgeschlagene Modifikation des Modells von Rubinstein et al. (2001) in der Vorbereitung auf eine Aufgabe gilt es in weiteren Untersuchungen zu bestätigen und die Möglichkeit der Anwendung auf alle Aspekte der exekutiven Kontrollprozesse zu prüfen.
The aim of this study was it to examine cognitive processes not only on the basis achievement parameters by means of the alternating runs paradigm, but additionally by electricalphysiological parameters. Parameters of event-correlated brain potentials (EKP) were also used to estimate age-related changes in tasks of response time.
According to Rubinstein et al. (2001) the response time consists of the duration of serially arranged processing levels. In Rubinstein's et al. (2001) stage model of the executive control processes of implementing control are involved only in switch trials and can run separately from the task processes. The information from response times do not aloud to define what cognitive processing processes are responsible for response time differences in respective to the experimental conditions. In this study the contingent negative variation (CNV) and P300 were used for the analysis of the cognitive processes.
Two age groups (20 to 30, and 49 to 61 years old) were included in the study. Numbers were presented, which should be compared to the cue number 5 either to the numeric value or character size. The stimuli were represented after the alternating runs paradigm (Rogers & Monsell, 1995).
As expected there were response time differences between old and young subjects with longer response times for the older ones. Age differences in the error rates could not be proven. It is possible that the reaction selection in older ones took place with more consideration than in the younger ones. This is reflected in longer but error free reactions. Probably all subjects prepared in the interval the respective task set between cue and stimulus completely. This could also explain, why with a task switching no rise of the response time and the error rate was noticed. Against expectation no switch costs showed up. Inverse switch costs were partly proven. Regarding the switch costs the stage model of the executive control (Rubinstein et al., 2001) could not be confirmed. The explicit cue however seems to have influence on the switch costs. Different explanations are discussed.
The contingent negative variation is higher before a task switching than before a repetition of task. By the cue an increased capacity is presented. According to the results of the CNV it can be assumed that older adults seem to profit more strongly than younger adults from the advance information. Obviously the older adults begin earlier with the preparation than the younger ones. Time pressure and task switching release a stronger P300. Therefore time pressure and task switching seem to require an increased capacity need. According to the stage model of the executive control (Rubinstein et al., (2001) the goal shift with goes along with the task switching leads to a higher P300.
The results of the study represented here clarify, that older adults need a more time in the stages of the individual executives of processes. This underlines the hypothesis of the selective slowing down. Older ones compensate this by more effort to preparation, which can be proven on the electrocortical level. The are not able to show this in the the response times, though. The results of this study support the simplified acceptance of Rubinstein et al. (2001), after which the subprocesses of the response time can be processed serially.
The results permit the conclusion that the switch costs regarding the response times are not the adaquate parameter for the measurement of the executive control.
The suggested modification of the model of Rubinstein et al. (2001) in the preparation for a task it applies to be confirmed in further investigations and the possibility of application to all aspects of the executives has to be tested.
Biester, Lars. "Cooperative automation in automobiles." Doctoral thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftliche Fakultät II, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.18452/15935.
Full textThe aim of this dissertation is to systematically develop a continuative concept of driver-automobile cooperation, to evaluate its suitability on the basis of empirical data, and to value its provable potential in relation to existing approaches.Assumptions and premises regarding the human-machine interaction constitute the starting point of this work. The decisive altercation and notional differentiation of cooperation are explained in just this context, leading logically to a definitional demarcation of existing approaches, the demand of a specific role understanding of the interaction as well as the derivation of conceptual basic conditions. The structural and procedural characteristics of this specific interaction are then elaborated upon and used to identify the general attributes of cooperation between driver and automobile. In the following, such indicators are derived by which the implied profit as a result of cooperation between driver and automobile can be controlled and valued. Within the framework of several preliminary investigations, those driving situations were identified that would profit most from a cooperative interaction between driver and automobile. As a result, the two driving scenarios "Overtaking on Highways" and "Turning Left on Urban and Country Roads with Oncoming Traffic" were utilized in the experiments. Both single scenarios have been compared in independent experiments with regard to alternative system variants. The prove of specific hypotheses was embedded in the prototypical surroundings of a driving simulator. Finally, the possibility of establishing and embedding this interaction concept into the overall socio-technical context will be presented, and future perspectives will be discussed.
Bornemann, Boris. "Body awareness, voluntary physiological regulation, and their modulation by contemplative mental training." Doctoral thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.18452/18079.
Full textIn this dissertation, I investigate interactions between mental and bodily processes, specifically by studying the influence of contemplative mental training (CMT) on interoception (inner body sensing) and physiological regulation. In a large-scale mental training study (n = 332, training durations 3–9 months), the ReSource Project, I find that CMT increases interoceptive accuracy in the training cohorts, but not in a retest control cohort. These increases in interoceptive accuracy co-occur with and predict improvements in emotional awareness. In line with these objective data on interoception, participants self-report training-related benefits on multiple dimensions of body awareness. The strongest changes occur in the ability to sustain attention to body sensations and the use of this ability to identify and regulate emotions. I also introduce a novel biofeedback task that measures the ability to voluntarily upregulate high frequency heart rate variability (HF-HRV), indicative of voluntary parasympathetic control. Cross-sectional data of the ReSource Project show that individual differences in voluntary parasympathetic control are related to the oxytocin receptor gene rs53576 polymorphism and correlate to individual differences in altruistically motivated behavior. Furthermore, CMT improves various aspects of voluntary HF-HRV regulation, with modulation of these improvements by rs53576 genotype. An additional investigation in cross-sectional data shows that subjective retrospective reports of an emotionally arousing experience partially mirror the objectively measured bodily arousal during the actual experience. Individual differences in this mind–body coherence are related to individual differences in interoceptive accuracy. Together, these studies highlight the tight interplay between physiological and mental processes and show how CMT improves inner body sensing and voluntary physiological regulation.
Kolb, Diana [Verfasser]. "Directed Expression: Eine neue Therapieform für Kinder mit physiologischen und psychologischen Erkrankungen / Diana Kolb. Fakultät für Erziehungswissenschaft." Bielefeld : Universitätsbibliothek Bielefeld, Hochschulschriften, 2012. http://d-nb.info/1019406801/34.
Full textHelbig, Susanne [Verfasser]. "Geschlechterunterschiede in der physiologischen und psychologischen Stressreaktion im experimentellen und naturalistischen Setting unter Berücksichtigung der Faktoren Schlaf und kognitive Bewertung / Susanne Helbig." Bonn : Universitäts- und Landesbibliothek Bonn, 2019. http://d-nb.info/1185486275/34.
Full textTrilla, Gros Irene. "Situated social cognition." Doctoral thesis, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, 2021. http://edoc.hu-berlin.de/18452/24079.
Full textThis dissertation presents four studies that investigated how altercentric (mimicry) and egocentric (self-projection) processes of social cognition are regulated according to the social context and personal dispositions. Study 1 showed that the tendency to mimic others’ happy facial expressions depends on the reward value associated with the observed agent. However, the effects of reward were not in the hypothesised direction, nor could we detect an influence of oxytocin treatment, a hormone involved in the neurobiology of social adaptation. Study 2 could not detect a general enhancement of the tendency to automatically imitate others’ hand actions following direct gaze compared to averted gaze, in contrast to previous studies. However, we could identify dispositional factors (e.g., autistic traits) that might underlie different mimicry responses to gaze cues. Combining brief emotion induction blocks with psychophysical measures of emotion perception, Study 3 showed that facial emotional expressions tend to be judged as happier when individuals feel happy than when they feel sad. Emotional egocentric biases were replicated in Study 4. But contrary to our predictions, we did not find stronger egocentric biases when participants judged emotional facial expressions of similar compared to dissimilar others. Across all studies, we found evidence supporting the contextual nature of social cognition. However, we could not replicate some of the phenomena reported in the literature. These results highlight the need to systematically re-evaluate the robustness and generalizability of prior findings.
Busching, Robert. "Affektive und kognitive Desensibilisierung als Konsequenz von Mediengewaltkonsum : eine experimentelle Untersuchung auf Basis lerntheoretischer Überlegungen." Phd thesis, Universität Potsdam, 2014. http://opus.kobv.de/ubp/volltexte/2014/7136/.
Full textUsers of violent media often state that they consume fictional violent media and claim that this does not influence their behavior outside of the media context. Media users reason that they can easily distinguish between things learned in a fictional context and things learned in reality. In contrast to these claims, meta-analyses report medium-sized effects between media violence consumption and aggressive behavior. These effects can only be explained if media users also apply learning experiences from the media outside the media context. One of the processes linking learning experiences in the media context to behavior in the real word is desensitization, which is often defined as a reduction of negative affect towards violence. To investigate the process of desensitization, four experiments were conducted. The first hypothesis investigated in these experiments was that persons who have a higher level of violent media exposure show less negative affect towards depictions of real violence compared to persons with a lower level. However, it was assumed that this reduction in reaction would be limited to depictions of real violence and would not be found in response to depictions of nonviolent pictures electing negative affect. The second hypothesis referred to the role of affect experienced during exposure to media depictions of violence, assuming that especially people who enjoy the violence while consuming the media show less negative affect towards depictions of real violence. The final hypothesis addressed cognitive desensitization effects and predicted that media violence use leads to transfer effects from responses to violent cues to initially neutral stimuli The first experiment (N = 57) examined to what extent the habitual use of media violence predicts the self-reported affect (Valence and Arousal) towards depictions of real violence and nonviolent pictures eliciting negative affect. Habitual media violence exposure predicted less negative valence and less arousal toward violent pictures and negative nonviolent pictures. The second experiment (N = 103) also examined the relationship between habitual media violence exposure and the affective reactions towards images of real violence as well as negative, nonviolent scenes. Additionally, affect while watching violent media was added as a second predictor. The affect toward the images was also obtained by psychophysical measures (Valence: activity of C. Supercilii; Activation: skin conductance). As in experiment 1, habitual media violence exposure predicted less self-reported negative valence and less self-reported arousal for the violent pictures and the negative nonviolent pictures. The physiological measures replicated this result. However, a different pattern emerged for the enjoyment of violent media. People who enjoyed violent media more showed a reduction of affective responsivity toward violence on all four different measures. Additionally, for three of the four measures (skin conductance, self-reported negative valence and the activity on the C. Supercilii) this relationship was limited to the violent pictures, with no or little effect on the negative, but nonviolent pictures. A third experiment (N = 73) studied affect while participants played a violent computer game. A game was specially programmed in which specific actions could be linked to the activity of the C. Supercilii, an indicator for negative affect. The analysis of the C. Supercilii showed that repeatedly performing aggressive actions during the game led to a decline in negative affect accompanying the aggressive actions. The negative affect during aggressive actions in turn predicted less negative affective responses to violent images of real-life violence but not to nonviolent, negative images. The fourth experiment (N=77) showed cognitive desensitization effects, which involved the formation of links between neutral and aggressive cognitions. Participants played a first-person shooter game either on a ship or a city level. The relationship between neutral constructs (ship/city) and aggressive cognitions was measured using a lexical decision task. Playing the game in a ship/city level led to shorter reaction time for aggressive words following ship/city primes respectively. This indicated that the neutral concepts introduced in the game had become associated with aggressive cognitions. The findings of these four experiments are discussed in terms of a learning theory approach to conceptualizing desensitization.
Heimes, Thomas. "Psychophysische Aktivation in einem "Active"-und einem "Passive-Coping"-Paradigma." 1987. http://catalog.hathitrust.org/api/volumes/oclc/17914758.html.
Full textKenntner-Mabiala, Ramona. "Affektive und aufmerksamkeitsbasierte Modulation somatosensorisch evozierter Potentiale : Die Wirkung von Emotionen und Aufmerksamkeit auf die Schmerzverarbeitung." Doctoral thesis, 2006. https://nbn-resolving.org/urn:nbn:de:bvb:20-opus-22211.
Full textBackground: The starting point for the present dissertation were correlative studies indicating that negative emotional states increase the frequency and magnitude of pain experience. The motivational priming hypothesis offers a theoretical framework to explain the effects of emotion on pain. The predictions of this theory have been extensively investigated for acoustic startle stimuli, but up to now, an evaluation of the motivational priming hypothesis for pain perception and processing is lacking. Furthermore, the role of attention for affective pain modulation is still a matter of debate. Experiment 1. The aim of the first experiment was to evaluate the motivational priming hypothesis for pain perception and processing. Methods: 30 participants viewed positive, neutral and negative pictures, while painful and nonpainful electrical stimuli were applied. Intensity ratings and somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) in response to the electrical stimuli were recorded. Results: Picture valence affected pain ratings and N150 amplitudes elicited by painful stimuli with lowest amplitudes for positive pictures and highest amplitudes for negative pictures. The P260 elicited by painful and non-painful stimuli was modulated by arousal with reduced amplitudes with arousing (positive or negative) compared to neutral pictures. Interpretation: N150 amplitudes varying with picture valence seem to reflect an affective modulation of pain perception while P260 amplitudes varying with picture arousal rather reflect non pain-specific attentional processes. Experiment 2: The aim of the second experiment was to disentangle the effects of attention and emotion on pain perception and to investigate if emotion and attention affect pain perception via the same or at least partially different neural mechanisms. Methods: Painful and nonpainful electrical stimuli were applied while 30 participants viewed positive, neutral and negative pictures. Attentional manipulation was realized by presenting a prompt before picture onset to focus attention on the pictures or on the intensity or on the unpleasantness of the electrical stimuli. Pain assessment included sensory and affective pain ratings and SEPs. The neural sources of N150 and P260 SEP components were analyzed using LORETA source localization. Results: Attention modulated sensory pain ratings with highest ratings when attention was focused on the stimulus intensity. Affect influenced sensory and affective pain ratings with higher ratings during negative than during positive pictures. The amplitudes of the N150 and the P260 were modulated by picture valence and picture arousal, respectively. Furthermore, P260 amplitudes were modulated by attention with highest amplitudes when attention was focused on the intensity of the stimuli. The LORETA analysis revealed different neural generators for the N150 and the P260 for the contrast painful vs. nonpainful stimuli: For the N150, significantly higher brain electrical activity was found in the ACC and in the precuneus. The P260 was localized in the superior and in the medial frontal gyrus as well as in the ACC. Discussion: Both experiments support and extend the motivational priming hypothesis to the perception and processing of painful stimuli as indicated by an affective modulation of pain ratings and of N150 amplitudes. Furthermore, the findings of the second experiment suggest that the effects of emotion and attention on pain are independent: Attentional manipulations affect only sensory pain ratings whereas affect induction primarily modulates affective pain ratings. In addition, emotional and attentional effects on pain seem to invoke at least partially different neural modulatory circuits: The LORETA analysis revealed different neural sources for the N150 and the P260 components, and emotion and attention have distinct effects on these components: Whereas the modulation of N150 amplitudes reflects an affective pain modulation, the modulation of P260 amplitudes is due to attentional processes
Lange, Léonie Frederica [Verfasser]. "Mentale Rotation bei Kindern : Trainingseffekte und physiologische Grundlagen / vorgelegt von Léonie Frederica Lange." 2009. http://d-nb.info/995643806/34.
Full textJeschonek, Susanna [Verfasser]. "Physiologische und behaviorale Korrelate der Kategorisierung in der frühen Kindheit : Herzrate, ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale, Blickzeiten / vorgelegt von Susanna Jeschonek." 2010. http://d-nb.info/1007467983/34.
Full textOrtner-Willnecker, Karin. "Elektrophysiologische Indikatoren für spezifische Prozesse der Vorbereitung." 1996. http://books.google.com/books?id=D-xqAAAAMAAJ.
Full textAlbrecht, Björn. "Kognitive Kontrolle bei Aufmerksamkeits Defizit / Hyperaktivitäts Störung." Thesis, 2009. http://hdl.handle.net/11858/00-1735-0000-0006-ADD0-7.
Full textAndor, Tanja [Verfasser]. "Wahrnehmung phasischer physiologischer Aktivierung und Metakognitionen bei generalisierter Angststörung / vorgelegt von Tanja Andor." 2007. http://d-nb.info/985750790/34.
Full textHinrichs, Sophie [Verfasser]. "Experimentalpsychologische Untersuchungen zu Navigationsstrategien unter Berücksichtigung physiologischer und endokrinologischer Variablen / vorgelegt von Sophie Hinrichs." 2009. http://d-nb.info/993430503/34.
Full textHinrichs, Sophie. "Experimentalpsychologische Untersuchungen zu Navigationsstrategien unter Berücksichtigung endokrinologischer und physiologischer Variablen." Doctoral thesis, 2008. http://hdl.handle.net/11858/00-1735-0000-0006-AD2C-C.
Full textRill, Hans-Georg [Verfasser]. "Forensische Psychophysiologie : ein Beitrag zu den psychologischen und physiologischen Grundlagen neuerer Ansätze der "Lügendetektion" / von Hans-Georg Rill." 2001. http://d-nb.info/962727717/34.
Full textThöns, Miriam [Verfasser]. "Die Beziehung emotionaler und physiologischer Reaktionen zu Gedächtnisleistungen : ein Assoziationsexperiment zu grundlegenden Annahmen der psychoanalytischen Theorie und umfassenden alternativen Erklärungsansätzen / vorgelegt von Miriam Thöns." 2002. http://d-nb.info/967968119/34.
Full textSchwarzer, Marco. "Physiologische Untersuchungen zur Regulation des Aminosäure-Stoffwechsels von Bacillus licheniformis DSM13." Doctoral thesis, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/11858/00-1735-0000-0006-AE04-B.
Full textNiederl, Tanja [Verfasser]. "Untersuchungen zu kumulativen psychischen und physiologischen Effekten des fliegenden Personals auf der Kurzstrecke : am Beispiel des Flugbetriebes der Boeing-737-Flotte der Deutschen Lufthansa AG / vorgelegt von Tanja Niederl." 2008. http://d-nb.info/988337304/34.
Full textSchumacher, Evelyn. "The impact of ants on the aboveground and belowground ecological network - field studies in a grassland and experiments with microcosms." Thesis, 2010. http://hdl.handle.net/11858/00-1735-0000-0006-ADF4-8.
Full text