To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP).

Journal articles on the topic 'Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Tengs, Tammy O., and Donald L. Fisher. "Rapid Serial Visual Presentation of Traffic Signs." Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Annual Meeting 29, no. 7 (October 1985): 720–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193128502900720.

Full text
Abstract:
Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) consists of the brief presentation of words, one at a time, at the same location in a display. Text presented in an RSVP format can often be read more quickly than static text. Recall for RSVP traffic signs is compared with recall for wide and narrow static signs. Two sign durations are tested, 600 and 1200 msec, and word length is varied. At the brief duration, recall for RSVP signs was best, but at the longer duration, words presented in static signs were recalled more often. Shorter words were also recalled more readily when presented in an RSVP format.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Spence, Robert. "Rapid, Serial and Visual: A Presentation Technique with Potential." Information Visualization 1, no. 1 (March 2002): 13–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/palgrave.ivs.9500008.

Full text
Abstract:
This commentary addresses the recently exploited presentation technique called Rapid Serial Visual Presentation, or RSVP, and identifies a number of applications. It describes a number of different RSVP modes, discusses potentially attractive features and reports the results of experimental investigations. Some of the many issues requiring further research are identified
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Witkowski, Mark, and Robert Spence. "Rapid serial visual presentation: An approach to design." Information Visualization 11, no. 4 (April 25, 2012): 301–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1473871612439643.

Full text
Abstract:
A familiar action in the physical world is the riffling of a book’s pages in order to gain, in a very short time, some idea about its content. That action is appropriately termed rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). A computational embodiment of RSVP can provide such a variety of possible image presentation modes and flexibility of control as to offer a powerful tool to support a wide range of tasks involving the presentation of a collection of images. Those images can be conventional pictures (e.g. the Mona Lisa), the current image of a television channel, a frame of a video recording, the layout of a letterhead or page, a line drawing, a logo or the layout and shape of some text as in a motorway sign. Although our understanding of RSVP is not complete, sufficient experimental evidence and application experience is now available to lead to a useful set of guidelines for the interaction designer interested in exploiting its potential. These guidelines relate to six specific areas of design: the design style, the rate at which images are presented, image size on screen, the value of user control, image density on screen and image overlap. In this paper those guidelines are presented and justified by experimental evidence, much of it, unsurprisingly, emerging from the nature of human visual processing. In formulating the guidelines we fully acknowledge the importance, in a practical application, of aesthetic considerations; likewise, we take account of the need to minimise constraints on the creativity of the interaction designer. While RSVP addresses a specific class of tasks, we suggest that the principles discussed in this paper will have a broader relevance to image presentation techniques.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Lin, Zhimin, Ying Zeng, Hui Gao, Li Tong, Chi Zhang, Xiaojuan Wang, Qunjian Wu, and Bin Yan. "Multirapid Serial Visual Presentation Framework for EEG-Based Target Detection." BioMed Research International 2017 (2017): 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2017/2049094.

Full text
Abstract:
Target image detection based on a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm is a typical brain-computer interface system with various applications, such as image retrieval. In an RSVP paradigm, a P300 component is detected to determine target images. This strategy requires high-precision single-trial P300 detection methods. However, the performance of single-trial detection methods is relatively lower than that of multitrial P300 detection methods. Image retrieval based on multitrial P300 is a new research direction. In this paper, we propose a triple-RSVP paradigm with three images being presented simultaneously and a target image appearing three times. Thus, multitrial P300 classification methods can be used to improve detection accuracy. In this study, these mechanisms were extended and validated, and the characteristics of the multi-RSVP framework were further explored. Two different P300 detection algorithms were also utilized in multi-RSVP to demonstrate that the scheme is universally applicable. Results revealed that the detection accuracy of the multi-RSVP paradigm was higher than that of the standard RSVP paradigm. The results validate the effectiveness of the proposed method, and this method can provide a whole new idea in the field of EEG-based target detection.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Acqualagna, Laura, and Benjamin Blankertz. "Gaze-independent BCI-spelling using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP)." Clinical Neurophysiology 124, no. 5 (May 2013): 901–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2012.12.050.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Ichikawa, Makoto, and Masataka Miyoshi. "Perceived Duration Depends Upon Target Detection in Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Sequence." i-Perception 11, no. 6 (November 2020): 204166952098199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2041669520981996.

Full text
Abstract:
It is well known that the perceived duration for a given time period decreases with the reduction of the number of perceived events. We examined whether target detection failures in viewing Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) sequence, caused by attentional blink, affect this reduction of perceived duration. In two experiments, trials consisted of displays of two series of RSVP sequences; in the first sequence (the comparison), two, one, or no numerals were presented as targets embedded within the string of letters, while in the second sequence (the standard), only alphabetic letters were presented. In each trial, participants judged whether the duration of the comparison is perceived as longer than that of the standard (Experiment 1), or whether the number of frames in the comparison is perceived as more than that in the standard (Experiment 2). Results showed that perceived duration was inflated with target detection, but not with the increment of presented frames although number of perceived frames was inflated with both target detection and increment of presented frames. These results suggest that perceived duration in viewing RSVP sequences is determined by the cognitive load necessary to accomplish target detection rather than by the number of perceived frames.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Aquilante, K., H. Wyatt, and D. Yager. "In Congenital Nystagmats, Reading Text with Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) Reduces Mean Eye Velocity." Perception 26, no. 1_suppl (August 1997): 73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v970043.

Full text
Abstract:
Most low-vision subjects read text faster from an RSVP display than they do with full PAGE reading. This result has been confirmed with a group of subjects with congenital nystagmus (Plass and Yager, 1995, paper presented at ARVO). The present experiment was intended to determine whether reading with RSVP also reduced the severity of the nystagmus, which may have contributed to the faster reading rates. Eye position and velocity of congenital nystagmats were recorded with an infrared reflection technique, sampled at 40 s−1. Recordings were made in three conditions: (1) passively viewing a blank computer monitor; (2) silently reading full PAGE text at a size of approximately four times Snellen acuity; (3) silently reading RSVP text at the same size, at about 200 words min−1. We found that: (1) in frequency histograms of instantaneous eye velocities at 25 ms intervals, the full width at half height was an average of 60% smaller for RSVP reading compared to PAGE; (2) with a window of ±5 to 20 deg s−1, depending on the subject's baseline eye velocity, the number of epochs that were at least 75 ms in length, and the total time in these epochs, were significantly greater during RSVP reading than during PAGE reading or viewing a blank screen. We conclude that reading with RSVP reduces nystagmus, and may contribute to greater reading speed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Niimi, Ryousuke, Takuro Iizumi, and Kazuhiko Yokosawa. "Extraction of scene gist in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stimuli." Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the Japanese Psychological Association 79 (September 22, 2015): 2AM—070–2AM—070. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/pacjpa.79.0_2am-070.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Croker, Steve, and Frances A. Maratos. "Visual Processing Speeds in Children." Child Development Research 2011 (May 14, 2011): 1–6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2011/450178.

Full text
Abstract:
The aim of this study was to investigate visual processing speeds in children. A rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task with schematic faces as stimuli was given to ninety-nine 6–10-year-old children as well as a short form of the WISC-III. Participants were asked to determine whether a happy face stimulus was embedded in a stream of distracter stimuli. Presentation time was gradually reduced from 500 ms per stimulus to 100 ms per stimulus, in 50 ms steps. The data revealed that (i) RSVP speed increases with age, (ii) children aged 8 years and over can discriminate stimuli presented every 100 ms—the speed typically used with RSVP procedures in adult and adolescent populations, and (iii) RSVP speed is significantly correlated with digit span and object assembly. In consequence, the RSVP paradigm presented here is appropriate for use in further investigations of processes of temporal attention within this cohort.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Franco, Ana, Julia Eberlen, Arnaud Destrebecqz, Axel Cleeremans, and Julie Bertels. "Rapid Serial Auditory Presentation." Experimental Psychology 62, no. 5 (November 2015): 346–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000295.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. The Rapid Serial Visual Presentation procedure is a method widely used in visual perception research. In this paper we propose an adaptation of this method which can be used with auditory material and enables assessment of statistical learning in speech segmentation. Adult participants were exposed to an artificial speech stream composed of statistically defined trisyllabic nonsense words. They were subsequently instructed to perform a detection task in a Rapid Serial Auditory Presentation (RSAP) stream in which they had to detect a syllable in a short speech stream. Results showed that reaction times varied as a function of the statistical predictability of the syllable: second and third syllables of each word were responded to faster than first syllables. This result suggests that the RSAP procedure provides a reliable and sensitive indirect measure of auditory statistical learning.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Jitsumori, Masako, and Tomokazu Ushitani. "Rapid visual processing of picture stimuli by pigeons in an RSVP (rapid serial visual presentation) task." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition 43, no. 2 (2017): 127–38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xan0000132.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

De Baene, Wouter, Elsie Premereur, and Rufin Vogels. "Properties of Shape Tuning of Macaque Inferior Temporal Neurons Examined Using Rapid Serial Visual Presentation." Journal of Neurophysiology 97, no. 4 (April 2007): 2900–2916. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00741.2006.

Full text
Abstract:
We used rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) to examine the tuning of macaque inferior temporal cortical (IT) neurons to five sets of 25 shapes each that varied systematically along predefined shape dimensions. A comparison of the RSVP technique using 100-ms presentations with that using a longer duration showed that shape preference can be determined with RSVP. Using relatively complex shapes that vary along relatively simple shape dimensions, we found that the large majority of neurons preferred extremes of the shape configuration, extending the results of a previous study using simpler shapes and a standard testing paradigm. A population analysis of the neuronal responses demonstrated that, in general, IT neurons can represent the similarities among the shapes at an ordinal level, extending a previous study that used a smaller number of shapes and a categorization task. However, the same analysis showed that IT neurons do not faithfully represent the physical similarities among the shapes. The responses to the two-part shapes could be predicted, virtually perfectly, from the average of the responses to the respective two parts presented in isolation. We also showed that IT neurons adapt to the stimulus distribution statistics. The neural shape discrimination improved when a shape set with a narrower stimulus range was presented, suggesting that the tuning of IT neurons is not static but adapts to the stimulus distribution statistics, at least when stimulated at a high rate with a restricted set of stimuli.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Russell, Mark C., and Barbara S. Chaparro. "Reading from a Palm Pilot& Trade Using Rsvp." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 46, no. 5 (September 2002): 685–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/154193120204600518.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examines the feasibility of using the text presentation method known as Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) as a means of optimizing reading on small screen interfaces. Participants read text on a hand-held device in both the page-like format and at various presentation rates in RSVP. Reading comprehension, user satisfaction, and format preference were examined as dependent variables. Results showed: (1) there were significant differences in comprehension between the RSVP presented at 250 wpm and the higher speeds of 450 and 650 wpm; but (2) there was no significant difference in comprehension scores between the RSVP at 250 wpm and the page condition. Participants were able to comprehend text presented via RSVP at 250 wpm and the page format equally well. Despite this comparable performance between these formats, participants were generally less satisfied with the RSVP, and preferred 250 and 450 wpm presentation rates significantly more than 650 wpm.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Levin, E., W. Roland, R. Habibi, Z. An, and R. Shults. "RAPID VISUAL PRESENTATION TO SUPPORT GEOSPATIAL BIG DATA PROCESSING." ISPRS - International Archives of the Photogrammetry, Remote Sensing and Spatial Information Sciences XLIII-B4-2020 (August 25, 2020): 463–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.5194/isprs-archives-xliii-b4-2020-463-2020.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. Given the limited number of human GIS/image analysts at any organization, use of their time and organizational resources is important, especially in light of Big Data application scenarios when organizations may be overwhelmed with vast amounts of geospatial data. The current manuscript is devoted to the description of experimental research outlining the concept of Human-Computer Symbiosis where computers perform tasks, such as classification on a large image dataset, and, in sequence, humans perform analysis with Brain-Computer Interfaces (BCIs) to classify those images that machine learning had difficulty with. The addition of the BCI analysis is to utilize the brain’s ability to better answer questions like: “Is the object in this image the object being sought?” In order to determine feasibility of such a system, a supervised multi-layer convolutional neural network (CNN) was trained to detect the difference between ‘ships’ and ‘no ships’ from satellite imagery data. A prediction layer was then added to the trained model to output the probability that a given image was within each of those two classifications. If the probabilities were within one standard deviation of the mean of a gaussian distribution centered at 0.5, they would be stored in a separate dataset for Rapid Serial Visual Presentations (RSVP), implemented with PsyhoPy, to a human analyst using a low cost EMOTIV “Insight” EEG BCI headset. During the RSVP phase, hundreds of images per minute can be sequentially demonstrated. At such a pace, human analysts are not capable of making any conscious decisions about what is in each image; however, the subliminal “aha-moment” still can be detected by the headset. The discovery of these moments are parsed out by exposition of Event Related Potentials (ERPs), specifically the P300 ERPs. If a P300 ERP is generated for detection of a ship, then the relevant image would be moved to its rightful designation dataset; otherwise, if the image classification is still unclear, it is set aside for another RSVP iteration where the time afforded to the analyst for observation of each image is increased each time. If classification is still uncertain after a respectable amount of RSVP iterations, the images in question would be located within the grid matrix of its larger image scene. The adjacent images to those of interest on the grid would then be added to the presentation to give an analyst more contextual information via the expanded field of view. If classification is still uncertain, one final expansion of the field of view is afforded. Lastly, if somehow the classification of the image is indeterminable, the image is stored in an archive dataset.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Gannon, Erin, Jibo He, Xuefei Gao, and Barbara Chaparro. "RSVP Reading on a Smart Watch." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 60, no. 1 (September 2016): 1130–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1541931213601265.

Full text
Abstract:
Reading with Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) has shown promise for optimizing screen space and increasing reading speed without compromising comprehension. Given the wide use of small-screen devices, the present study compared RSVP and traditional reading on three types of reading comprehension, reading speed, and subjective measures on a smart watch. Results confirm previous studies that show faster reading speed with RSVP without detracting from comprehension. Subjective data indicate that Traditional is strongly preferred to RSVP as a primary reading method. Given the optimal use of screen space, increased speed and comparable comprehension, future studies should focus on making RSVP a more comfortable format.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Kornrumpf, Benthe, Florian Niefind, Werner Sommer, and Olaf Dimigen. "Neural Correlates of Word Recognition: A Systematic Comparison of Natural Reading and Rapid Serial Visual Presentation." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 28, no. 9 (September 2016): 1374–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00977.

Full text
Abstract:
Neural correlates of word recognition are commonly studied with (rapid) serial visual presentation (RSVP), a condition that eliminates three fundamental properties of natural reading: parafoveal preprocessing, saccade execution, and the fast changes in attentional processing load occurring from fixation to fixation. We combined eye-tracking and EEG to systematically investigate the impact of all three factors on brain-electric activity during reading. Participants read lists of words either actively with eye movements (eliciting fixation-related potentials) or maintained fixation while the text moved passively through foveal vision at a matched pace (RSVP-with-flankers paradigm, eliciting ERPs). The preview of the upcoming word was manipulated by changing the number of parafoveally visible letters. Processing load was varied by presenting words of varying lexical frequency. We found that all three factors have strong interactive effects on the brain's responses to words: Once a word was fixated, occipitotemporal N1 amplitude decreased monotonically with the amount of parafoveal information available during the preceding fixation; hence, the N1 component was markedly attenuated under reading conditions with preview. Importantly, this preview effect was substantially larger during active reading (with saccades) than during passive RSVP with flankers, suggesting that the execution of eye movements facilitates word recognition by increasing parafoveal preprocessing. Lastly, we found that the N1 component elicited by a word also reflects the lexical processing load imposed by the previously inspected word. Together, these results demonstrate that, under more natural conditions, words are recognized in a spatiotemporally distributed and interdependent manner across multiple eye fixations, a process that is mediated by active motor behavior.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

FINE, ELISABETH M., and ELI PELI. "Benefits of Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) Over Scrolled Text Vary with Letter Size." Optometry and Vision Science 75, no. 3 (March 1998): 191–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006324-199803000-00024.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Hollingsworth, Deidre E., Sean P. McAuliffe, and Barbara J. Knowlton. "Temporal Allocation of Visual Attention in Adult Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13, no. 3 (April 1, 2001): 298–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/08989290151137359.

Full text
Abstract:
In two experiments, we examined the ability of adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) to preocess multiple targets appearing in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream. Using a standard attentional blink (AB) task, subjects were required to both identify a target in the RSVP stream and detect a probe appearing in one of several posttarget serial positions. In Experiment 1, ADHD adults exhibited a protracted AB compared to controls, in that their probe detection did not improve as a function of increasing probe-to-target intervals (450-720 msec). In Experiment 2, the ADHD group performed as well as controls in detectin probes appearing immediately (i.e., 90 msec) after the target. Taken together, the results demonstrate that adults with ADHD exhibit a selective deficit in rapidly shifting attention between the target and the probe, when two appear several hundred milliseconds apart. These results suggest that adults with ADHD can use automatic (reflexive) attention to detect items in close temporal proximity in the RSVP stream, but have difficulty allocating controlled attention to multiple stimuli separated by several hundred milliseconds.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Hsieh, Shulan, and Alan Allport. "Shifting Attention in a Rapid Visual Search Paradigm." Perceptual and Motor Skills 79, no. 1 (August 1994): 315–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1994.79.1.315.

Full text
Abstract:
A method is introduced for studying shifts of attention in semantic space, testing 56 subjects in four experiments on a semantic monitoring task based on rapid, serial, visually presented (RSVP) word-sequences. Following a cue to shift attention, accuracy of semantic monitoring drops abruptly to a low level, then gradually recovers to reach preshift levels over successive stimuli in the RSVP sequence. Using this method, we compared two kinds of criterion-shifts, one requiring a set-reversal (‘reversal shifts’), the other involving a shift between orthogonally defined categories (‘orthogonal shifts’); no differences were found. We have also examined the difference in a shift between two different processing domains (semantic vs typographic) compared with a shift of criterion within the same processing domain. The results showed no differences for within- vs between-domain shifts. Finally, we studied the time-course of a semantic attention shift. Execution of a semantic shift did not follow an internally controlled time-course but was a direct function of the rate of stimulus presentation. No evidence was found for the operation of a ‘supervisory attentional system’ independent of external stimulus triggering.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Hester, Michelle, Steffen Werner, Cassie Greenwald, and Jessica Gunning. "Exploring the Effects of Text Length and Difficulty on RSVP Reading." Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting 60, no. 1 (September 2016): 1294–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1541931213601300.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent marketing efforts claim that by removing saccades, well designed Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) text displays can increase reading speed without an overall loss of comprehension. Due to the cognitive load when displaying long text it has been suggested that RSVP is better utilized in shorter text or longer text presented in small sections. With the introduction of smart watches as display mediums, opportunities for these types of text display have become more prominent due to their small form-factor. These small screen displays have limited space and often are used to convey short messages, which have motivated a closer look at RSVP. In the current study, we investigate the effects of text length and text reading level on comprehension. We compare RSVP to traditional (left-to-right) text presentations. In line with previous studies our results show that there were no differences between comprehension for RSVP and traditional text displays at normal reading speeds. Future, research may focus on the effects of training and familiarity with RSVP and its performance at the marketed higher readings speeds.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Chen, Chien-Hsiung, and Yu-Hung Chien. "Effect of Dynamic Display and Speed of Display Movement on Reading Chinese Text Presented on a Small Screen." Perceptual and Motor Skills 100, no. 3 (June 2005): 865–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.100.3.865-873.

Full text
Abstract:
Automatic dynamic displays, e.g., scrolling displays, are frequently used to present text information on small screens. This study examined the effects of three dynamic displays [leading, scrolling, and rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP)] and three presentations speeds [171, 250, and 305 wpm (words per minute)] on subjects' reading comprehension for different types of small screens for laptops, personal digital assistants (PDAs), and mobile phones. 12 college students who were native speakers of Chinese (4 men and 8 women between the ages of 19 and 36 years) participated. Scores for reading comprehension indicated (1) for laptops, both leading displays and RSVP, at speeds of about 250 wpm, comprehension was highest; (2) for PDAs and mobile phones, scrolling displays and RSVP, speeds of about 305 wpm were more suitable for presenting information in Chinese text; (3) consequently, RSVP appeared to be the most viable dynamic display on small screens and led to the highest reading comprehension for these Chinese readers.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Klein, Raymond M., and Bruce Dick. "Temporal Dynamics of Reflexive Attention Shifts: A Dual-Stream Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Exploration." Psychological Science 13, no. 2 (March 2002): 176–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00432.

Full text
Abstract:
We combined a prototypical exogenous cuing procedure with rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) to provide a precise characterization of the temporal dynamics of reflexive attention shifts. The novel paradigm thus created has several useful properties, most notably that the physical presentation of the target is neither an onset nor a unique event and that temporal precision is provided without the requirement for a speeded response. A biphasic pattern was observed, with early benefits followed by later costs (inhibition of return) at the cued location relative to the uncued location. The finding of inhibition of return in this paradigm disproves the assertion that inhibition of return is merely a reluctance to respond in the target's direction. It may be partly that, but encoding mechanisms linked to attention must also be involved.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Santangelo, Valerio, and Charles Spence. "Assessing the Automaticity of the Exogenous Orienting of Tactile Attention." Perception 36, no. 10 (October 2007): 1497–505. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5848.

Full text
Abstract:
We examined whether or not abrupt tactile onsets are capable of exogenously capturing tactile spatial attention when visual spatial attention is focused elsewhere. In experiment 1, we compared performance under dual-task conditions (where participants performed a tactile exogenous cuing task and a rapid serial visual presentation—RSVP—task at the same time) with their performance under single-task conditions (where the participants had to perform only the cuing task, although the RSVP stream was still presented in the background) and to a no-stream condition (where only the cuing task was presented). Tactile cuing was completely suppressed in both the dual-task and single-task conditions, showing that exogenous tactile spatial orienting is modulated by visual-spatial attention, which hence appears to be far from truly automatic. In experiment 2, we demonstrated that the abolishment of exogenous tactile orienting was not caused by the transient presentation of abrupt onset stimuli (letters). These results therefore show that exogenous spatial attentional orienting toward abrupt peripheral tactile stimuli is possible as long as perceptual resources are not depleted by a perceptually demanding (RSVP) task.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Abrams, Lise, Jennifer R. Dyer, and Donald G. MacKay. "Repetition Blindness Interacts with Syntactic Grouping in Rapidly Presented Sentences." Psychological Science 7, no. 2 (March 1996): 100–104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00337.x.

Full text
Abstract:
This study tested for predicted effects of syntax on a repetition deficit (RD) known as repetition blindness, the reduced probability of recall for repeated words in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) sentences The syntactic variable was phrase-congruent versus phrase-incongruent grouping within simultaneous RSVP displays With phrase-congruent grouping, each RSVP display contained a syntactic phrase (e g, “to play sports” in the sentence “They wanted to play sports but sports were not allowed”), whereas with phrase-incongruent grouping, RSVP displays contained nonphrases (e g, “sports but sports”) RD was extensive with phrase-incongruent grouping (29%), but nonsignificant (6%) with phrase-congruent grouping, as if phrase-congruent groups free up the extra processing time needed to connect repeated words into phrases in rapidly presented sentences, enabling top-down retrieval of the phrases during recall The present results comport with effects of syntax and prosody on auditory RD, and suggest that visual and auditory RD are identical underlying phenomena
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Ying, Haojiang, and Hong Xu. "Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) of Emotional Faces Generates Substantial Emotion Aftereffect as the Average Face of the RSVP Sequence." Journal of Vision 16, no. 12 (September 1, 2016): 157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/16.12.157.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Berger, Carole, Sylviane Valdois, Marie Lallier, and Sophie Donnadieu. "Age-Related Changes in Temporal Allocation of Visual Attention: Evidence From the Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) Paradigm." Journal of Cognition and Development 16, no. 1 (January 2015): 129–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2013.824882.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Zeng, Ying, Qunjian Wu, Kai Yang, Li Tong, Bin Yan, Jun Shu, and Dezhong Yao. "EEG-Based Identity Authentication Framework Using Face Rapid Serial Visual Presentation with Optimized Channels." Sensors 19, no. 1 (December 20, 2018): 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/s19010006.

Full text
Abstract:
Electroencephalogram (EEG) signals, which originate from neurons in the brain, have drawn considerable interests in identity authentication. In this paper, a face image-based rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) paradigm for identity authentication is proposed. This paradigm combines two kinds of biometric trait, face and EEG, together to evoke more specific and stable traits for authentication. The event-related potential (ERP) components induced by self-face and non-self-face (including familiar and not familiar) are investigated, and significant differences are found among different situations. On the basis of this, an authentication method based on Hierarchical Discriminant Component Analysis (HDCA) and Genetic Algorithm (GA) is proposed to build subject-specific model with optimized fewer channels. The accuracy and stability over time are evaluated to demonstrate the effectiveness and robustness of our method. The averaged authentication accuracy of 94.26% within 6 s can be achieved by our proposed method. For a 30-day averaged time interval, our method can still reach the averaged accuracy of 88.88%. Experimental results show that our proposed framework for EEG-based identity authentication is effective, robust, and stable over time.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Bekhtereva, Valeria, Matt Craddock, and Matthias M. Müller. "Affective Bias without Hemispheric Competition: Evidence for Independent Processing Resources in Each Cortical Hemisphere." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 32, no. 5 (May 2020): 963–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01526.

Full text
Abstract:
We assessed the extent of neural competition for attentional processing resources in early visual cortex between foveally presented task stimuli and peripheral emotional distracter images. Task-relevant and distracting stimuli were shown in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) streams to elicit the steady-state visual evoked potential, which serves as an electrophysiological marker of attentional resource allocation in early visual cortex. A task-related RSVP stream of symbolic letters was presented centrally at 15 Hz while distracting RSVP streams were displayed at 4 or 6 Hz in the left and right visual hemifields. These image streams always had neutral content in one visual field and would unpredictably switch from neutral to unpleasant content in the opposite visual field. We found that the steady-state visual evoked potential amplitude was consistently modulated as a function of change in emotional valence in peripheral RSVPs, indicating sensory gain in response to distracting affective content. Importantly, the facilitated processing for emotional content shown in one visual hemifield was not paralleled by any perceptual costs in response to the task-related processing in the center or the neutral image stream in the other visual hemifield. Together, our data provide further evidence for sustained sensory facilitation in favor of emotional distracters. Furthermore, these results are in line with previous reports of a “different hemifield advantage” with low-level visual stimuli and are suggestive of independent processing resources in each cortical hemisphere that operate beyond low-level visual cues, that is, with complex images that impact early stages of visual processing via reentrant feedback loops from higher order processing areas.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Vierck, Esther, and Jeff Miller. "Effects of task factors on selection by color in the rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task." Perception & Psychophysics 68, no. 8 (November 2006): 1324–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193731.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Barber, Horacio A., Shir Ben-Zvi, Shlomo Bentin, and Marta Kutas. "Parafoveal perception during sentence reading? An ERP paradigm using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) with flankers." Psychophysiology 48, no. 4 (August 18, 2010): 523–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01082.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Janson, Jolanda, Maarten De Vos, Jeremy D. Thorne, and Cornelia Kranczioch. "Endogenous and Rapid Serial Visual Presentation-induced Alpha Band Oscillations in the Attentional Blink." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26, no. 7 (July 2014): 1454–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00551.

Full text
Abstract:
The attentional blink (AB) is a deficit in conscious perception of the second of two targets if it follows the first within 200–500 msec. The AB phenomenon has been linked to pre-target oscillatory alpha activity. However, this is based on paradigms that use a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) stimulus stream in which the targets are embedded. This distracter stream is usually presented at a frequency of 10 Hz and thus generates a steady-state visual-evoked potential (ssVEP) at the center of the alpha frequency band. This makes the interpretation of alpha findings in the AB difficult. To be able to relate these findings either to the presence of the ssVEP or to an effect of endogenously generated alpha activity, we compared AB paradigms with and without different pre-target distracter streams. The distracter stream was always presented at 12 Hz, and power and intertrial phase coherence were analyzed in the alpha range (8–12 Hz). Without a distracter stream alpha power dropped before target presentation, whereas coherence did not change. Presence of a distracter stream was linked to stronger pre-target power reduction and increased coherence, which were both modulated by distracter stream characteristics. With regard to the AB results indicated that, whereas ssVEP-related power tended to be higher when both targets were detected, endogenous alpha power tended to be lower. We argue that the pattern of results indicates that in the pre-target interval several processes act in parallel. The balance between these processes relates to the occurrence of an AB.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Adhiambo Oguda, Benta G., George Vikiru, and Christine Wasanga. "The Influence of Presentation Format on Responses of Male Sex Offenders to Digital Paintings that illustrate the Consequences Sexual Crimes." Journal of Sociological Research 12, no. 2 (March 25, 2021): 26. http://dx.doi.org/10.5296/jsr.v12i2.18454.

Full text
Abstract:
Although art viewing experiences occur in varied contexts, responses of audience may be profoundly affected by the presentation format. In this paper, focus is drawn upon participation in a digital paintings exhibition and visual representation in assessing the responses of male sex offenders to digital paintings that illustrate the consequences of sexual crimes. The relationship between arts and technology is an emerging area of interest in modern research. In addition to the traditional gallery displays, digital technologies have provided new ways of audience participation in arts, enabling more involvement in the way art is consumed. The authors sought to determine the effect of viewing screen projected images in Rapid Serial Visual Presentation and in a gallery display. The study applied brief repeated exposures as described in Mere Exposure research. The study utilized temporary exhibition displays and projection by Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) where the respondents were passive participants, simply viewing the artwork. The participants were drawn from male sexual offenders aged 18-45 years at Nairobi West Prison, a male offenders’ facility in Nairobi City County. Stratified random sampling was used to select 61 male offenders convicted for defilement and rape. Respondents were randomly assigned to two experimental conditions involving viewing in projection by RSVP and gallery display. A five-point Likert scale was used to measure the participants’ responses to digital paintings illustrating consequences of sexual crimes. A Multivariate Analysis was used to assess ratings of the digital paintings against the various components of art. Findings show that painting style, colour schemes, themes and exposure frequency significantly influenced the participants’ responses to the digital paintings. The study recommends use of comparative analysis to determine how exposure to digital paintings impacts differently on other types of audiences.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Keysers, C., D. K. Xiao, P. Földiák, and D. I. Perrett. "The Speed of Sight." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13, no. 1 (January 1, 2001): 90–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/089892901564199.

Full text
Abstract:
Macaque monkeys were presented with continuous rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) sequences of unrelated naturalistic images at rates of 14-222 msec/image, while neurons that responded selectively to complex patterns (e.g., faces) were recorded in temporal cortex. Stimulus selectivity was preserved for 65% of these neurons even at surprisingly fast presentation rates (14 msec/image or 72 images/sec). Five human subjects were asked to detect or remember images under equivalent conditions. Their performance in both tasks was above chance at all rates (14-111 msec/image). The performance of single neurons was comparable to that of humans and responded in a similar way to changes in presentation rate. The implications for the role of temporal cortex cells in perception are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Stein, Timo, Jan Zwickel, Maria Kitzmantel, Johanna Ritter, and Werner X. Schneider. "Irrelevant Words Trigger an Attentional Blink." Experimental Psychology 57, no. 4 (December 1, 2010): 301–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000035.

Full text
Abstract:
It has been argued that salient distractor items displayed during rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) trigger an attentional blink (AB) when they share features with the target item. Here we demonstrate that salient distractor words induce an AB independently of feature overlap with the target. In two experiments a color-highlighted irrelevant word preceded a target by a variable lag in an RSVP series of false font strings. Target identification was reduced at short relative to long temporal lags between the distractor word and the target, irrespective of feature sharing with the distractor word. When the target shared features with the distractor word, target accuracy was reduced across all lags. Accordingly, feature sharing between the distractor word and the target did not amplify the AB, but had an additive effect on attentional capture by the distractor word.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

MORAWETZ, CARMEN, PETRA HOLZ, JUERGEN BAUDEWIG, STEFAN TREUE, and PETER DECHENT. "Split of attentional resources in human visual cortex." Visual Neuroscience 24, no. 6 (November 2007): 817–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0952523807070745.

Full text
Abstract:
Visual spatial attention has been described as a process that favors the processing of sensory information that falls into the “spotlight of attention.” Recent studies have provided support for an ability to split this attentional focus to selectively process spatially separate locations. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, the signature for the presence of multiple spotlights is the presence of multiple retinotopically specific foci of activation in striate and extrastriate visual areas. We used this approach to investigate the presence of such separable activations as a function of the eccentricity of the spatial foci of attention. Visual stimuli consisted of letters and digits displayed in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). Five RSVP streams were presented simultaneously, one in the center of the visual field and one in each visual field quadrant. Subjects had to deploy their attention either to a single peripheral location or two non-contiguous regions performing a match-mismatch judgment. The results show that dividing attention leads to multiple spotlights of attention for central as well as more peripheral locations of the visual field. However, depending on the exact location and width of the attentional spotlights, resulting activation maps might reveal merged activation patterns even in the presence of distinct attentional spotlights.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Rummer, Ralf. "Immediate and Delayed Recall of Visually Presented Sentences:." Experimental Psychology 51, no. 1 (January 2004): 15–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.51.1.15.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. The lure intrusion effect refers to the observation that lexical priming affects recall of sentences. This effect is taken as evidence against the contribution of surface information, even with immediate sentence recall. Recently, Rummer and Engelkamp (2003a ) demonstrated that this effect, which is usually observed under rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP), does not appear under immediate recall of auditorily presented sentences. This finding indicates that surface information (i.e., phonological or acoustic-sensory information) can contribute to immediate sentence recall. So far, however, the findings do not allow for a decision on whether phonological and/or acoustic-sensory information is used in immediate sentence recall. In order to dissociate the two kinds of surface information, an experiment was conducted in which immediate and delayed recall were tested for sentences that were visually presented for a longer period of time than in RSVP. This kind of presentation should support phonological representations, but does not allow for acoustic-sensory representations. The findings showed a smaller intrusion effect for immediate than for delayed recall. This indicates that, if available, phonological information is involved in immediate sentence recall thereby reducing the lure intrusion effect. Furthermore, the findings support the assumption that the phonological trace provided via RSVP reading is weaker than in normal reading.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Craston, Patrick, Brad Wyble, Srivas Chennu, and Howard Bowman. "The Attentional Blink Reveals Serial Working Memory Encoding: Evidence from Virtual and Human Event-related Potentials." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21, no. 3 (March 2009): 550–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21036.

Full text
Abstract:
Observers often miss a second target (T2) if it follows an identified first target item (T1) within half a second in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP), a finding termed the attentional blink. If two targets are presented in immediate succession, however, accuracy is excellent (Lag 1 sparing). The resource sharing hypothesis proposes a dynamic distribution of resources over a time span of up to 600 msec during the attentional blink. In contrast, the ST2 model argues that working memory encoding is serial during the attentional blink and that, due to joint consolidation, Lag 1 is the only case where resources are shared. Experiment 1 investigates the P3 ERP component evoked by targets in RSVP. The results suggest that, in this context, P3 amplitude is an indication of bottom–up strength rather than a measure of cognitive resource allocation. Experiment 2, employing a two-target paradigm, suggests that T1 consolidation is not affected by the presentation of T2 during the attentional blink. However, if targets are presented in immediate succession (Lag 1 sparing), they are jointly encoded into working memory. We use the ST2 model's neural network implementation, which replicates a range of behavioral results related to the attentional blink, to generate “virtual ERPs” by summing across activation traces. We compare virtual to human ERPs and show how the results suggest a serial nature of working memory encoding as implied by the ST2 model.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Dell'Acqua, Roberto, Paul E. Dux, Brad Wyble, Mattia Doro, Paola Sessa, Federica Meconi, and Pierre Jolicœur. "The Attentional Blink Impairs Detection and Delays Encoding of Visual Information: Evidence from Human Electrophysiology." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27, no. 4 (April 2015): 720–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00752.

Full text
Abstract:
This article explores the time course of the functional interplay between detection and encoding stages of information processing in the brain and the role they play in conscious visual perception. We employed a multitarget rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) approach and examined the electrophysiological P3 component elicited by a target terminating an RSVP sequence. Target-locked P3 activity was detected both at frontal and parietal recording sites and an independent component analysis confirmed the presence of two distinct P3 components. The posterior P3b varied with intertarget lag, with diminished amplitude and postponed latency at short relative to long lags—an electroencephalographic signature of the attentional blink (AB). Under analogous conditions, the anterior P3a was also reduced in amplitude but did not vary in latency. Collectively, the results provide an electrophysiological record of the interaction between frontal and posterior components linked to detection (P3a) and encoding (P3b) of visual information. Our findings suggest that, although the AB delays target encoding into working memory, it does not slow down detection of a target but instead reduces the efficacy of this process. A functional characterization of P3a in attentive tasks is discussed with reference to current models of the AB phenomenon.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Parks, Nathan A., Matthew R. Hilimire, and Paul M. Corballis. "Steady-state Signatures of Visual Perceptual Load, Multimodal Distractor Filtering, and Neural Competition." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23, no. 5 (May 2011): 1113–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21460.

Full text
Abstract:
The perceptual load theory of attention posits that attentional selection occurs early in processing when a task is perceptually demanding but occurs late in processing otherwise. We used a frequency-tagged steady-state evoked potential paradigm to investigate the modality specificity of perceptual load-induced distractor filtering and the nature of neural-competitive interactions between task and distractor stimuli. EEG data were recorded while participants monitored a stream of stimuli occurring in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) for the appearance of previously assigned targets. Perceptual load was manipulated by assigning targets that were identifiable by color alone (low load) or by the conjunction of color and orientation (high load). The RSVP task was performed alone and in the presence of task-irrelevant visual and auditory distractors. The RSVP stimuli, visual distractors, and auditory distractors were “tagged” by modulating each at a unique frequency (2.5, 8.5, and 40.0 Hz, respectively), which allowed each to be analyzed separately in the frequency domain. We report three important findings regarding the neural mechanisms of perceptual load. First, we replicated previous findings of within-modality distractor filtering and demonstrated a reduction in visual distractor signals with high perceptual load. Second, auditory steady-state distractor signals were unaffected by manipulations of visual perceptual load, consistent with the idea that perceptual load-induced distractor filtering is modality specific. Third, analysis of task-related signals revealed that visual distractors competed with task stimuli for representation and that increased perceptual load appeared to resolve this competition in favor of the task stimulus.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Metzner, Paul, Titus von der Malsburg, Shravan Vasishth, and Frank Rösler. "Brain Responses to World Knowledge Violations: A Comparison of Stimulus- and Fixation-triggered Event-related Potentials and Neural Oscillations." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27, no. 5 (May 2015): 1017–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00731.

Full text
Abstract:
Recent research has shown that brain potentials time-locked to fixations in natural reading can be similar to brain potentials recorded during rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). We attempted two replications of Hagoort, Hald, Bastiaansen, and Petersson [Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304, 438–441, 2004] to determine whether this correspondence also holds for oscillatory brain responses. Hagoort et al. reported an N400 effect and synchronization in the theta and gamma range following world knowledge violations. Our first experiment (n = 32) used RSVP and replicated both the N400 effect in the ERPs and the power increase in the theta range in the time–frequency domain. In the second experiment (n = 49), participants read the same materials freely while their eye movements and their EEG were monitored. First fixation durations, gaze durations, and regression rates were increased, and the ERP showed an N400 effect. An analysis of time–frequency representations showed synchronization in the delta range (1–3 Hz) and desynchronization in the upper alpha range (11–13 Hz) but no theta or gamma effects. The results suggest that oscillatory EEG changes elicited by world knowledge violations are different in natural reading and RSVP. This may reflect differences in how representations are constructed and retrieved from memory in the two presentation modes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Yago, Hinako, and Minoru Nakayama. "Influence of Manupulation for Image Features of Kanji Stimuli on the Attentional Blink During Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Tasks." Journal of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers 70, no. 7 (2016): J166—J169. http://dx.doi.org/10.3169/itej.70.j166.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Demeter, Elise, and Marty G. Woldorff. "Transient Distraction and Attentional Control during a Sustained Selective Attention Task." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 28, no. 7 (July 2016): 935–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00949.

Full text
Abstract:
Distracting stimuli in the environment can pull our attention away from our goal-directed tasks. fMRI studies have implicated regions in right frontal cortex as being particularly important for processing distractors [e.g., de Fockert, J. W., & Theeuwes, J. Role of frontal cortex in attentional capture by singleton distractors. Brain and Cognition, 80, 367–373, 2012; Demeter, E., Hernandez-Garcia, L., Sarter, M., & Lustig, C. Challenges to attention: A continuous arterial spin labeling (ASL) study of the effects of distraction on sustained attention. Neuroimage, 54, 1518–1529, 2011]. Less is known, however, about the timing and sequence of how right frontal or other brain regions respond selectively to distractors and how distractors impinge upon the cascade of processes related to detecting and processing behaviorally relevant target stimuli. Here we used EEG and ERPs to investigate the neural consequences of a perceptually salient but task-irrelevant distractor on the detection of rare target stimuli embedded in a rapid, serial visual presentation (RSVP) stream. We found that distractors that occur during the presentation of a target interfere behaviorally with detection of those targets, reflected by reduced detection rates, and that these missed targets show a reduced amplitude of the long-latency, detection-related P3 component. We also found that distractors elicited a right-lateralized frontal negativity beginning at 100 msec, whose amplitude negatively correlated across participants with their distraction-related behavioral impairment. Finally, we also quantified the instantaneous amplitude of the steady-state visual evoked potentials elicited by the RSVP stream and found that the occurrence of a distractor resulted in a transient amplitude decrement of the steady-state visual evoked potential, presumably reflecting the pull of attention away from the RSVP stream when distracting stimuli occur in the environment.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Prins, N., and J. F. Juola. "Attention Switching vs Attention Sharing in Searching Dual RSVP Streams." Perception 25, no. 1_suppl (August 1996): 122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v96p0304.

Full text
Abstract:
Ideas about how visual attention is distributed over space include spotlight, zoom lens, and various resource allocation models. Spotlight and serial allocation models assume that attention is narrowly focused and switches from one object to another in visual search. Zoom lens and parallel allocation models, on the other hand, describe a flexible gradient within which attention can be shared among several items simultaneously. We report two experiments in which simultaneous rapid serial visual presentations (RSVPs) of two streams of digits were used, one above and one below a fixation point. In experiment 1, subjects were told to report the digit immediately following a uniquely coloured signal digit. In some trial blocks the coloured signal digit always appeared in either the top or bottom stream, and in other blocks the signal digit could occur in either stream. Stream location probabilities were varied between blocks in order to induce strategic variations in attentional allocation. In experiment 2, subjects were told to report the first two digits visible when the fixation point changed colour. Subjects were instructed to report one digit from the top stream and one from the bottom, with report order counterbalanced between blocks. The lag between the response signal and the actual digit reported was shown to vary strongly with signal location probability (experiment 1), and the lag between items reported from the top and bottom streams depended heavily on the order of report (experiment 2). The results were more consistent with an attention-switching model than with an attention-sharing model of visual attention.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Matin, Ethel, David C. Nofer, and Lisa Gische. "Separating Perception Time from Response Time: The Slope Transition Paradigm." Perception 31, no. 3 (March 2002): 323–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p3304.

Full text
Abstract:
This paper describes the slope transition paradigm (STP), a variant of rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) that separates early (perceptual) processing time from total response time. The paradigm is based on a very simple idea: provide varying amounts of time for perceptual processing and find the moment when the subject begins to waste time waiting for more data. That moment is a measure of how much time was actually needed. The method was used in two experiments. Results are discussed in relation to set-size effects, perceptual capacity limits, attentional dwell times, and some related neurophysiological findings. The method appears to tap aspects of information processing that differ from those tapped in studies of the psychological refractory period, the attentional blink, and repetition blindness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Yago, Hinako, and Minoru Nakayama. "Influence of Variation in Image Features of Kanji Stimuli on the Attentional Blink during Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Tasks." Journal of the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers 69, no. 5 (2015): J190—J196. http://dx.doi.org/10.3169/itej.69.j190.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Raymond, Jane E. "New Objects, Not New Features, Trigger the Attentional Blink." Psychological Science 14, no. 1 (January 2003): 54–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01418.

Full text
Abstract:
When two different targets must be selected from a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) of images, perception of the second target will be markedly reduced if it is presented within about a half second of the first. Known as the attentional blink (AB), this effect reflects temporal limitations in attentional processes enabling awareness of image representations. I tested whether these limitations occur at an object or feature level of processing by presenting (in RSVP) multiple images of the same (old) object depicted in different orientations. Targets were defined by new features added either to this or to a new object. When the first target feature appeared on the old object, no AB effects were found even when the second target was a new object. When a new object carried the first target feature, an AB effect was found even when the second target feature appeared on the same “new” object. The AB appears to reflect limitations in the creation of new object representations, rather than temporal limitations of awareness per se.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Chan, Jason L., Aaron Kucyi, and Joseph F. X. DeSouza. "Stable Task Representations under Attentional Load Revealed with Multivariate Pattern Analysis of Human Brain Activity." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27, no. 9 (September 2015): 1789–800. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00819.

Full text
Abstract:
Performing multiple tasks concurrently places a load on limited attentional resources and results in disrupted task performance. Although human neuroimaging studies have investigated the neural correlates of attentional load, how attentional load affects task processing is poorly understood. Here, task-related neural activity was investigated using fMRI with conventional univariate analysis and multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) while participants performed blocks of prosaccades and antisaccades, either with or without a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task. Performing prosaccades and antisaccades with RSVP increased error rates and RTs, decreased mean activation in frontoparietal brain areas associated with oculomotor control, and eliminated differences in activation between prosaccades and antisaccades. However, task identity could be decoded from spatial patterns of activation both in the absence and presence of an attentional load. Furthermore, in the FEFs and intraparietal sulcus, these spatial representations were found to be similar using cross-trial-type MVPA, which suggests stability under attentional load. These results demonstrate that attentional load may disrupt the strength of task-related neural activity, rather than the identity of task representations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Zivony, Alon, and Martin Eimer. "Perceptual competition between targets and distractors determines working memory access and produces intrusion errors in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) tasks." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 46, no. 12 (December 2020): 1490–510. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000871.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

INGLES, JANET L., and GAIL A. ESKES. "A comparison of letter and digit processing in letter-by-letter reading." Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 14, no. 1 (December 14, 2007): 164–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617708080119.

Full text
Abstract:
The extent to which letter-by-letter reading results from a specific orthographic deficit, as compared with a nonspecific disturbance in basic visuoperceptual mechanisms, is unclear. The current study directly compared processing of letters and digits in a letter-by-letter reader, G.M., using a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task and a speeded matching task. Comparisons were made to a group of six brain-damaged individuals without reading deficits. In the RSVP task, G.M. had increased difficulty reporting the target identities when they were letters, as compared with digits. Although this general pattern was also evident in the control group, the magnitude of the letter–digit accuracy difference was greater in G.M. Similarly, in the matching task, G.M. was slower to match letters than digits, relative to the control group, although his response times to both item types were increased. These data suggest that letter-by-letter reading, at least in this case, results from a visuoperceptual encoding deficit that particularly affects letters, but also extends to processing of digits to a lesser extent. Results are consistent with the notion that a left occipitotemporal area is specialized for letter processing with greater bilaterality in the visual processing of digits. (JINS, 2008,14, 164–173.)
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Zhao, Qi, Hui He, Huang Gu, Junfeng Zhao, Peilian Chi, and Xiaoming Li. "Facial Expression Processing of Children Orphaned by Parental HIV/AIDS: A Cross-Sectional ERP Study with Rapid Serial Visual Presentation." International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 19 (September 23, 2021): 9995. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18199995.

Full text
Abstract:
Existing behavioral studies have suggested that individuals with early life stress usually show abnormal emotional processing. However, limited event-related brain potentials (ERPs) evidence was available to explore the emotional processes in children orphaned by parental HIV/AIDS (“AIDS orphans”). The current study aims to investigate whether there are behavioral and neurological obstacles in the recognition of emotional faces in AIDS orphans and also to further explore the processing stage at which the difference in facial emotion recognition exists. A total of 81 AIDS orphans and 60 non-orphan children were recruited through the local communities and school systems in Henan, China. Participants completed a computer version of the rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task while recording ERPs. Behavioral results showed that orphans displayed higher response accuracy and shorter reaction time than the control (ps < 0.05). As for the ERPs analysis, the attenuated amplitude of N170 (i.e., an early component sensitive to facial configuration) was observed in AIDS orphans compared to the non-orphan control with happy and neutral faces; P300 (i.e., an endogenous component for affective valence evaluation in emotional processing) also showed significant differences in parietal lobe between groups, the non-orphan control group produced larger P300 amplitudes than orphans (p < 0.05). The results suggested that compared to the control group, AIDS orphans showed impaired facial emotion recognition ability with reduced brain activation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography