To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Proprioceptive Drift.

Journal articles on the topic 'Proprioceptive Drift'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Proprioceptive Drift.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Lewis, Richard F., David S. Zee, Herschel P. Goldstein, and Barton L. Guthrie. "Proprioceptive and Retinal Afference Modify Postsaccadic Ocular Drift." Journal of Neurophysiology 82, no. 2 (1999): 551–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1999.82.2.551.

Full text
Abstract:
Drift of the eyes after saccades produces motion of images on the retina (retinal slip) that degrades visual acuity. In this study, we examined the contributions of proprioceptive and retinal afference to the suppression of postsaccadic drift induced by a unilateral ocular muscle paresis. Eye movements were recorded in three rhesus monkeys with a unilateral weakness of one vertical extraocular muscle before and after proprioceptive deafferentation of the paretic eye. Postsaccadic drift was examined in four visual states: monocular viewing with the normal eye (4-wk period); binocular viewing (2
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Fiorio, Mirta, Caterina Mariotti, Marta Panzeri, Emanuele Antonello, Joseph Classen, and Michele Tinazzi. "The Role of the Cerebellum in Dynamic Changes of the Sense of Body Ownership: A Study in Patients with Cerebellar Degeneration." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26, no. 4 (2014): 712–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00522.

Full text
Abstract:
The sense of the body is deeply rooted in humans, and it can be experimentally manipulated by inducing illusions in at least two aspects: a subjective feeling of ownership and a proprioceptive sense of limb position. Previous studies mapped these different aspects onto anatomically distinct neuronal regions, with the ventral premotor cortex processing subjective experience of ownership and the inferior parietal lobule processing proprioceptive calibration. Lines of evidence suggest an involvement also of the cerebellum, but its precise role is not clear yet. To investigate the contribution of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kodaka, Kenri, and Ayaka Kanazawa. "Innocent Body-Shadow Mimics Physical Body." i-Perception 8, no. 3 (2017): 204166951770652. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2041669517706520.

Full text
Abstract:
The paradigm of the rubber hand illusion was applied to a shadow to determine whether the body-shadow is a good candidate for the alternative belonging to our body. Three kinds of shadows, a physical hand, a hand-shaped cloth, and a rectangle cloth, were tested for this purpose. The questionnaire results showed that both anatomical similarity and visuo-proprioception correlation were effective in enhancing illusory ownership of the shadow. According to the proprioceptive drift measurement, whether the shadow purely originated from the physical body was a critical factor in yielding the signifi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gerber, Corinna N., Didier L. Gasser, and Christopher John Newman. "Hand Ownership Is Altered in Teenagers with Unilateral Cerebral Palsy." Journal of Clinical Medicine 11, no. 16 (2022): 4869. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jcm11164869.

Full text
Abstract:
We explored hand ownership in teenagers with unilateral cerebral palsy (UCP) compared with typically developing teenagers. Eighteen participants with UCP and 16 control teenagers participated. We used the rubber hand illusion to test hand ownership (HO). Both affected/non-affected hands (UCP) and dominant/non-dominant hands (controls) were tested during synchronous and asynchronous strokes. HO was assessed by measuring the proprioceptive drift toward the fake hand (as a percentage of arm length) and conducting a questionnaire on subjective HO. Both groups had significantly higher proprioceptiv
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Risi, Nicoletta, Valay Shah, Leigh A. Mrotek, Maura Casadio, and Robert A. Scheidt. "Supplemental vibrotactile feedback of real-time limb position enhances precision of goal-directed reaching." Journal of Neurophysiology 122, no. 1 (2019): 22–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00337.2018.

Full text
Abstract:
We examined vibrotactile stimulation as a form of supplemental limb state feedback to enhance planning and ongoing control of goal-directed movements. Subjects wore a two-dimensional vibrotactile display on their nondominant arm while performing horizontal planar reaching with the dominant arm. The vibrotactile display provided feedback of hand position such that small hand displacements were more easily discriminable using vibrotactile feedback than with intrinsic proprioceptive feedback. When subjects relied solely on proprioception to capture visuospatial targets, performance was degraded b
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Dempsey-Jones, Harriet, and Ada Kritikos. "Handedness modulates proprioceptive drift in the rubber hand illusion." Experimental Brain Research 237, no. 2 (2018): 351–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5391-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Cannella, Stefania, Alessia Folegatti, Massimiliano Zampini, and Francesco Pavani. "Multisensory integration in body perception is unaffected by concurrent interoceptive and exteroceptive tasks." Seeing and Perceiving 25 (2012): 33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/187847612x646550.

Full text
Abstract:
A recent study (Tsakiris et al., 2011) suggested that lower interoceptive sensitivity, as assessed by heat-rate estimation, predicts malleability of body representations, as measured by proprioceptive drift and ownership in a rubber hand illusion (RHI) task. The authors suggested that one explanation of their finding is linked to the notion of limited attentional resources: individuals with high interoceptive sensitivity are more aware of internal states and, in turns, they have less attentional resources available for multisensory processing. If this is the case, the competition between inter
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Sakamoto, Masanori, Yuta Akaike, Kazuya Tatsumi, and Hirotoshi Ifuku. "Hand Cooling Enhances the Proprioceptive Drift during Rubber Hand Illusion." Journal of Behavioral and Brain Science 14, no. 07 (2024): 210–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/jbbs.2024.147013.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Cameron, Brendan D., Cristina de la Malla, and Joan López-Moliner. "Why do movements drift in the dark? Passive versus active mechanisms of error accumulation." Journal of Neurophysiology 114, no. 1 (2015): 390–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00032.2015.

Full text
Abstract:
When vision of the hand is unavailable, movements drift systematically away from their targets. It is unclear, however, why this drift occurs. We investigated whether drift is an active process, in which people deliberately modify their movements based on biased position estimates, causing the real hand to move away from the real target location, or a passive process, in which execution error accumulates because people have diminished sensory feedback and fail to adequately compensate for the execution error. In our study participants reached back and forth between two targets when vision of t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Perepelkina, O., G. Arina, M. Boboleva, and V. Nikolaeva. "Somatoform Symptoms’ Influence on the Rubber Hand Illusion: Additional Analysis." European Psychiatry 41, S1 (2017): S231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.2240.

Full text
Abstract:
IntroductionIn the rubber hand illusion (RHI) [1], sense of body ownership is changed by synchronous touches to the hidden participant's hand and a visible rubber hand. It was previously shown [2] that medically unexplained symptoms were associated with the weaker RHI (n = 40).ObjectivesWe used data from our previous research [3] and supplementary questionnaires to test the hypothesis that somatoform symptoms would be associated with the decreased response to RHI.MethodsSubjects (n = 78) voluntarily undergo the following procedures: RHI experiment with measurement of proprioceptive drift and s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Rohde, M., M. Di Luca, and M. O. Ernst. "The time course of proprioceptive drift in the rubber hand illusion." Journal of Vision 9, no. 8 (2010): 712. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/9.8.712.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Patterson, Jacqueline R., Liana E. Brown, David A. Wagstaff, and Robert L. Sainburg. "Limb position drift results from misalignment of proprioceptive and visual maps." Neuroscience 346 (March 2017): 382–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.040.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Romano, Daniele, Elisa Caffa, Alejandro Hernandez-Arieta, Peter Brugger, and Angelo Maravita. "The robot hand illusion: Inducing proprioceptive drift through visuo-motor congruency." Neuropsychologia 70 (April 2015): 414–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.033.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Rana, Alex, Annie A. Butler, Simon C. Gandevia, and Martin E. Héroux. "Judgements of hand location and hand spacing show minimal proprioceptive drift." Experimental Brain Research 238, no. 7-8 (2020): 1759–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05836-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Riemer, Martin, Florian Bublatzky, Jörg Trojan, and Georg W. Alpers. "Defensive activation during the rubber hand illusion: Ownership versus proprioceptive drift." Biological Psychology 109 (July 2015): 86–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.04.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Ventura de Oliveira, João Roberto, Marco Aurélio Romano-Silva, Herbert Ugrinowitsch, et al. "Cathodal tDCS of the Left Posterior Parietal Cortex Increases Proprioceptive Drift." Journal of Motor Behavior 51, no. 3 (2018): 272–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00222895.2018.1468311.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Campos, Jennifer L., Graziella El-Khechen Richandi, Babak Taati, and Behrang Keshavarz. "The Rubber Hand Illusion in Healthy Younger and Older Adults." Multisensory Research 31, no. 6 (2018): 537–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002614.

Full text
Abstract:
Percepts about our body’s position in space and about body ownership are informed by multisensory feedback from visual, proprioceptive, and tactile inputs. The Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) is a multisensory illusion that is induced when an observer sees a rubber hand being stroked while they feel their own, spatially displaced, and obstructed hand being stroked. When temporally synchronous, the visual–tactile interactions can create the illusion that the rubber hand belongs to the observer and that the observer’s real hand is shifted in position towards the rubber hand. Importantly, little is un
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Asai, Tomohisa. "Agency elicits body-ownership: proprioceptive drift toward a synchronously acting external proxy." Experimental Brain Research 234, no. 5 (2015): 1163–74. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4231-y.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Thøgersen, Mikkel, John Hansen, Herta Flor, Lars Arendt-Nielsen, and Laura Petrini. "A novel method for investigating the importance of visual feedback on somatosensation and bodily-self perception." Scandinavian Journal of Pain 16, no. 1 (2017): 185. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sjpain.2017.04.059.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractAimsVisual feedback is hypothesized to play an important role in the phantom limb condition. In this study we attempt to create an illusory experimental model of phantom limb wherein this condition is simulated by removing the visual input from the upper limb in a group of intact participants. The aim of the study is to investigate the role of visual feedback on somatosensation, nociception and bodily-self perception.MethodsUsing a novel mixed reality (MR) system, the visual feedback of the left hand is removed in order to visually simulate a left hand amputation on 30 healthy particip
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Tsay, A., G. Savage, T. J. Allen, and U. Proske. "Limb position sense, proprioceptive drift and muscle thixotropy at the human elbow joint." Journal of Physiology 592, no. 12 (2014): 2679–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2013.269365.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Darnai, Gergely, Tibor Szolcsányi, Gábor Hegedüs, et al. "Hearing visuo-tactile synchrony - Sound-induced proprioceptive drift in the invisible hand illusion." British Journal of Psychology 108, no. 1 (2016): 91–106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12185.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Optican, L. M., and F. A. Miles. "Visually induced adaptive changes in primate saccadic oculomotor control signals." Journal of Neurophysiology 54, no. 4 (1985): 940–58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1985.54.4.940.

Full text
Abstract:
Saccades are the rapid eye movements used to change visual fixation. Normal saccades end abruptly with very little postsaccadic ocular drift, but acute ocular motor deficits can cause the eyes to drift appreciably after a saccade. Previous studies in both patients and monkeys with peripheral ocular motor deficits have demonstrated that the brain can suppress such postsaccadic drifts. Ocular drift might be suppressed in response to visual and/or proprioceptive feedback of position and/or velocity errors. This study attempts to characterize the adaptive mechanism for suppression of postsaccadic
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Kodaka, Kenri, Yutaro Sato, and Kento Imai. "The slime hand illusion: Nonproprioceptive ownership distortion specific to the skin region." i-Perception 13, no. 6 (2022): 204166952211377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/20416695221137731.

Full text
Abstract:
The “slime hand illusion” is a simple and robust technique that uses mirror-visual feedback to produce a nonproprioceptive ownership distortion. The illusion can be easily evoked by the participant watching the experimenter pinching and pulling a chunk of slime in a mirror while the participant's hand, hidden behind the mirror, is similarly manipulated. This procedure produces a feeling of one of their fingers or the skin of their whole hand being stretched or deformed in a similar way to the visible slime. A public experiment found that more than 90% of participants reported a strong sense of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Erro, Roberto, Angela Marotta, and Mirta Fiorio. "Proprioceptive drift is affected by the intermanual distance rather than the distance from the body’s midline in the rubber hand illusion." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 82, no. 8 (2020): 4084–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13414-020-02119-7.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In the rubber hand illusion (RHI), simultaneous brush stroking of a subject’s hidden hand and a visible rubber hand induces a transient illusion of the latter to “feel like it’s my hand” and a proprioceptive drift of the hidden own hand toward the rubber hand. Recent accounts of the RHI have suggested that the illusion would only occur if weighting of conflicting sensory information and their subsequent integration results in a statistically plausible compromise. In three different experiments, we investigated the role of distance between the two hands as well as their proximity to th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Gallagher, Maria, Cristian Colzi, and Anna Sedda. "Dissociation of proprioceptive drift and feelings of ownership in the somatic rubber hand illusion." Acta Psychologica 212 (January 2021): 103192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103192.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Holle, Henning, Neil McLatchie, Stefanie Maurer, and Jamie Ward. "Proprioceptive drift without illusions of ownership for rotated hands in the “rubber hand illusion” paradigm." Cognitive Neuroscience 2, no. 3-4 (2011): 171–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2011.603828.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Shibuya, Satoshi, Satoshi Unenaka, and Yukari Ohki. "Body ownership and agency: task-dependent effects of the virtual hand illusion on proprioceptive drift." Experimental Brain Research 235, no. 1 (2016): 121–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-016-4777-3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Tagini, Sofia, Federica Scarpina, Francesca Bruni, Massimo Scacchi, Alessandro Mauro, and Massimiliano Zampini. "The Virtual Hand Illusion in Obesity: Dissociation Between Multisensory Interactions Supporting Illusory Experience and Self-Location Recalibration." Multisensory Research 33, no. 3 (2020): 337–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20191425.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) is used widely to investigate the multisensory integration mechanisms that support bodily self-consciousness and, more specifically, body ownership and self-location. It has been reported that individuals affected by obesity show anomalous multisensory integration processes. We propose that these obesity-induced changes could lead to an unusual susceptibility to the RHI and anomalous bodily self-experience. To test this hypothesis, we administered a modified version of the RHI (using a picture of the participant’s hand) to individuals affected by obesity
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Eshkevari, E., E. Rieger, M. R. Longo, P. Haggard, and J. Treasure. "Increased plasticity of the bodily self in eating disorders." Psychological Medicine 42, no. 4 (2011): 819–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0033291711002091.

Full text
Abstract:
BackgroundThe rubber hand illusion (RHI) has been widely used to investigate the bodily self in healthy individuals. The aim of the present study was to extend the use of the RHI to examine the bodily self in eating disorders (EDs).MethodThe RHI and self-report measures of ED psychopathology [the Eating Disorder Inventory – 3 (EDI-3) subscales of Drive for Thinness, Bulimia, Body Dissatisfaction, Interoceptive Deficits, and Emotional Dysregulation; the 21-item Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21); and the Self-Objectification Questionnaire (SOQ)] were administered to 78 individuals w
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Rohde, Marieke, Massimiliano Di Luca, and Marc O. Ernst. "The Rubber Hand Illusion: Feeling of Ownership and Proprioceptive Drift Do Not Go Hand in Hand." PLoS ONE 6, no. 6 (2011): e21659. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021659.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Teaford, Max, Jason Gilliland, Olivia Hodkey, et al. "Preliminary Evaluation of the Moving Rubber Foot Illusion in a Sample of Female University Students." Perception 50, no. 11 (2021): 966–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/03010066211058802.

Full text
Abstract:
The Rubber Foot Illusion (RFI) is an illusion in which one is made to feel that a model foot is their own through synchronous visuo-tactile stimulation. Previous research suggests that the conditions the RFI can be elicited under are similar to those of the Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI). However, it was unknown if the RFI could be elicited by synchronous movement of a participant’s foot and a model foot. To examine this, we developed the Moving Rubber Foot Illusion (mRFI) and compared participants’ experience of it to the RFI. The results of this study suggests that the RFI can be elicited throug
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Schürmann, Tim, Joachim Vogt, Oliver Christ, and Philipp Beckerle. "The Bayesian causal inference model benefits from an informed prior to predict proprioceptive drift in the rubber foot illusion." Cognitive Processing 20, no. 4 (2019): 447–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-019-00928-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Vorobeva, V. P., O. S. Perepelkina, and G. A. Arina. "Equivalence of the Classical Rubber Hand Illusion and the Virtual Hand Illusion." Experimental Psychology (Russia) 13, no. 3 (2020): 31–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.17759/exppsy.2020130303.

Full text
Abstract:
Computer technologies implementation into the body illusions research is increasing because they allow to controllably model complex processes that cannot be realised in ordinary life. It was previously demonstrated that the rubber hand illusion may be reconstructed in the virtual setting and cause similar changes in the somatoperception when the virtual hand begins to feel like your own. This result suggests that the phenomenological experience obtained in the classical illusion and in its virtual reality version has much in common. However, a direct experimental comparison of the two illusio
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Merveille, Fomekong Fomekong Rachel, Baozhu Jia, Zhizun Xu, and Bissih Fred. "Advancements in Sensor Fusion for Underwater SLAM: A Review on Enhanced Navigation and Environmental Perception." Sensors 24, no. 23 (2024): 7490. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s24237490.

Full text
Abstract:
Underwater simultaneous localization and mapping (SLAM) has significant challenges due to the complexities of underwater environments, marked by limited visibility, variable conditions, and restricted global positioning system (GPS) availability. This study provides a comprehensive analysis of sensor fusion techniques in underwater SLAM, highlighting the amalgamation of proprioceptive and exteroceptive sensors to improve UUV navigational accuracy and system resilience. Essential sensor applications, including inertial measurement units (IMUs), Doppler velocity logs (DVLs), cameras, sonar, and
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

van der Westhuizen, Donné, Teneille Page, Mark Solms, and Jack van Honk. "The Territory of my Body: Testosterone Prevents Limb Cooling in the Rubber Hand Illusion." Multisensory Research 33, no. 2 (2020): 161–87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-20191361.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The Rubber Hand Illusion (RHI) is an experimental paradigm for assessing changes in body ownership. Recent findings in the field suggest that social emotions can influence such changes and that empathic motivation in particular appears to positively predict the malleability of body representations. Since the steroid hormone, testosterone, is well known to interrupt certain forms of empathic processing, in the current study we investigated whether 0.5 mg of testosterone affected ownership indices of the RHI. Forty-nine females participated in a double-blind, placebo-controlled experime
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Reschechtko, Sasha, Cristian Cuadra, and Mark L. Latash. "Force illusions and drifts observed during muscle vibration." Journal of Neurophysiology 119, no. 1 (2018): 326–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00563.2017.

Full text
Abstract:
We explored predictions of a scheme that views position and force perception as a result of measuring proprioceptive signals within a reference frame set by ongoing efferent process. In particular, this hypothesis predicts force illusions caused by muscle vibration and mediated via changes in both afferent and efferent components of kinesthesia. Healthy subjects performed accurate steady force production tasks by pressing with the four fingers of one hand (the task hand) on individual force sensors with and without visual feedback. At various times during the trials, subjects matched the perce
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Aimola Davies, Anne M., Rebekah C. White, and Martin Davies. "Spatial limits on the nonvisual self-touch illusion and the visual rubber hand illusion: Subjective experience of the illusion and proprioceptive drift." Consciousness and Cognition 22, no. 2 (2013): 613–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.006.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Campos, Jennifer L., Graziella El-Khechen Richandi, Marge Coahran, Lindsey E. Fraser, Babak Taati, and Behrang Keshavarz. "Virtual Hand Illusion in younger and older adults." Journal of Rehabilitation and Assistive Technologies Engineering 8 (January 2021): 205566832110593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/20556683211059389.

Full text
Abstract:
Introduction Embodiment involves experiencing ownership over our body and localizing it in space and is informed by multiple senses (visual, proprioceptive and tactile). Evidence suggests that embodiment and multisensory integration may change with older age. The Virtual Hand Illusion (VHI) has been used to investigate multisensory contributions to embodiment, but has never been evaluated in older adults. Spatio-temporal factors unique to virtual environments may differentially affect the embodied perceptions of older and younger adults. Methods Twenty-one younger (18–35 years) and 19 older (6
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Weinberg, Marc S., Conrad Wall, Jimmy Robertsson, Edward O’Neil, Kathleen Sienko, and Robert Fields. "Tilt Determination in MEMS Inertial Vestibular Prosthesis." Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 128, no. 6 (2006): 943–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/1.2378922.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: There is a clear need for a prosthesis that improves postural stability in the balance impaired. Such a device would be used as a temporary aid during recovery from ablative inner-ear surgery, a postural monitor during rehabilitation (for example, hip surgery), and as a permanent prosthesis for those elderly prone to falls. Method of approach: Recently developed, small instruments have enabled wearable prostheses to augment or replace vestibular functions. The current prosthesis communicates by vibrators mounted on the subject’s trunk. In this paper we emphasize the unique algorith
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Wittkopf, Priscilla G., Donna M. Lloyd, and Mark I. Johnson. "Changing the size of a mirror-reflected hand moderates the experience of embodiment but not proprioceptive drift: a repeated measures study on healthy human participants." Experimental Brain Research 235, no. 6 (2017): 1933–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-4930-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Nam, Dinh Van, and Kim Gon-Woo. "Robust Stereo Visual Inertial Navigation System Based on Multi-Stage Outlier Removal in Dynamic Environments." Sensors 20, no. 10 (2020): 2922. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s20102922.

Full text
Abstract:
Robotic mapping and odometry are the primary competencies of a navigation system for an autonomous mobile robot. However, the state estimation of the robot typically mixes with a drift over time, and its accuracy is degraded critically when using only proprioceptive sensors in indoor environments. Besides, the accuracy of an ego-motion estimated state is severely diminished in dynamic environments because of the influences of both the dynamic objects and light reflection. To this end, the multi-sensor fusion technique is employed to bound the navigation error by adopting the complementary natu
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Perepelkina, Olga, Maria Boboleva, Galina Arina, and Valentina Nikolaeva. "Higher Emotional Intelligence Is Associated With a Stronger Rubber Hand Illusion." Multisensory Research 30, no. 7-8 (2017): 615–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002577.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of the study was to investigate how emotion information processing factors, such as alexithymia and emotional intelligence, modulate body ownership and influence multisensory integration during the ‘rubber hand illusion’ (RHI) task. It was previously shown that alexithymia correlates with RHI, and we suggested that emotional intelligence should also be a top-down factor of body ownership, since it was not shown in previous experiments. We elaborated the study of Grynberg and Pollatos [Front. Hum. Neurosci.9(2015) 357] with an additional measure of emotional intelligence, and propose an
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Critchley, Hugo D., Vanessa Botan, and Jamie Ward. "Absence of reliable physiological signature of illusory body ownership revealed by fine-grained autonomic measurement during the rubber hand illusion." PLOS ONE 16, no. 4 (2021): e0237282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237282.

Full text
Abstract:
The neural representation of a ‘biological self’ is linked theoretically to the control of bodily physiology. In an influential model, selfhood relates to internal agency and higher-order interoceptive representation, inferred from the predicted impact of efferent autonomic nervous activity on afferent viscerosensory feedback. Here we tested if an altered representation of physical self (illusory embodiment of an artificial hand) is accompanied by sustained shifts in autonomic activity. Participants (N = 37) underwent procedures for induction of the rubber hand illusion (synchronous stroking o
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Padilla-Castañeda, Miguel A., Antonio Frisoli, Silvia Pabon, and Massimo Bergamasco. "The Modulation of Ownership and Agency in the Virtual Hand Illusion under Visuotactile and Visuomotor Sensory Feedback." Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 23, no. 2 (2014): 209–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/pres_a_00181.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known by the virtual hand illusion (VHI) that simultaneous and synchronous visuotactile sensory feedback within a virtual environment elicits the feeling of ownership of a virtual hand, by observing for some seconds in a scene a virtual hand being touched while at the same time receiving tactile stimulation on the real hand in the corresponding positions. In this paper, we investigate possible modulations in the feeling of ownership (sensation of owning a virtual hand) and of agency (sensation of owning virtual movements and actions) according to whether or not the participant's own
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

D'Alonzo, Marco, Alessandro Mioli, Domenico Formica, and Giovanni Di Pino. "Modulation of Body Representation Impacts on Efferent Autonomic Activity." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32, no. 6 (2020): 1104–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01532.

Full text
Abstract:
The afferent branch of the autonomic nervous system contributes with interoception to the multimodal sensory correlation continuously needed to update our representation of the body. To test whether the modulation of body representation would have an impact on the efferent branch of the autonomic nervous system, nonspecific skin conductance has been measured in three rubber hand illusion (RHI) experiments, controlled with asynchronous brush-stroking and incongruent fake hand position. Nonspecific skin conductance standard deviation (SCSD) computed along the whole 90 sec of stroking was found t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Frey, Vanessa N., Kevin Butz, Georg Zimmermann, et al. "Effects of Rubber Hand Illusion and Excitatory Theta Burst Stimulation on Tactile Sensation: A Pilot Study." Neural Plasticity 2020 (April 1, 2020): 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2020/3069639.

Full text
Abstract:
Synchronous visuotactile stimulation on the own hidden hand and a visible fake limb can alter bodily self-perception and influence spontaneous neuroplasticity. The rubber hand illusion (RHI) paradigm experimentally produces an illusion of rubber hand ownership and arm shift by simultaneously stroking a rubber hand in view and a participant’s visually occluded hand. The aim of this cross-over, placebo-controlled, single-blind study was to assess whether RHI, in combination with high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) given as intermittent (excitatory) theta burst stim
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Desmurget, Michel, Philippe Vindras, Hélène Gréa, Paolo Viviani, and Scott T. Grafton. "Proprioception does not quickly drift during visual occlusion." Experimental Brain Research 134, no. 3 (2000): 363–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s002210000473.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Brown, Liana E., David A. Rosenbaum, and Robert L. Sainburg. "Limb Position Drift: Implications for Control of Posture and Movement." Journal of Neurophysiology 90, no. 5 (2003): 3105–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00013.2003.

Full text
Abstract:
In the absence of visual feedback, subject reports of hand location tend to drift over time. Such drift has been attributed to a gradual reduction in the usefulness of proprioception to signal limb position. If this account is correct, drift should degrade the accuracy of movement distance and direction over a series of movements made without visual feedback. To test this hypothesis, we asked participants to perform six series of 75 repetitive movements from a visible start location to a visible target, in time with a regular, audible tone. Fingertip position feedback was given by a cursor dur
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Nayati, Jasir T., and Alan R. Hirsch. "137 Menstrual Synchrony of Burning Mouth Syndrome." CNS Spectrums 23, no. 1 (2018): 86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1092852918000330.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractStudy ObjectiveBurning mouth syndrome (BMS) is characterized by oral mucosal burning sensations, with normal clinical and laboratory results. Menstrual synchrony of migraines and epilepsy have been discussed; however, menstrual synchrony of BMS has not heretofore been described.MethodsCase Study: A 29 year old right-handed female exhibited intermittent BMS symptoms, one month after suffering a left parietal infarction. She describes the pain as a burningsensation, localized to the bilateral and anterior aspects of her tongue. It lasts for four days, starts three days prior to her mense
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Campbell, Jasmine M., Jasir T. Nayati, and Alan R. Hirsch. "139 Burning Mouth Syndrome After Hemicolectomy and Hyperalimentation." CNS Spectrums 23, no. 1 (2018): 87–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1092852918000354.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractStudy ObjectiveBurning mouth syndrome (BMS) is characterized by a burning sensation in the tongue or other oral sites [Grushka 2002]. Vitamin B complex deficiencies have been associated with BMS, including B1 (thiamine) [Lamey 1988]. Replacement with thiamine and other B vitamins was noted to cause relief of BMS in 34 of 150 patients [Lamey 1988]. BMS secondary to vitamin deficiencies have been discussed; however, hemicolectomy and hyperalimentation associated thiamine deficiency inducing chronic BMS has not heretofore been described.MethodsCase Study: A 63 year old female presents wit
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!