Academic literature on the topic 'Primates Behavior'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Primates Behavior.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Hunt, Gavin R., and Russell D. Gray. "Genetic assimilation of behaviour does not eliminate learning and innovation." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30, no. 4 (August 2007): 412–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x07002439.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractRamsey et al. attempt to clarify methodological issues for identifying innovative behaviour. Their effort is seriously weakened by an underlying presumption that the behavior of primates is generally learned and that of non-primates is generally “innate.” This presumption is based on a poor grasp of the non-primate literature and a flawed understanding of how learned behaviour is genetically assimilated.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Kamilar, Jason M., and Lydia Beaudrot. "Effects of Environmental Stress on Primate Populations." Annual Review of Anthropology 47, no. 1 (October 21, 2018): 417–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-102317-045949.

Full text
Abstract:
Environmental stress on primate populations can take many forms. Abiotic factors, such as temperature and precipitation, may directly influence the behavior of primates owing to physiological demands of thermoregulation or through indirect influences on vegetation that primates rely on for food. These effects can also scale up to the macro scale, impacting primate distributions and evolution. Primates also encounter stress during interactions within and between species (i.e., biotic interactions). For example, selective pressure from male-perpetrated infanticide can drive the development of female counterstrategies and can impact life-history traits. Predation on primates can modify group size, ranging behavior, and habitat use. Finally, humans have influenced primate populations for millennia. More recently, hunting, habitat disturbance, disease, and climate change have increased in frequency and severity with detrimental impacts on primate populations worldwide. These effects and recent evidence from camera traps emphasize the importance of maintaining protected areas for conserving primate populations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Berber, Andrea. "The significance of evolutionary explanation of altruism for the understanding of morality." Theoria, Beograd 58, no. 2 (2015): 73–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.2298/theo1502073b.

Full text
Abstract:
In this paper I discuss the question whether the evolutionary explanation of altruistic behavior can help understanding the emergence of morality in human species. Two models of evolution of the altruistic behaviour are presented - the Hamilton?s model of kin selection and the Trivers? model of reciprocal altruism.Then, I point out some limitations of these models when applied to human behaviour. I consider the social life of primates and call attention to the limited extent of primate altruistic behaviour. I emphasize that the capacity of normative guidance of behaviour, which is essential feature of morality, is not present in social life of primates. I conclude that models for evolution of altruism and studies of primate social life can be useful for understanding the origins of social behavior in general, but can not offer a complete picture of the emergence of morality.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Dominy, Nathaniel J., and Amanda D. Melin. "Liminal Light and Primate Evolution." Annual Review of Anthropology 49, no. 1 (October 21, 2020): 257–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-010220-075454.

Full text
Abstract:
The adaptive origins of primates and anthropoid primates are topics of enduring interest to biological anthropologists. A convention in these discussions is to treat the light environment as binary—night is dark, day is light—and to impute corresponding selective pressure on the visual systems and behaviors of primates. In consequence, debate has tended to focus on whether a given trait can be interpreted as evidence of nocturnal or diurnal behavior in the primate fossil record. Such classification elides the variability in light, or the ways that primates internalize light in their environments. Here, we explore the liminality of light by focusing on what it is, its many sources, and its flux under natural conditions. We conclude by focusing on the intensity and spectral properties of twilight, and we review the mounting evidence of its importance as a cue that determines the onset or offset of primate activities as well as the entrainment of circadian rhythms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Esteve-Altava, Borja. "Cranial Anatomical Integration and Disparity Among Bones Discriminate Between Primates and Non-primate Mammals." Evolutionary Biology 49, no. 1 (November 10, 2021): 37–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11692-021-09555-9.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe primate skull hosts a unique combination of anatomical features among mammals, such as a short face, wide orbits, and big braincase. Together with a trend to fuse bones in late development, these features define the anatomical organization of the skull of primates—which bones articulate to each other and the pattern this creates. Here, I quantified the anatomical organization of the skull of 17 primates and 15 non-primate mammals using anatomical network analysis to assess how the skulls of primates have diverged from those of other mammals, and whether their anatomical differences coevolved with brain size. Results show that primates have a greater anatomical integration of their skulls and a greater disparity among bones than other non-primate mammals. Brain size seems to contribute in part to this difference, but its true effect could not be conclusively proven. This supports the hypothesis that primates have a distinct anatomical organization of the skull, but whether this is related to their larger brains remains an open question.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Mundinano, Inaki-Carril, Dylan M. Fox, William C. Kwan, Diego Vidaurre, Leon Teo, Jihane Homman-Ludiye, Melvyn A. Goodale, David A. Leopold, and James A. Bourne. "Transient visual pathway critical for normal development of primate grasping behavior." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, no. 6 (January 3, 2018): 1364–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1717016115.

Full text
Abstract:
An evolutionary hallmark of anthropoid primates, including humans, is the use of vision to guide precise manual movements. These behaviors are reliant on a specialized visual input to the posterior parietal cortex. Here, we show that normal primate reaching-and-grasping behavior depends critically on a visual pathway through the thalamic pulvinar, which is thought to relay information to the middle temporal (MT) area during early life and then swiftly withdraws. Small MRI-guided lesions to a subdivision of the inferior pulvinar subnucleus (PIm) in the infant marmoset monkey led to permanent deficits in reaching-and-grasping behavior in the adult. This functional loss coincided with the abnormal anatomical development of multiple cortical areas responsible for the guidance of actions. Our study reveals that the transient retino–pulvinar–MT pathway underpins the development of visually guided manual behaviors in primates that are crucial for interacting with complex features in the environment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Marino, Lori. "Brain-Behavior Relations in Primates and Cetaceans: Implications for the Ubiquity of Factors Leading to the Evolution of Complex Intelligence." International Astronomical Union Colloquium 161 (January 1997): 553–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0252921100015098.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis paper describes the results of a project aimed at addressing questions about the evolution of intelligence by utilizing comparisons of brain-behavior relations in cetaceans (dolphins and whales) and primates. Three specific questions were asked. First, have any cetaceans reached a hominid level of encephalization? Second, are cetacean-primate comparisons of brain organization useful for grappling with the differences that are bound to exist between any extraterrestrial organism and ourselves? Third, are there similarities in brain-behavior relations in cetaceans and primates that suggest behavioral evolution is shaped by general factors? Brain and body weights for cetacean and primate species were collected and compared with each other and with data on brain organization and social behavior. The results revealed that a hominid level of encephalization is not unique in mammalian history. Furthermore, cetacean-primate comparisons can be useful in understanding the different ways in which information processing systems can be organized. Finally, a comparison of the relation between encephalization and social group size in primates and cetaceans preliminarily suggests that similar mental capacities can be achieved through different independent evolutionary paths and that there may be factors common to behavioral evolution in all intelligent organisms.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Chapman, Colin A., Julio Cesar Bicca-Marques, Amy E. Dunham, Pengfei Fan, Peter J. Fashing, Jan F. Gogarten, Songtao Guo, et al. "Primates Can Be a Rallying Symbol to Promote Tropical Forest Restoration." Folia Primatologica 91, no. 6 (2020): 669–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000505951.

Full text
Abstract:
With 60% of all primate species now threatened with extinction and many species only persisting in small populations in forest fragments, conservation action is urgently needed. But what type of action? Here we argue that restoration of primate habitat will be an essential component of strategies aimed at conserving primates and preventing the extinctions that may occur before the end of the century and propose that primates can act as flagship species for restoration efforts. To do this we gathered a team of academics from around the world with experience in restoration so that we could provide examples of why primate restoration ecology is needed, outline how primates can act as flagship species for restoration efforts of tropical forest, review what little is known about how primate populations respond to restoration efforts, and make specific recommendations of the next steps needed to make restoration of primate populations successful. We set four priorities: (1) academics must effectively communicate both the value of primates and the need for restoration; (2) more research is needed on how primates contribute to forest restoration; (3) more effort must be put into Masters and PhD level training for tropical country nationals; and finally (4) more emphasis is needed to monitor the responses of regenerating forest and primate populations where restoration efforts are initiated. We are optimistic that populations of many threatened species can recover, and extinctions can be prevented, but only if concerted large-scale efforts are made soon and if these efforts include primate habitat restoration.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Immerman, Ronald S., and Wade C. Mackey. "Perspectives on Human Attachment (Pair Bonding): Eve's Unique Legacy of a Canine Analogue." Evolutionary Psychology 1, no. 1 (January 1, 2003): 147470490300100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/147470490300100110.

Full text
Abstract:
The mother-child bond is undoubtedly homologous with that of other primates (and mammals). However, the man-woman pair bond and man(to)child pair bond are not paralleled by any terrestrial primate nor many mammals. Hence, knowledge of primate behavior would not be predictive of the pan-human (i) social father and (ii) the extended pair bond between a man and woman (with the cultural overlay of marriage). It is suggested that female choice of mating partner shifted in the direction of a canid analogue in which men's motivations to share resources with the female and to exhibit paternalistic behaviors were positively selected. Accordingly, it would be predicted that, compared to other terrestrial primates, the neuro-hormonal bases for the mother-child affiliative bond would be similar, but the bases of man-woman affiliative bond and the man(to)child affiliative bond would be dissimilar.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Souza-Alves, João Pedro, and Isadora P. Fontes. "Anti-predator behavior of Coimbra-Filho’s titi monkeys (<i>Callicebus coimbrai</i>)." Neotropical Primates 26, no. 1 (September 1, 2020): 10–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.62015/np.2020.v26.74.

Full text
Abstract:
Predation pressure has an important role in shaping the behavioral, ecological and social patterns of primates. In this study, we describe two types of anti-predator behavior performed by Coimbra-Filho’s titi monkeys (Callicebus coimbrai), and compare our reports with the anti-predator strategies adopted by other Neotropical primates. The reports were performed on a small and isolated Atlantic forest fragment (14 ha) located in a highly-fragmented landscape in north-eastern Brazil. Although the titi monkey group was systematically monitored (July/2009 - August/2012: 1,513 hours) over the years, the events were recorded ab libitum method. Four events of anti-predator behavior were reported: three were in response to a medium-sized arboreal primate, the yellow-breasted capuchin monkey (Sapajus xanthosternos), and one in response to a large bird, the turkey vulture (Cathartes aura). When in the presence of S. xanthosternos, the titi monkeys emitted a quiet alert vocalization and descended to the dense undergrowth in order to avoid predation. On the other hand, when the titi monkeys were exploiting fleshy fruit on a tree, they verified the presence of an individual of C. aura and jumped to the ground. Both types of anti-predator behavior had been previously described for Neotropical primates. The anti-predator behaviors performed by the titi monkeys can contribute to the understanding of the adaptive processes of these poorly studied primates in fragmented habitats. Moreover, the passive behavior and the habitat characteristics may be an important aspect in the choice of strategy to be used, since the habitat pro- vided adequate refuges and allowed the titi monkeys to seek an alternative escape route.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Custance, Deborah M. "Social learning and imitation in human and nonhuman primates." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 1995. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/15076.

Full text
Abstract:
Most people assume that monkeys and apes can imitate, but recently, several researchers have suggested there is little convincing evidence of imitation in any nonhuman species. The purpose of the present thesis is to compare the imitative abilities of human and non-human primates. Some of the most convincing evidence for imitation comes from anecdotal reports of imitative behaviour in great apes. A survey of the literature was performed and a database of imitative episodes in chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans was compiled (using a similar approach to Whiten & Byrne's 1988a tactical deception database). Each report was subjected to a strict evaluation, and it was deemed that 23 reports from chimpanzees, 3 from gorillas and 4 from orangutans provided relatively convincing evidence of imitation. An experiment was conducted to test if chimpanzees can imitate as the anecdotal data suggests. Two chimpanzees were taught to reproduce 15 arbitrary gestures on the command "Do this". Next they were presented with 48 novel items. They imitated 13 and 20 novel gestures respectively. Using a rigorous coding system, two independent observers correctly identified a significant number of the chimpanzees' imitations (P< 0.0001). These results show that chimpanzees are capable of the complex intermodal visual-motor co-ordination and control necessary for imitation. The second experimental chapter explores whether monkeys, apes, and/or humans imitate in the context of a functional task. Six capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), eight chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and 24 children were presented with an analogue of a natural food processing task. The subjects were divided into two groups and each saw a different method for opening an artificial fruit. The children showed quite extensive imitation; the capuchin monkeys showed little to none; while the chimpanzees showed marginal imitative abilities. This constitutes the first experimental evidence of functional object imitation in a nonhuman specie.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Nagy-Reis, Mariana Baldy 1986. "Ecologia alimentar e comportamento de Callicebus nigrifrons em um fragmento florestal de Mata Atlântica em Campinas, SP." [s.n.], 2012. http://repositorio.unicamp.br/jspui/handle/REPOSIP/315842.

Full text
Abstract:
Orientadora: Eleonore Zulnara Freire Setz
Dissertação (mestrado) - Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Instituto de Biologia
Made available in DSpace on 2018-08-20T18:01:50Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 1 Nagy-Reis_MarianaBaldy_M.pdf: 70885205 bytes, checksum: c47da08b5a6303f670a2ad95f9faf467 (MD5) Previous issue date: 2012
Resumo: O gênero Callicebus é o segundo maior dentre os primatas neotropicais e possui ampla distribuição geográfica. No entanto, também e um dos gêneros menos estudados e pouco se sabe sobre sua ecologia e comportamento. Dessa forma, estudamos a ecologia alimentar e o comportamento de C. nigrifrons, com ênfase na dieta, padrão de atividade, uso de espaço e estratégias adotadas em resposta a abundancia de recursos no ambiente. Observações sistemáticas de um grupo de sauás foram conduzidas ao Iongo de um ano (547hs) em um fragmento de Mata Atlântica em Campinas, SP...Observação: O resumo, na íntegra, poderá ser visualizado no texto completo da tese digital
Abstract: Callicebus is the second largest genus among neotropical primates and is widely distributed. However, it is also one of the less-studied genera and there is little information on its ecology and behavior. Therefore, we studied the feeding ecology and behavior of C. nigrifrons, focusing on its diet, activity pattern, use of space, and strategies in response to food abundance. Information was collected during systematic observations on a titi monkey group during a one-year period (547hs) in an Atlantic Forest fragment in Carnpinas, SP...Note: The complete abstract is available with the full electronic document
Mestrado
Ecologia
Mestre em Ecologia
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Emery, Nathan J. "Neuroethological studies of primate social perception." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 1997. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/15078.

Full text
Abstract:
The neuroethological basis of social signals was investigated using a multidisciplinary approach, involving connectional and comparative analysis of anatomical data, single cell recording and behavioural techniques. Previous literature implicates the amygdala, anterior temporal and prefrontal cortex in primate social functions. Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) and cluster analysis were used to analyse the connectional relatedness of macaque cortico-cortical and amygdalo-cortical connections. This objective analysis separated the amygdala nuclei into two groups, the basolateral (BL) and centromedial (CM) complexes. A comparative analysis was made of the possible functions of the amygdala nuclei by correlating amygdala nuclear volume with 5 socio-ecological indices, across 44 primate species. The lateral basal (LB) nucleus and BL size was found to correlate positively with social complexity. CM size correlated negatively. The LB nucleus receives information from the STS, which contains visual neurons responsive to eyes, heads and bodies. These cells were assessed for coding of socially relevant information. Single cell recording localised within the macaque superior temporal sulcus (STS) revealed neurons responsive to specific views, elevations and orientations of the head, eye position, and specific views of bodies walking in specific directions and reaching to objects. The tuning of these neurons could therefore support the function of recognition of another's purposive behaviour (e.g. direction of attention or intention). Visually responsive neurons in the STS also differentiated faces of different species (i.e. monkeys, humans and other animals). Behavioural studies suggest that monkeys do not follow the direction of attention of humans, yet monkeys appear to have the neural capacity. A behavioural study using video stimuli, revealed that monkeys spontaneously follow other monkeys' gaze onto an object or point in space. It is concluded that the amygdala and STS are part of a neural system which enable monkeys to interpret another's gaze and actions within a purposive behavioural framework.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Street, Sally E. "Phylogenetic comparative investigations of sexual selection and cognitive evolution in primates." Thesis, University of St Andrews, 2014. http://hdl.handle.net/10023/11198.

Full text
Abstract:
A full understanding of any biological trait requires investigation of its evolutionary origin. Primates inspire great curiosity amongst researchers due to the remarkable diversity across species in both anatomical and behavioural traits, including sociality, sexual behaviour, life histories, neuro-anatomy, cognitive abilities and behavioural repertoires. The study of primates has involved comparative approaches since its inception, however, the necessary tools for statistically investigating the macro-evolutionary processes responsible for current diversity in biological traits have been developed only in the last 30 years or so, namely phylogenetic reconstruction and phylogenetic comparative methods. Amongst a multitude of evolutionary questions that can be addressed by phylogenetic comparative analyses, this thesis attempts to address two in particular, concerning primates. First, chapters 3 and 4 use meta-analysis and phylogenetic comparative analyses to investigate the evolution of large, brightly coloured ‘exaggerated sexual swellings' in female Catarrhine (‘Old World') primates. Together, chapters 3 and 4 show that such swellings are signals of temporal fertility, and present evidence to suggest that swellings co-evolved with conditions favouring male mate choice and cryptic female choice, therefore shedding light on the general conditions under which female signals of temporal fertility should evolve. Second, chapters 5 and 6 use phylogenetic comparative analyses investigate the evolution of enlarged brain size in the primate order. Together, chapters 5 and 6 suggest that multiple selection pressures have contributed to diversity in brain size and cognitive traits across primates, including sociality, intra-sexual competition and extended life history. Further, analyses presented in chapter 6 suggest that reliance on learned behaviour is a self-reinforcing evolutionary process, favouring ‘runaway' increases in cognitive abilities and reliance on culture in some primate lineages, which parallels increases in brain size, cognitive ability and reliance on culture in human evolution.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Olson, Monica A. "Preliminary observations of the ecology and behavior of the primates of North Pagai Island; Hylobates klossii, Macaca pagensis, Presbytis potenziani, and Simias concolor." Scholarly Commons, 1992. https://scholarlycommons.pacific.edu/uop_etds/2227.

Full text
Abstract:
The following study was conducted from August to November of 1991 on North Pagai Island in the Mentawai Archipelago, Indonesia. The purpose of the study was to obtain preliminary information on the ecology and behavior of the four Mentawai endemic primates: Hylobatcs klossii, Macaca pagensis, Presbytis potenziani, and Simias concolor. All four species are considered endangered due to human activities, especially hunting and various forms of habitat destruction. Therefore, the present critical status of these primates makes their conservation and that of the Mentawai Islands' rain forest a priority. It appears that resource partitioning occurs among the four primate species, M. pagensis occupied all vegetation levels and was less arboreal relative to the other three species which were primarily arboreal. All four species were found in forest habitat with S. concolor also occurring in forest swamp. M. pagensis and P. potenziani were found throughout the study area and only P. potenziani occupied coastal swamp. In addition, it is hypothesized here that for many P. potenziani groups, sleeping trees are located outside of the primary forest and these groups migrate on a daily basis to the forest. As a result, this may reduce interspecific competition since this species and H. klossii overlap in terms of resource use. Group size and composition are discussed and a monogamous social organization was identified for H. klossii and P. potenziani while M. pagensis. was found to form polygamous families. S. concolor is identified as forming one-male polygamous groups, however, based on my observations it might initially be interpreted that this species formed monogamous groups in the study area. Most behaviors, such as parental care, aggression, grooming, play, and feeding behavior, were rarely observed due to the wanness of these primates. Alarm response to detection of humans is discussed and distraction-like behaviors are described for each of the species. Population density was determined for each species and these values corresponded to those found in other studies although all values in this study were relatively low compared to other studies.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Chang, Tina R. "Social behavior and activity budgets of captive mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) : a comparison of a traditional exhibit and an ecologically representative habitat." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 1991. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/30988.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Bassett, Lois. "Effects of predictability of feeding routines on the behaviour and welfare of captive primates." Thesis, University of Stirling, 2003. http://hdl.handle.net/1893/3319.

Full text
Abstract:
The effects of variations in the predictability of appetitive events, such as feeding, have rarely been studied in animals in general or primates in particular. Feeding animals on highly predictable temporal schedules often results in the performance of food anticipatory activity (FAA), characterised by increased arousal and activity and thought to be detrimental to welfare. Temporally unpredictable feeding schedules have been interpreted as resulting in improved welfare. However, if feeding is made unpredictable by preceding it with an unreliable signal, it may result in frustration and aggression. It is suggested here that two distinct but overlapping types of predictability exist. 'Temporal' predictability describes whether an event occurs at fixed or variable intervals, whereas 'signalled' predictability relates to the reliability of a signal preceding the event. This thesis examines the effects of each of these types of predictability in relation to feeding. Welfare was assessed in laboratory-housed common marmosets( Callithrix jacchus) using behavioural measures, which were identified in the context of the routine stressor of human handling and weighing. The signalled and temporal predictability of presentation of a desirable titbit was subsequently experimentally manipulated. It was found that temporally unpredictable presentation of food, preceded by an unreliable signal, was associated with substantially increased stress-related behaviours in this species. If no signal was used, stress increased to a moderate level, but if the food delivery followed a reliable signal there were few behavioural changes compared to control animals. Temporally predictable feeding, without a signal, was associated with lower rates of stress-related behaviour than temporally unpredictable, unsignalled feeding. However, deviations from this temporally predictable schedule, representing delays to feeding, resulted in marked increases in stress. The results were confirmed with a further study, worked around existing feeding routines and using a different primate species, the stump-tailed macaque (Macaca arctoides). Based on these findings it is suggested that the most beneficial schedule for feeding captive primates is a temporally unpredictable one, which appears to buffer animals against the negative effects of delays as well as minimising FAA. Presentation of a reliable signal before food delivery appears to minimise the stress intrinsically associated with a temporally unpredictable routine. These recommendations represent a simple and inexpensive method of improving the welfare of captive primates.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Martin, Allison L. "Analysis of a quantitative behavioral assessment program to identify and treat abnormal behaviors in captive primates." Diss., Georgia Institute of Technology, 2016. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/54980.

Full text
Abstract:
Facilities housing non-human primates are required to make provisions for their psychological wellbeing, which may include monitoring animals for signs of decreased wellbeing such as the presence of abnormal behaviors or alopecia. By analyzing archival behavioral data collected by the Behavior Management Unit at the Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC), I aimed to identify behavioral predictors of self-wounding and alopecia and to evaluate the effectiveness of current treatments in reducing abnormal behavior and alopecia in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). The behaviors of self-biting and hair plucking (conditional logistic regression, p < .05) as well as floating limb and self-oral behaviors (Mantel-Haenszel chi-square tests, p < .05) were identified as behavioral predictors of self-wounding. Fear behaviors were associated with an increased risk of developing alopecia (Mantel-Haenszel chi square, p < .05). An inverse relationship was found between alopecia and stereotypic locomotor behaviors such as pacing, with animals who displayed these behaviors being less likely to develop significant hair loss (conditional logistic regression, p < .05). Overall, the type of treatment provided (e.g., additional foraging opportunities, the provision of toys, or the provision of visual barriers) did not predict improvement in levels of abnormal behavior or alopecia (logistic regression, p > .05). The results of these analyses add to the literature on self-wounding and alopecia and will allow refinement of the quantitative behavioral monitoring system at YNPRC such that more at-risk animals can be identified and treated prior to the development of abnormal or harmful behaviors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hyatt, Charles Winton. "Discrimination learning in the African elephant." Thesis, Georgia Institute of Technology, 1991. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/28887.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Florkiewicz, Brittany Nicole. "Facial Behavior and Pair Bonds in Hylobatids." OpenSIUC, 2016. https://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/theses/1844.

Full text
Abstract:
Among primates, humans have the largest and most complex facial repertoires, followed not by their closest living hominid relatives but by hylobatids. Facial behavior is an important component of primate communication that transfers and modulates intentions and motivations. However, why great variation in primate facial expressions evolved and why hylobatid facial repertoires seem to be more similar to humans than other apes is unclear. The current study compared 206 hours of video and 103 hours of focal animal data of facial expression repertoires, measures of pair bond strength, and behavioral synchrony of ten hylobatid pairs from three genera (Nomascus, Hoolock, and Hylobates) living at the Gibbon Conservation Center, Santa Clarita, CA. This study explored whether facial repertoire breath or frequency were linked to social parameters of pair-bonds, how facial expressions related to behavioral synchrony, and if facial feedback (i.e., the transfer of behaviors and intentions by mimicking observed facial expressions) were important between pair-partners. Intra-pair facial repertoires correlated strongly with repertoire composition and rate of use, suggesting that facial feedback was important, while behavioral synchrony showed no correlation with facial behavior. The results of this study suggest that larger facial repertoires contribute to strengthening pair bonds, because richer facial repertoires provide more opportunities for facial feedback which effectively creates a better ‘understanding’ between partners through smoother and better coordinated interaction patterns.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Collinge, Nancy E. Introduction to primate behavior. Dubuque, Iowa: Kendall/Hunt Pub. Co., 1993.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Bernard, Chapais, and Berman Carol M, eds. Kinship and behavior in primates. New York: Oxford University Press, 2004.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

F. B. M. de Waal. Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press, 1990.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

F. B. M. de Waal. Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press, 1989.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

F. B. M. de Waal. Peacemaking among primates. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press, 1989.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

E, Pereira Michael, and Fairbanks Lynn A, eds. Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press, 1993.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

A, Mittermeier Russell, ed. Ecology and behavior of neotropical primates. Washington, DC: World Wildlife Fund, 1988.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Phyllis, Dolhinow, and Fuentes Agustín, eds. The nonhuman primates. Mountain View, Calif: Mayfield Pub., 1999.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Small, Meredith F. Female choices: Sexual behavior of female primates. Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 1993.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Desmond, Morris, ed. Primate ethology. New Brunswick, NJ: Aldine Transaction, 2005.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Book chapters on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Behie, Alison M. "Primates." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 1–11. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1819-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Behie, Alison M. "Primates." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 5604–14. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55065-7_1819.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M. "Behavior Requirements for Self-Sustaining Primate Populations-Some Theoretical Considerations and a Closer Look at Social Behavior." In Primates, 307–19. New York, NY: Springer New York, 1986. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4918-4_23.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Strier, Karen B. "Northern Muriqui Muriqui Monkeys: Behavior Behavior , Demography Demography , and Conservation Conservation." In Primates and Cetaceans, 233–47. Tokyo: Springer Japan, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54523-1_12.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Mitchell, G., and Terry L. Maple. "Dominance in Nonhuman Primates." In Power, Dominance, and Nonverbal Behavior, 49–66. New York, NY: Springer New York, 1985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5106-4_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Jacobs, G. H. "Vision and Behavior in Primates." In Adaptive Mechanisms in the Ecology of Vision, 629–50. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0619-3_21.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Cezar, Leonardo, and Carlito da Silva Evangelista Junior. "Primates (Non-Human): Copulatory Behavior." In Encyclopedia of Sexual Psychology and Behavior, 1–10. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2023. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-08956-5_1908-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Manca, Claudia, and Karen L. Bales. "Adult Social Relationships in Primates." In Neuroendocrinology of Behavior and Emotions, 27–56. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2024. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-51112-7_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Newman, John D. "Ethopharmacology of Vocal Behavior in Primates." In Primate Vocal Communication, 145–53. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-73769-5_10.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Love, Melany, and David A. Washburn. "Computerized Testing Paradigm in Primates." In Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, 1595–601. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-55065-7_1585.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Fletcher, R. R., K. Amemori, M. Goodwin, and A. M. Graybiel. "Wearable wireless sensor platform for studying autonomic activity and social behavior in non-human primates." In 2012 34th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). IEEE, 2012. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/embc.2012.6346855.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Goncharova, Nadezhda. "FEATURES OF ENDOCRINE FUNCTIONS UNDER AGING IN NONHUMAN PRIMATES WITH VARIOUS TYPES OF STRESS BEHAVIOR." In XVII INTERNATIONAL INTERDISCIPLINARY CONGRESS NEUROSCIENCE FOR MEDICINE AND PSYCHOLOGY. LCC MAKS Press, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.29003/m2087.sudak.ns2021-17/111-112.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Gilja, Vikash, Michael D. Linderman, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "Multiday Electrophysiological Recordings from Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.260412.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gilja, Vikash, Michael D. Linderman, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "Multiday Electrophysiological Recordings from Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.4398736.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Ghadar, Nastaran, Xikang Zhang, Kang Li, Deniz Erdogmus, Guillaume Thibault, Alireza Bayestehtashk, Izhak Shafran Kris Coleman, and Kathleen A. Grant. "Visual hull reconstruction for automated primate behavior observation." In 2013 IEEE International Workshop on Machine Learning for Signal Processing (MLSP). IEEE, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/mlsp.2013.6661922.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Linderman, Michael D., Vikash Gilja, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "Neural Recording Stability of Chronic Electrode Arrays in Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.260814.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Linderman, Michael D., Vikash Gilja, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "Neural Recording Stability of Chronic Electrode Arrays in Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.4398424.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

VETEŠNÍK, A., and R. NOBILI. "PHASE BEHAVIOR OF THE PRIMARIES IN DISTORTION PRODUCT ANALYSIS." In Proceedings of the International Symposium. WORLD SCIENTIFIC, 2003. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/9789812704931_0079.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Linderman, Michael D., Vikash Gilja, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "An Autonomous, Broadband, Multi-channel Neural Recording System for Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.260813.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Linderman, Michael D., Vikash Gilja, Gopal Santhanam, Afsheen Afshar, Stephen Ryu, Teresa H. Meng, and Krishna V. Shenoy. "An Autonomous, Broadband, Multi-channel Neural Recording System for Freely Behaving Primates." In Conference Proceedings. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. IEEE, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/iembs.2006.4397626.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Primates Behavior"

1

Orr, J. (Investigation of effects of 60-Hz electric and magnetic fields on operant and social behavior and on the neuroendocrine system of nonhuman primates). Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), March 1989. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/6904585.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Orr, J. Investigation of effects of 60-Hz electric and magnetic fields on operant and social behavior and on the neuroendocrine system of nonhuman primates. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), April 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/6831470.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Orr, J. Investigation of effects of 60-Hz electric and magnetic fields on operant and social behavior and on the neuroendocrine systems of nonhuman primates. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), January 1990. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/7189423.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Orr, J. Investigation of effects of 60-Hz electric and magnetic fields on operant and social behavior and on the neuroendocrine system of nonhuman primates. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), October 1989. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/7146506.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Rogers, W., A. Coelho, Jr, S. Easley, J. Lucas, G. Moore, J. Orr, H. Smith, L. Taylor, and M. Tuttle. Effects of 60 Hz electric fields on operant and social stress behaviors of nonhuman primates. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), October 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/6904605.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Rogers, W., A. Coelho, Jr, S. Easley, and J. Orr. Effects of 60 Hz electrical fields on operant and social stress behaviors of nonhuman primates: Summary. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), April 1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/6904606.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Terdal, Erik. Captive Environmental Influences on Behavior in Zoo Drills and Mandrills (Mandrillus), a Threatened Genus of Primate. Portland State University Library, January 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.15760/etd.3009.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Rogers, W., A. Coelho, Jr, S. Easley, J. Orr, H. Smith, L. Taylor, and M. Tuttle. Effects of 60 Hz electric fields on operant and social stress behaviors of nonhuman primates: Projects 3 and 4. Office of Scientific and Technical Information (OSTI), January 1987. http://dx.doi.org/10.2172/6831473.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Kraus, Cornelia, Manfred Eberle, and Peter M. Kappeler. The costs of risky male behaviour: sex differences in seasonal survival in a small sexually monomorphic primate. Rostock: Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research, February 2008. http://dx.doi.org/10.4054/mpidr-wp-2008-005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography