To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Rejection (Psychology).

Journal articles on the topic 'Rejection (Psychology)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Rejection (Psychology).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Villar, R. "The psychology of rejection." Journal of Hip Preservation Surgery 2, no. 1 (January 1, 2015): 1–2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jhps/hnv008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Zhang, Ming, Manuela Barreto, and David Doyle. "Stigma-Based Rejection Experiences Affect Trust in Others." Social Psychological and Personality Science 11, no. 3 (March 27, 2019): 308–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550619829057.

Full text
Abstract:
Rejection experiences are likely to influence individuals’ subsequent feelings about others and their behavior in social interactions. The present study specifically examined whether stigma-based rejection leads to decreased trust in others, compared to rejections that are not stigma based. Trust was assessed behaviorally with an online task where the interaction partner was preprogrammed. Participants showed less trust after stigma-based rejection than after a nonstigma-based rejection. This research provides the first experimental evidence that stigma-based rejection uniquely influences trust in others.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Judah, Matt R., Nathan M. Hager, Alicia L. Milam, Gabrielle Ramsey-Wilson, Hannah C. Hamrick, and Tiphanie G. Sutton. "Out of Sight, Still in Mind: The Consequences of Nonfoveal Viewing of Emotional Faces in Social Anxiety." Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 41, no. 6 (December 2022): 578–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2022.41.6.578.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: Anxiety sensitivity social concerns (ASSC) is a risk factor for social anxiety disorder that may motivate avoidance of eye contact (i.e., gaze avoidance), thereby maintaining anxiety. Gaze avoidance displaces socially relevant stimuli (e.g., faces) from foveal (i.e., center) vision, possibly reducing visual sensation of faces and giving an opportunity to misperceive others as rejecting. Methods: We tested the effects of non-foveal viewing on perceiving faces as rejecting, whether there is an indirect effect of ASSC on state anxiety explained by perceived rejection, and whether the indirect effect depended on non-foveal viewing of faces. Participants (N = 118) viewed faces presented within foveal and non-foveal positions and rated how rejecting each face appeared to be, followed by ratings of their own state anxiety. Results: ASSC was associated with perceiving faces as rejecting regardless of face position. There was an indirect effect of ASSC on state anxiety ratings that was explained by perceived rejection, but only in the non-foveal positions. The indirect effect was due to an association between perceived rejection and state anxiety that was only present when faces were viewed in non-foveal vision. Discussion: The findings suggest ASSC may maintain state anxiety partially through the perceived rejection someone experiences while avoiding the gaze of others. This study supports cognitive theories of social anxiety and encourages cognitive-behavioral interventions for gaze avoidance in people with social anxiety disorder.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

WILLIAMS, MEREDITH. "Wittgenstein' s Rejection of Scientific Psychology." Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 15, no. 2 (July 1985): 203–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1985.tb00052.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Colombo, Lucia, Eraldo Nicotra, and Barbara Marino. "Preference reversal in decision making: The attraction effect in choice and rejection 1The research was funded by a 60% grant of the University of Padua. The authors wish to thank Sandro Bettella for his invaluable technical assistance." Swiss Journal of Psychology 61, no. 1 (March 2002): 21–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1024//1421-0185.61.1.21.

Full text
Abstract:
When people express a preference between two alternatives A and B in terms of a positive choice of one option, this can exceed in strength the same preference expressed as a rejection of the alternative. This effect of response mode has been interpreted by Shafir (1993) in terms of response compatibility theory, according to which decision makers display an influence of the compatibility between the type of response (choose/reject) and the positive/negative attributes of the options. In the present study we investigated the influence on response mode (choice/rejection) of the attraction effect, in which a decoy similar to one of two options, but lower in value, modifies the share of the option to which it is similar when added to the original set ( Huber, Payne & Puto, 1982 ; Simonson & Tversky, 1992 ). A decoy negative in value, but similar to one of the alternatives was added to a two-option set, one with a high variation in attributes (enriched) and one with a low variation (impoverished). We investigated whether both choice and rejections were influenced by the decoy’s presence, as compared to the baseline two-option condition, and whether, consequently, the pattern of dominance between choice and rejection was modified. We found a pattern of rejection dominance in the two-option condition (Experiment 1). In Experiment 2, where the attraction effect was investigated, there was an increase in the proportion of choices (and a correspondent decrease in rejections) of the similar alternative, as compared to the original two-option set, only for the option with low variation in the attributes. For the enriched option, rejection, but not choice, was influenced by the presence of the decoy.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

MACNAMARA, JOHN, and John Macnamara. "Cognitive Psychology and the Rejection of Brentano." Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 23, no. 2 (June 1993): 117–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1993.tb00233.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Palmer, David C. "On Skinner’s Rejection of S-R Psychology." Behavior Analyst 21, no. 1 (April 1998): 93–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf03392782.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Cournoyer, David E., and Ronald P. Rohner. "Reliability of Retrospective Reports of Perceived Maternal Acceptance-Rejection in Childhood." Psychological Reports 78, no. 1 (February 1996): 147–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1996.78.1.147.

Full text
Abstract:
Research reported here addresses the issue of the reliability of retrospective reports of children's perceptions of maternal acceptance-rejection as measured by the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire. A sample of 49 middle-class Caucasian 7- to 11-yr.-old children were asked to respond to the questionnaire reflecting on their mothers' current accepting-rejecting behaviors. Seven years later the same children—now adolescents—responded to the same questionnaire with the instruction to reflect back on their mothers' behavior when the youth were about 7 to 11 years of age. None of these youth recalled having been tested seven years earlier. A simple zero-order correlation between scores in childhood and adolescence was .62, indicating that adolescents' retrospective recollections were in moderate agreement with their reports during childhood. Thus, it seems clear that, at least with respect to perceptions of maternal acceptance-rejection as measured by the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire, researchers can have reasonable confidence that adolescents' current recollections about their experiences of maternal acceptance-rejection are likely to be in moderate agreement with what they would have reported had they been tested during childhood.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Pronk, Tila M., and Jaap J. A. Denissen. "A Rejection Mind-Set: Choice Overload in Online Dating." Social Psychological and Personality Science 11, no. 3 (August 21, 2019): 388–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1948550619866189.

Full text
Abstract:
The paradox of modern dating is that online platforms provide more opportunities to find a romantic partner than ever before, but people are nevertheless more likely to be single. We hypothesized the existence of a rejection mind-set: The continued access to virtually unlimited potential partners makes people more pessimistic and rejecting. Across three studies, participants immediately started to reject more hypothetical and actual partners when dating online, cumulating on average in a decrease of 27% in chance on acceptance from the first to the last partner option. This was explained by an overall decline in satisfaction with pictures and perceived dating success. For women, the rejection mind-set also resulted in a decreasing likelihood of having romantic matches. Our findings suggest that people gradually “close off” from mating opportunities when online dating.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Park, Lora E., and Melissa J. Harwin. "Visible versus non-visible rejection: Consequences of appearance-based rejection sensitivity." Journal of Research in Personality 44, no. 1 (February 2010): 128–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.10.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Pizarro-Campagna, Elizabeth, Gill Terrett, Martina Jovev, Peter G. Rendell, Julie D. Henry, and Andrew M. Chanen. "Cognitive Reappraisal Impairs Negative Affect Regulation in the Context of Social Rejection for Youth With Early-Stage Borderline Personality Disorder." Journal of Personality Disorders 37, no. 2 (April 2023): 156–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2023.37.2.156.

Full text
Abstract:
Application of emotion regulation strategies might be susceptible to the context of social rejection for individuals with borderline personality disorder (BPD). This study compared the ability of 27 outpatient youths (15-25 years old) with early-stage BPD and 37 healthy controls (HC) to apply expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal in standard and socially rejecting laboratory contexts. BPD youths were largely as able as HCs to regulate negative affect across instruction and contexts. However, cognitive reappraisal in the context of social rejection heightened BPD negative facial expression relative to HCs. Thus, while BPD emotion regulation ability was largely normative, cognitive reappraisal might be ineffective in the context of social rejection for this group, with social rejection acting as an accelerant that heightens the expression of negative affect. Given the common experience of perceived and actual social rejection for this group, clinicians should carefully consider treatments that include cognitive reappraisal strategies because they might be contraindicated.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Murphy, Anna M., and Gemma Russell. "Rejection Sensitivity, Jealousy, and the Relationship to Interpersonal Aggression." Journal of Interpersonal Violence 33, no. 13 (January 21, 2016): 2118–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260515622572.

Full text
Abstract:
The development and maintenance of interpersonal relationships lead individuals to risk rejection in the pursuit of acceptance. Some individuals are predisposed to experience a hypersensitivity to rejection that is hypothesized to be related to jealous and aggressive reactions within interpersonal relationships. The current study used convenience sampling to recruit 247 young adults to evaluate the relationship between rejection sensitivity, jealousy, and aggression. A mediation model was used to test three hypotheses: Higher scores of rejection sensitivity would be positively correlated to higher scores of aggression (Hypothesis 1); higher scores of rejection sensitivity would be positively correlated to higher scores of jealousy (Hypothesis 2); jealousy would mediate the relationship between rejection sensitivity and aggression (Hypothesis 3). Study results suggest a tendency for individuals with high rejection sensitivity to experience higher levels of jealousy, and subsequently have a greater propensity for aggression, than individuals with low rejection sensitivity. Future research that substantiates a link between hypersensitivity to rejection, jealousy, and aggression may provide an avenue for prevention, education, or intervention in reducing aggression within interpersonal relationships.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Mayo, Ruth. "Knowledge and Distrust May Go a Long Way in the Battle With Disinformation: Mental Processes of Spontaneous Disbelief." Current Directions in Psychological Science 28, no. 4 (June 6, 2019): 409–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721419847998.

Full text
Abstract:
A common claim is that people have an easier time accepting information than rejecting it, resulting in gullibility. In this article, I review empirical research demonstrating how the human mind is equipped with successful and spontaneous rejection processes that may protect us from disinformation.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Leary, Mark R., Jean M. Twenge, and Erin Quinlivan. "Interpersonal Rejection as a Determinant of Anger and Aggression." Personality and Social Psychology Review 10, no. 2 (May 2006): 111–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_2.

Full text
Abstract:
This article reviews the literature on the relationship between interpersonal rejection and aggression. Four bodies of research are summarized: laboratory experiments that manipulate rejection, rejection among adults in everyday life, rejection in childhood, and individual differences that may moderate the relationship. The theoretical mechanisms behind the effect are then explored. Possible explanations for why rejection leads to anger and aggression include: rejection as a source of pain, rejection as a source of frustration, rejection as a threat to self-esteem, mood improvementfollowing aggression, aggression as social influence, aggression as a means of reestablishing control, retribution, disinhibition, and loss of self-control.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Stratmoen, Evelyn, Emilio D. Rivera, and Donald A. Saucier. "“Sorry, I already have a boyfriend”: Masculine honor beliefs and perceptions of women’s use of deceptive rejection behaviors to avert unwanted romantic advances." Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 37, no. 2 (August 7, 2019): 467–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407519865615.

Full text
Abstract:
We examined the relationships between masculine honor beliefs (MHB) and women’s endorsement of various rejection-related behaviors, as well as both men’s and women’s perceptions of men’s aggressive responses after being romantically rejected by a woman who uses an avoidant/deceptive rejection technique. In Study 1, women with stronger MHB were more likely to endorse their own use of an avoidant/deceptive rejection technique and expressed fewer expectations of men aggressing against them after their overt rejection. In Study 2, men with stronger MHB perceived a woman’s use of deception to reject a man’s unwanted romantic advance as a greater threat to the rejected man’s honor, while women with stronger MHB expressed greater expectations of retaliatory aggression from the rejected man, regardless of the use of deception. These results suggest women who adhere to masculine honor norms may be in a difficult predicament when faced with rejecting men and may choose to mitigate the honor threat to the rejected man by using avoidance/deception to avert his unwanted romantic advance to avoid potential retaliatory aggression.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Breines, Juliana G., and Ozlem Ayduk. "Rejection Sensitivity and Vulnerability to Self-Directed Hostile Cognitions Following Rejection." Journal of Personality 83, no. 1 (December 20, 2013): 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12077.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Liu, Shen, Lin Zhang, Qun Tan, Yanlin Zhao, and Qiang Xu. "Attentional characteristics of Chinese college students receiving social threat cues in rejection situations." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 45, no. 8 (September 13, 2017): 1293–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.6234.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigated the attentional characteristics of 98 Chinese college students when they received social threat cues in explicit and ambiguous rejection situations, and further examined the moderating effect of degree of rejection sensitivity. Participants were instructed to play an interactive game in pairs, after which they completed the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire for College Students and, finally, a dot-probe task. The results showed that all participants had an attentional bias toward social rejection cues in both social rejection and general situations. In the ambiguous rejection situation, highly rejection-sensitive individuals showed attentional bias and tended to avoid social threat cues and nonsocial negative cues. Degree of rejection sensitivity moderated the relationship of ambiguous rejection, influencing individuals' attentional processing of threat cues. We sought to develop some specific interventions that could be used to alert highly rejection-sensitive college students to the characteristics of the attentional processing strategies they use for social avoidance.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Vitz, Paul C., and Evelyn Birge Vitz. "Pretentious, Preposterous, Pathetic: A Response to Susan Henking and to Mary Daly on Psychology and Religion." Journal of Psychology and Theology 21, no. 3 (September 1993): 210–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009164719302100303.

Full text
Abstract:
This response provides critique both of Mary Daly's work and of Henking's (1993) treatment of Daly's material. Critique of Daly's works focuses on the inappropriate use of psychological concepts and the rejecting and paranoid attitudes she displays toward religion, society, men, and women. Although Daly's works appear to be founded in psychology, she applies terms meant to describe intrapersonal psychological phenomena (e.g., projection, introjection) to entire religious and ideological systems. Daly advocates dissolution of psychology as patriarchal, but does so using terms borrowed from Freud, Jung, Maslow (i.e., the patriarchs of psychology). Rather than reconstructing psychology, her own pathological hatred is evidenced in splitting (Good Women, Bad Men) and paranoia. The solution Daly espouses is to annihilate men and despise women. Henking's (1993) treatment, though quoting extensively from Daly, reduces the material to bland, footnoted academic prose. She thereby misses communication of Daly's willful rejection of reason.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Harman, Gilbert. "No Character or Personality." Business Ethics Quarterly 13, no. 1 (January 2003): 87–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.5840/beq20031316.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract:Solomon argues that, although recent research in social psychology has important implications for business ethics, it does not undermine an approach that stresses virtue ethics. However, he underestimates the empirical threat to virtue ethics, and his a priori claim that empirical research cannot overturn our ordinary moral psychology is overstated. His appeal to seemingly obvious differences in character traits between people simply illustrates the fundamental attribution error. His suggestion that the Milgram and Darley and Batson experiments have to do with such character traits as obedience and punctuality cannot help to explain the relevant differences in the way people behave in different situations. His appeal to personality theory fails, because, as an intellectual academic discipline, personality theory is in shambles, mainly because it has been concerned with conceptions of personality rather than with what is true about personality. Solomon’s rejection of Doris’s claims about the fragmentation of character is at odds with the received view in social psychology. Finally, he is mistaken to think that rejecting virtue ethics implies rejecting free will and moral responsibility.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Betts, Kevin R., and Verlin B. Hinsz. "The (Modified) In Game: An Experimental Paradigm for Manipulating Interpersonal Rejection." Small Group Research 48, no. 3 (January 11, 2017): 311–35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1046496416687374.

Full text
Abstract:
We offer a unique paradigm for manipulating interpersonal rejection that models the complexities of real-life rejection, allows for significant participant immersion in the rejection experience, and requires limited materials. Our approach modifies an existing paradigm, the In Game, to encourage the development of coalitions whereby two individuals are included and one is rejected. The original In Game was developed to reflect real-life situations that involve the exchange of resources or use of power, legitimacy, and obligation. We fuse these elements with new features involving coalition formation to produce a powerful rejection manipulation. Results demonstrate that the modified In Game effectively and reliably produces coalitions that result in one randomly assigned individual being rejected. Moreover, rejection in this paradigm is linked to thwarted psychological need fulfillment and reduced positive affect. Implications of this unique paradigm for studying interpersonal rejection are considered along with suggestions for its successful implementation in other research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Lewandowsky, Stephan, and Klaus Oberauer. "Motivated Rejection of Science." Current Directions in Psychological Science 25, no. 4 (August 2016): 217–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721416654436.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Rohner, Ronald P. "The Parental "Acceptance-Rejection Syndrome": Universal Correlates of Perceived Rejection." American Psychologist 59, no. 8 (November 2004): 830–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.59.8.830.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Gerber, Jonathan, and Ladd Wheeler. "On Being Rejected: A Meta-Analysis of Experimental Research on Rejection." Perspectives on Psychological Science 4, no. 5 (September 2009): 468–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2009.01158.x.

Full text
Abstract:
This article presents the first meta-analysis of experimental research on rejection, sampling 88 studies. The results are consistent with a needs account, which states that rejection frustrates basic psychological needs, but not with a numbness account, which states that rejection causes physical and emotional numbness. Rejection moderately lowers mood (d = −0.50) and self-esteem (d = −0.70), but does not decrease arousal or flatten affect. Both belonging (d = 0.69) and control (d = 1.16) are frustrated by rejection. Aggressive responses to rejection, considered paradoxical by some, appear to be due to attempts to gain control; measures that contrast belonging and control (d = −1.17) cause antisocial responding, whereas measures that do not allow for control to be restored cause prosocial responding (d = 1.21). These findings suggest that rejection makes individuals feel bad—ready to act to restore control or belonging—and that they will prioritize restoring control even if it requires being antisocial.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Farrell, Lara J., Eva C. Gregertsen, Caroline L. Donovan, Amy Pammenter, and Melanie Zimmer-Gembeck. "Maternal Rejection and Idealized Value of Appearance: Exploring the Origins of Body Dysmorphic Concerns Among Young Adults." Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 30, no. 3 (2016): 154–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.30.3.154.

Full text
Abstract:
Body dysmorphic disorder is a clinical disorder characterized by a preoccupation with an imagined or exaggerated defect in one’s appearance (American Psychological Association, 2013), causing impaired functioning. Cognitive-behavioral models of body dysmorphic disorder have been proposed, whereby social anxiety and parental rejection may be predisposing factors, whereas maladaptive cognitive biases, such as appraisals of rejection, may serve as maintenance factors. The primary aim of this study was to test the role that perceived parental rejection in childhood may play in understanding the development of body dysmorphic symptoms. Furthermore, this study examined whether idealized values of appearance act as a mediator between perceived maternal rejection and body dysmorphic symptoms. The sample comprised 239 Australian undergraduate psychology students. Social anxiety, appearance-based rejection sensitivity, maternal rejection, and idealized values of appearance uniquely predicted body dysmorphic symptoms. Furthermore, the relationship between maternal rejection and body dysmorphic symptoms was partially mediated by idealized values of appearance. Findings support cognitive-behavioral models of body dysmorphic disorder.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Rajecki, D. W. "From Acceptance to Rejection: Food Contamination in the Classroom." Teaching of Psychology 16, no. 1 (February 1989): 16–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top1601_4.

Full text
Abstract:
This demonstration allows to experience and measure a shift from food acceptance to food rejection, based on the phenomenon of contamination sensitivity to disgusting substances. Students rate the desirability of a snack, which is then progressively contaminated by contact with, human residues. Repeated measurements, show progressive contamination. The students' ratings reveal quick and statistically reliable shifts from accepting (Liking) to rejecting (disliking) the snack. This exercise generates meaningful data sets and is recommended for several courses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Nascimento, Teresa Maria, and Mauro Bianchi. "Does TB stigma affect emotion recognition? A study with a Portuguese sample." PSICOLOGIA 35, no. 1 (March 11, 2021): 35–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.17575/psicologia.v35i1.1587.

Full text
Abstract:
TB stigma constitutes a major barrier to disease control and social distress. This study aims to understand better the inherent social processes on the perception of emotions. Thus far, the specific role of TB stigma in this respect was not tested. We hypothesize that individuals in TB treatment (vs. a control non-clinical group) would identify more negative emotions in the faces of others, specifically rejecting emotions (e.g., disgust) when preoccupied with TB stigma. Two groups of participants completed a questionnaire with 23 faces, identified the emotions portrayed in the pictures, and reported their level of Stigma Consciousness, Interpersonal-Rejection Related to Stigma, and Rejection Sensitivity. Results show that the two groups significantly differ in their perceptions of negative emotions: participants in treatment identify less disgust and more sadness, fear, and anger versus the control group. Findings are discussed concerning the literature on stigma and its consequences.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Stilwell, Brad T., and Shaun P. Vecera. "Cued distractor rejection disrupts learned distractor rejection." Visual Cognition 27, no. 3-4 (January 30, 2019): 327–42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2018.1564808.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Sultana, Momtaz, and Muhammad Kamal Uddin. "Parental Rejection, Depression, and Internet Addiction Among Young Adults." Pakistan Journal of Psychological Research 34, no. 2 (July 19, 2019): 419–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.33824/pjpr.2019.34.2.23.

Full text
Abstract:
The present study tested whether relation between parental rejection and internet addiction was mediated through depression. Two hundred and thirty university students, ranging in age from 18 to 25 years, responded to the Adult versions of the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire for fathers and for mothers (Uddin & Aktar, 2015), Beck Depression Inventory (Uddin, 2013), and Internet Addiction Test (Uddin & Sultana, 2016), along with a Personal Information Form. Results of independent sample t-test indicated significant gender differences in paternal rejection only. Bivariate correlations showed that parental rejection was positively associated with depression and internet addiction. Multiple regression analysis revealed that mother and father rejection jointly explained about 26% variance in depression and 21% variance in internet addiction of both female and male university students. Mediation analysis showed that depression partially mediated the relation between parental rejection and internet addiction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Bartholomeu, Daniel, and José Maria Montiel. "Behavioral and Personality Predictors of Acceptance and Rejection in University." Paidéia (Ribeirão Preto) 27, no. 68 (December 2017): 272–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-43272768201704.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract: Acceptance and rejection in the group are related to both personality characteristics and social skills and most studies focus on children instead of college students. The objective of this study was to investigate whether acceptance and rejection would be more associated with personality tendencies, specifically socialization or social skills. We collected data from 187 college students attending the Physical Education (67%) and Psychology (32%) courses. The instruments were the sociometric test, the Factorial Scale of Socialization and the Social Skills Inventory. A moderating effect of gender in the relationship between assertiveness and acceptance and rejection to go out on college was observed. Social skills were better predictors of acceptance and social rejection in the university group.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Erozkan, Atilgan. "Rejection sensitivity levels with respect to attachment styles, gender, and parenting styles: A study with Turkish students." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 37, no. 1 (February 1, 2009): 1–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2009.37.1.1.

Full text
Abstract:
The effects of attachment styles of a group of university students on their rejection sensitivity levels in relation to gender, and parenting styles were investigated. Instruments used were the Rejection Sensitivity Questionnaire (Downey & Feldman, 1996), and the Relationship Scales Questionnaire (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994). To analyze data, F statistics, t test, regression analysis, and correlation analysis were employed. Rejection sensitivity levels of female students who had fearful attachment styles and of students who experienced authoritarian parenting styles were found to be significantly higher than those of others. Effect of attachment styles on rejection sensitivity and relationship between rejection sensitivity and attachment styles were both found to be significant.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Clouse, Bonnidell. "“Can Two Walk Together, Except They be Agreed?”: Psychology and Theology — a Journey Together or Paths Apart?" Journal of Psychology and Theology 25, no. 1 (March 1997): 38–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009164719702500104.

Full text
Abstract:
The integration of psychology and theology is likened to a journey in which two people share their ideas. Sometimes there is agreement and the two walk the same path; sometimes no agreement is possible and the two must go their separate ways. The psychologies of psychoanalysis, learning psychology (behaviorism), cognitive psychology, and humanistic psychology are evaluated using Scripture as the criterion for acceptance or rejection of basic ideas and methodology.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Bruce, Darryl. "Lashley's rejection of connectionism." History of Psychology 1, no. 2 (1998): 160–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1093-4510.1.2.160.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Erkan, Semra, and Mehmet Toran. "Child acceptance-rejection behaviors of lower and upper socioeconomic status mothers." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 38, no. 3 (April 1, 2010): 427–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2010.38.3.427.

Full text
Abstract:
In this study we sought to show the difference between the child acceptance-rejection behaviors of lower and upper socioeconomic status mothers, and establish whether or not their acceptance-rejection behaviors were affected by their age, and the number and sex of their children. A total of 246 mothers living in Diyarbakır, 123 with lower and 123 with upper socioeconomic status, was enrolled in the study. Data were gathered by using a personal information form and the Parental Acceptance-Rejection Questionnaire-Mother Form (Rohner, Saavedra, & Granum, 1980). The results revealed a significant difference between lower and upper socioeconomic status mothers' child acceptance-rejection behaviors (t = 7.709; p < 0.05). While the mothers' age (r = .202; p < 0.05) and number of children (r = .238; p < 0.01) were positively correlated to their acceptance-rejection behaviors, their children's sex did not make a significant difference to the mothers' acceptance-rejection behaviors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Graupmann, Verena, Michaela Pfundmair, Peter Matsoukas, and Ralph Erber. "Rejection Via Video." Social Psychology 47, no. 6 (November 2016): 345–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000283.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. This study introduces an easy-to-implement, controlled, vivid, and functional rejection paradigm. Participants empathized with the protagonist of a video who was rejected or accepted individually or as part of a group. In the rejection condition, more perceived exclusion and lower basic need fulfillment were reported. The paradigm also induced nuance in situational factors: Observing somebody being rejected as part of a group led to less pronounced reactions than individual rejection. The video-based rejection paradigm taps into the less studied area of group rejection and offers a new method to test established and novel theoretical predictions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Gyurak, Anett, and Özlem Ayduk. "Defensive Physiological Reactions to Rejection." Psychological Science 18, no. 10 (October 2007): 886–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01996.x.

Full text
Abstract:
We examined the hypothesis that rejection automatically elicits defensive physiological reactions in people with low self-esteem (SE) but that attentional control moderates this effect. Undergraduates ( N = 67) completed questionnaire measures of SE and attentional control. Their eye-blink responses to startle probes were measured while they viewed paintings related to rejection and acceptance themes. The stimuli also included positive-, negative-, and neutral-valence control paintings unrelated to rejection. As predicted, compared with people high in SE, those low in SE showed stronger startle eye-blink responses to paintings related to rejection, but not to negative paintings. Paintings related to acceptance did not attenuate their physiological reactivity. Furthermore, attentional control moderated their sensitivity to rejection, such that low SE was related to greater eye-blink responses to rejection only among individuals who were low in attentional control. Implications of the role of attentional control as a top-down process regulating emotional reactivity in people with low SE are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Cull, Matthew. "Can the ‘Theory of Mind’ Hypothesis Survive, Given Theoretical Insights Derived from the Study of Autism? A Response to Hacking and McGeer." KRITERION – Journal of Philosophy 1, no. 28 (January 1, 2014): 45–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/krt-2014-012805.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract In this paper I agree with both Ian Hacking and Victoria McGeer that the ‘Theory of Mind’ theory (ToM) is fundamentally flawed. However, I find reasons to reject both of their critiques of ToM as incoherent and instead build upon certain parts of McGeer’s work to develop my own rejection of ToM. I end by suggesting routes this rejection might take the philosophy of psychology down
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Godleski, Stephanie A., Rina D. Eiden, Lorig Kachadourian, and Joseph F. Lucke. "Etiological Pathways to Rejection Sensitivity in a High-Risk Sample." Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 45, no. 5 (September 21, 2018): 715–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167218795486.

Full text
Abstract:
Rejection sensitivity is associated with social–emotional maladjustment in both childhood and adulthood. However, less is known about the etiology of rejection sensitivity. The present study tests an etiological model for rejection sensitivity using a high-risk sample ( N = 227) with prospective data from infancy (i.e., 12 months) to adolescence (i.e., eighth grade). Evidence for social learning and attachment theories was demonstrated. In particular, family and parenting factors, such as family conflict and maternal harshness, were predictive of rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Implications for intervention and prevention efforts are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Gunther Moor, Bregtje, Eveline A. Crone, and Maurits W. van der Molen. "The Heartbrake of Social Rejection." Psychological Science 21, no. 9 (August 9, 2010): 1326–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610379236.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

French, Doran C. "The Complexities of Peer Rejection." Contemporary Psychology: A Journal of Reviews 37, no. 12 (December 1992): 1275–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/031692.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Hafner, Nuala, Christopher A. Pepping, and Eleanor H. Wertheim. "Dispositional mindfulness, rejection sensitivity, and behavioural responses to rejection: The role of emotion regulation." Australian Journal of Psychology 71, no. 2 (September 25, 2018): 163–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12224.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Rosenbach, Charlotte, and Babette Renneberg. "Rejection sensitivity as a mediator of the relationship between experienced rejection and borderline characteristics." Personality and Individual Differences 69 (October 2014): 176–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.05.032.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Khaleque, Abdul, Muhammad Kamal Uddin, Kazi Nur Hossain, Md Nur-E.-Alam Siddique, and Anjuman Shirin. "Perceived Parental Acceptance–Rejection in Childhood Predict Psychological Adjustment and Rejection Sensitivity in Adulthood." Psychological Studies 64, no. 4 (August 6, 2019): 447–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12646-019-00508-z.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Bobowik, Magdalena, Borja Martinovic, Nekane Basabe, Lisa S. Barsties, and Gusta Wachter. "‘Healthy’ identities? Revisiting rejection-identification and rejection-disidentification models among voluntary and forced immigrants." European Journal of Social Psychology 47, no. 7 (September 28, 2017): 818–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2306.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Misic, Maja, Jelisaveta Todorovic, and Andjelija Petrovic. "THE ROLE OF COMPLEX POSTTRAUMATIC STRESS DISORDER AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN ACCEPTING-REJECTING DIMENSION OF PARENTING OF WOMEN SURVIVORS OF ABUSE." Annual of social work 28, no. 3 (March 11, 2022): 583–614. http://dx.doi.org/10.3935/ljsr.v28i3.368.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACT The research intended to establish a connection between complex posttraumatic stress disorder (C-PTSD) and the accepting/rejecting parenting of mothers, survivors of complex trauma. The goal was also to examine how protective factors, resilience, and perceived social support moderate the effect of C-PTSD on the mother’s rejecting parenting, as well as how parental traumas and their parenting predicts parenting of women survivors. The study results are based on a survey completed by 100 women at the age 19 – 64. The sample had two groups: mothers with C-PTSD and a control group without C-PSTD. The results indicate that complex trauma can predict mother’s parenting rejection. C-PTSD displays correlations with all five dimensions of the negative parenting styles (lackof affection/neglect/aggression/control/undifferentiated rejection). Resilience acted as a moderator between C-PTSD and rejecting parenting. Parental traumas and their rejecting parenting manifest in women’s parenting Key words: C-PTSD, trauma exposure, women, parenting,resilience
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Berenson, Kathy R., Olga Nynaes, Emily S. Wakschal, Laura M. Kapner, and Erin C. Sweeney. "Attributions for Rejection and Acceptance in Young Adults With Borderline and Avoidant Personality Features." Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 37, no. 6 (June 2018): 431–52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2018.37.6.431.

Full text
Abstract:
Individuals with borderline and avoidant personality disorders show interpersonal dysfunction that includes maladaptive responses to rejection and reduced emotional benefits from acceptance. To identify the attributional styles that may underlie these difficulties, we examined causal attributions for rejection and acceptance among undergraduates high in features of each disorder and a healthy comparison group. In Study 1, participants rated how likely they were to attribute hypothetical rejection and acceptance experiences to positive and negative qualities of the self and others, as well as external circumstances. In Study 2, we examined these same attributions in daily diary assessments of real rejection and acceptance experiences. Although the two studies showed some differences in results, they both linked borderline personality features with suspicious, selfbolstering responses and avoidant personality features with perceived inferiority. Distinct attributional styles may contribute to the distinct interpersonal problems characteristic of these conditions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Sohn, David. "Psychology of the Scientist: LXVI. The Idiot Savants Have Taken over the Psychology Labs! or Why in Science Using the Rejection of the Null Hypothesis as the Basis for Affirming the Research Hypothesis is Unwarranted." Psychological Reports 73, no. 3_suppl (December 1993): 1167–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1993.73.3f.1167.

Full text
Abstract:
The fable of a group of idiot savants, who conduct methodologically flawless research to test senseless research hypotheses and are able to affirm some of them on the basis of the rejection of the null hypothesis, is used to dramatize the argument that the rejection of the null hypothesis, by itself, is not sufficient grounds for affirming the research hypothesis. Reasons and examples are given to argue that affirmation of a scientific hypothesis must be based primarily on substantive evidence that is independent of an hypothesis test.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Chen, Zhansheng, Kai-Tak Poon, Michael J. Bernstein, and Fei Teng. "Rejecting another pains the self: The impact of perceived future rejection." Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 50 (January 2014): 225–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.10.007.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Luterek, Jane A., Gerlinde C. Harb, Richard G. Heimberg, and Brian P. Marx. "Interpersonal Rejection Sensitivity in Childhood Sexual Abuse Survivors." Journal of Interpersonal Violence 19, no. 1 (January 2004): 90–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260503259052.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigated whether interpersonal rejection sensitivity serves a mediating role between childhood sexual abuse (CSA) and three long-term psychological correlates of CSA in adult female survivors: depressive symptoms, anger suppression, and attenuated emotional expression. Interpersonal rejection sensitivity has been shown to be a risk factor for the development of depression and is elevated in CSA survivors. Similarly, attenuated emotional expression, particularly anger, has been related to adjustment difficulties in CSA survivors. Participants in this study were 355 female undergraduates, 34 ofwhomreported a history of CSA. Results demonstrated that interpersonal rejection sensitivity mediates the relationship between CSA and later depressive symptoms. Interpersonal rejection sensitivity partially mediated the relationship between CSA and anger suppression; however, it did not mediate the relationship between CSA and attenuated emotional expression. These results are examined within the context of the current literature on adult CSA survivors and their implications are discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Dasborough, Marie T. "Two tales of rejection: Being rejected and rejecting others' (re)views." Journal of Organizational Behavior 40, no. 2 (February 2019): 123–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.2356.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Hepp, Johanna, Sean P. Lane, Ryan W. Carpenter, Inga Niedtfeld, Whitney C. Brown, and Timothy J. Trull. "Interpersonal Problems and Negative Affect in Borderline Personality and Depressive Disorders in Daily Life." Clinical Psychological Science 5, no. 3 (February 13, 2017): 470–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/2167702616677312.

Full text
Abstract:
Theories of Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) suggest that interpersonal problems in BPD act as triggers for negative affect and, at the same time, are a possible result of affective dysregulation. Therefore, we assessed the relations between momentary negative affect (hostility, sadness, fear) and interpersonal problems (rejection, disagreement) in a sample of 80 BPD and 51 depressed outpatients at six time points over 28 days. Data were analyzed using multivariate multilevel modeling to separate momentary-, day-, and person-level effects. Results revealed a mutually reinforcing relationship between disagreement and hostility, rejection and hostility, and rejection and sadness in both groups at the momentary and day level. The mutual reinforcement between hostility and rejection/disagreement was significantly stronger in the BPD group. Moreover, the link between rejection and sadness was present at all three levels of analysis for the BPD group, whereas it was localized to the momentary level in the depressed group.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography