Journal articles on the topic 'Reasoning (Psychology) Logic'
Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles
Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Reasoning (Psychology) Logic.'
Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.
You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.
Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.
Stenning, Keith, and Michiel van Lambalgen. "Reasoning, logic, and psychology." Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 2, no. 5 (2010): 555–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.134.
Full textOaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. "New Paradigms in the Psychology of Reasoning." Annual Review of Psychology 71, no. 1 (2020): 305–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-051132.
Full textTerrell, Dudley J., and J. M. Johnston. "Logic, Reasoning, and Verbal Behavior." Behavior Analyst 12, no. 1 (1989): 35–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf03392475.
Full textHobson, J. Allan, Suchada Sangsanguan, Henry Arantes, and David Kahn. "Dream logic—The inferential reasoning paradigm." Dreaming 21, no. 1 (2011): 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022860.
Full textAune, Bruce. "Formal logic and practical reasoning." Theory and Decision 20, no. 3 (1986): 301–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00134043.
Full textAllott, Nicholas, and Hiroyuki Uchida. "Classical logic, conditionals and “nonmonotonic” reasoning." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000296.
Full textWidlok, Thomas, and Keith Stenning. "Seeking Common Cause between Cognitive Science and Ethnography: Alternative Logic in Cooperative Action." Journal of Cognition and Culture 18, no. 1-2 (2018): 1–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340027.
Full textLegrenzi, Paolo, and Maria Sonino Legrenzi. "Reasoning and Social Psychology : From Mental Logic To a Perspective Approach." Intellectica. Revue de l'Association pour la Recherche Cognitive 11, no. 1 (1991): 53–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.3406/intel.1991.1378.
Full textKlaczynski, Paul A., and Wejdan Felmban. "Effects of Thinking Dispositions, General Ability, Numeracy, and Instructional Set on Judgments and Decision-Making." Psychological Reports 123, no. 2 (2018): 341–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0033294118806473.
Full textOaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. "The uncertain reasoner: Bayes, logic, and rationality." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 105–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x0900051x.
Full textDe Neys, Wim, and Gordon Pennycook. "Logic, Fast and Slow: Advances in Dual-Process Theorizing." Current Directions in Psychological Science 28, no. 5 (2019): 503–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721419855658.
Full textPfeifer, Niki, and Gernot D. Kleiter. "Mental probability logic." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 98–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000442.
Full textBall, Linden J., Peter Phillips, Caroline N. Wade, and Jeremy D. Quayle. "Effects of Belief and Logic on Syllogistic Reasoning." Experimental Psychology 53, no. 1 (2006): 77–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.77.
Full textJohnson-Laird, P. N. "Peirce, logic diagrams, and the elementary operations of reasoning." Thinking & Reasoning 8, no. 1 (2002): 69–95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780143000099.
Full textNeys, Wim De. "Dual Processing in Reasoning." Psychological Science 17, no. 5 (2006): 428–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01723.x.
Full textUrbański, Mariusz. "Formal modeling of human reasoning: errors, limitations and Baconian bees." Logical Investigations 26, no. 2 (2020): 106–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.21146/2074-1472-2020-26-2-106-115.
Full textStryker, Robin, Danielle Docka-Filipek, and Pamela Wald. "Employment Discrimination Law and Industrial Psychology: Social Science as Social Authority and the Co-Production of Law and Science." Law & Social Inquiry 37, no. 04 (2012): 777–814. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-4469.2011.01277.x.
Full textCollins, Allan, and Ryszard Michalski. "The Logic of Plausible Reasoning: A Core Theory." Cognitive Science 13, no. 1 (1989): 1–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1301_1.
Full textStalder, Daniel R. "Does Logic Moderate the Fundamental Attribution Error?" Psychological Reports 86, no. 3 (2000): 879–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3.879.
Full textBeller, Sieghard, and Hans Spada. "The logic of content effects in propositional reasoning: The case ofconditional reasoning with a point of view." Thinking & Reasoning 9, no. 4 (2003): 335–78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780342000007.
Full textBlanchette, I., and A. Richards. "Reasoning About Emotional and Neutral Materials: Is Logic Affected by Emotion?" Psychological Science 15, no. 11 (2004): 745–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00751.x.
Full textBennett, Bruce M., Donald D. Hoffman, and Paresh Murthy. "Lebesgue Logic for Probabilistic Reasoning and Some Applications to Perception." Journal of Mathematical Psychology 37, no. 1 (1993): 63–103. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmps.1993.1004.
Full textOaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. "Précis ofBayesian Rationality: The Probabilistic Approach to Human Reasoning." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 69–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000284.
Full textFerrigno, Stephen, Yiyun Huang, and Jessica F. Cantlon. "Reasoning Through the Disjunctive Syllogism in Monkeys." Psychological Science 32, no. 2 (2021): 292–300. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797620971653.
Full textEvans, Jonathan St B. T. "Logic and human reasoning: An assessment of the deduction paradigm." Psychological Bulletin 128, no. 6 (2002): 978–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.978.
Full textGirotto, Vittorio, Paul Light, and Christopher Colbourn. "Pragmatic Schemas and Conditional Reasoning in Children." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 40, no. 3 (1988): 469–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02724988843000023.
Full textMarkovits, Henry, and Fabien Savary. "Pragmatic Schemas and the Selection Task: To Reason or Not to Reason." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 45, no. 1 (1992): 133–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640749208401319.
Full textForcada, Miquel. "IBN BĀJJA ONTAṢAWWURANDTAṢDĪQ: SCIENCE AND PSYCHOLOGY". Arabic Sciences and Philosophy 24, № 1 (2014): 103–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0957423913000106.
Full textDenison, Stephanie, and Fei Xu. "Infant Statisticians: The Origins of Reasoning Under Uncertainty." Perspectives on Psychological Science 14, no. 4 (2019): 499–509. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1745691619847201.
Full textStenning, Keith, and Jon Oberlander. "A Cognitive Theory of Graphical and Linguistic Reasoning: Logic and Implementation." Cognitive Science 19, no. 1 (1995): 97–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1901_3.
Full textSokolov, I. A. "Theory and practice in artificial intelligence." Вестник Российской академии наук 89, no. 4 (2019): 365–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.31857/s0869-5873894365-370.
Full textvan den Daele, Leland. "Cognitive Development of the Binary Operations for Standard and Nonstandard Logic." Perceptual and Motor Skills 83, no. 3_suppl (1996): 1271–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1996.83.3f.1271.
Full textKhalil, Elias L. "Are stomachs rational?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 91–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000375.
Full textBlanchette, Isabelle. "The effect of emotion on interpretation and logic in a conditional reasoning task." Memory & Cognition 34, no. 5 (2006): 1112–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193257.
Full textBarrouillet, Pierre, and Henry Markovits. "Is the self-organizing consciousness framework compatible with human deductive reasoning?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, no. 3 (2002): 330–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02220063.
Full textStupple, Edward J. N., and Linden J. Ball. "Belief–logic conflict resolution in syllogistic reasoning: Inspection-time evidence for a parallel-process model." Thinking & Reasoning 14, no. 2 (2008): 168–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546780701739782.
Full textOaksford, Mike, and Nick Chater. "Probabilities, causation, and logic programming in conditional reasoning: reply to Stenning and Van Lambalgen (2016)." Thinking & Reasoning 22, no. 3 (2016): 336–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2016.1139505.
Full textBuchtel, Emma E., and Ara Norenzayan. "Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning." Asian Journal of Social Psychology 11, no. 4 (2008): 264–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839x.2008.00266.x.
Full textCOLBERG, MAGDA. "LOGIC-BASED MEASUREMENT OF VERBAL REASONING: A KEY TO INCREASED VALIDITY AND ECONOMY." Personnel Psychology 38, no. 2 (1985): 347–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1985.tb00552.x.
Full textStenning, Keith, and Michiel van Lambalgen. "Logic programming, probability, and two-system accounts of reasoning: a rejoinder to Oaksford and Chater (2014)." Thinking & Reasoning 22, no. 3 (2016): 355–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2016.1139504.
Full textEvans, Jonathan St B. T. "Does rational analysis stand up to rational analysis?" Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 88–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000338.
Full textWeinstein, Mark Leonard, and Dan Fisherman. "on the relevance of cognitive neuroscience for community of inquiry." childhood & philosophy 15 (January 30, 2019): 01–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.12957/childphilo.2019.37513.
Full textBraunstein, Myron L. "A better understanding of inference can reconcile constructivist and direct theories." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25, no. 1 (2002): 99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x02240029.
Full textDaniel, David B., and Paul A. Klaczynski. "Developmental and Individual Differences in Conditional Reasoning: Effects of Logic Instructions and Alternative Antecedents." Child Development 77, no. 2 (2006): 339–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00874.x.
Full textSchroyens, Walter. "On is an ought: Levels of analysis and the descriptive versus normative analysis of human reasoning." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 101–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000478.
Full textDe Neys, Wim. "Beyond response output: More logical than we think." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32, no. 1 (2009): 87–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x09000326.
Full textStupple, Edward J. N., Linden J. Ball, Jonathan St B. T. Evans, and Emily Kamal-Smith. "When logic and belief collide: Individual differences in reasoning times support a selective processing model." Journal of Cognitive Psychology 23, no. 8 (2011): 931–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.589381.
Full textStupple, Edward J. N., and Eleanor F. Waterhouse. "Short article: Negations in syllogistic reasoning: Evidence for a heuristic–analytic conflict." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62, no. 8 (2009): 1533–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470210902785674.
Full textDe Neys, Wim, and Elke Van Gelder. "Logic and belief across the lifespan: the rise and fall of belief inhibition during syllogistic reasoning." Developmental Science 12, no. 1 (2009): 123–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00746.x.
Full textSt.B.T. Evans, Jonathan, John Clibbens, and Benjamin Rood. "Bias in Conditional Inference: Implications for Mental Models and Mental Logic." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 48, no. 3 (1995): 644–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640749508401409.
Full text