Academic literature on the topic 'Rejection sensitivity'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Rejection sensitivity.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

Dorfman, Anna, Harrison Oakes, and Igor Grossmann. "Rejection Sensitivity Hurts Your Open Mind: Rejection Sensitivity and Wisdom in Workplace Conflicts." Academy of Management Proceedings 2020, no. 1 (August 2020): 20479. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/ambpp.2020.20479abstract.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Richter, Marianne, and Dominik Schoebi. "Rejection Sensitivity in Intimate Relationships." Zeitschrift für Psychologie 229, no. 3 (September 2021): 165–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000448.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract. The goal of the study was to investigate whether and how perceptions of rejection are predictive of perceptions of the partner’s responsiveness, and the intimacy felt with a romantic partner, daily. Moreover, we examined whether people who are more anxious and sensitive to rejection perceived more rejection in daily life and whether this foreshadowed perception of the partner to be less responsive. Analyses of daily data from a sample of 75 couples ( N = 150) who reported on their daily relational experiences suggest that rejection sensitivity and rejection experiences play a significant role in couples’ felt intimacy in daily life, and specifically for perceptions of responsiveness. Results also indicate that for women, rejection sensitivity is associated with more rejection experiences. We discuss the current results from a clinical and from a social psychological perspective, and we highlight how anxious apprehension and experience of rejection, and its interpersonal consequences, can be further considered in clinical practice.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Anna, Li, and Zhao Junyan. "A Review on Rejection Sensitivity." Psychology of China 4, no. 1 (2022): 82–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.35534/pc.0401011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Kross, Ethan, Tobias Egner, Kevin Ochsner, Joy Hirsch, and Geraldine Downey. "Neural Dynamics of Rejection Sensitivity." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19, no. 6 (June 2007): 945–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.945.

Full text
Abstract:
Rejection sensitivity (RS) is the tendency to anxiously expect, readily perceive, and intensely react to rejection. This study used functional magnetic resonance imaging to explore whether individual differences in RS are mediated by differential recruitment of brain regions involved in emotional appraisal and/or cognitive control. High and low RS participants were scanned while viewing either representational paintings depicting themes of rejection and acceptance or nonrepresentational control paintings matched for positive or negative valence, arousal and interest level. Across all participants, rejection versus acceptance images activated regions of the brain involved in processing affective stimuli (posterior cingulate, insula), and cognitive control (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; medial frontal cortex). Low and high RS individuals' responses to rejection versus acceptance images were not, however, identical. Low RS individuals displayed significantly more activity in left inferior and right dorsal frontal regions, and activity in these areas correlated negatively with participants' self-report distress ratings. In addition, control analyses revealed no effect of viewing negative versus positive images in any of the areas described above, suggesting that the aforementioned activations were involved in rejection-relevant processing rather than processing negatively valenced stimuli per se. Taken together, these findings suggest that responses in regions traditionally implicated in emotional processing and cognitive control are sensitive to rejection stimuli irrespective of RS, but that low RS individuals may activate prefrontal structures to regulate distress associated with viewing such images.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

McLachlan, Julie, Melanie J. Zimmer-Gembeck, and Leanne McGregor. "Rejection Sensitivity in Childhood and Early Adolescence: Peer Rejection and Protective Effects of Parents and Friends." Journal of Relationships Research 1, no. 1 (November 1, 2010): 31–40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1375/jrr.1.1.31.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractTheory suggests that rejection sensitivity, a social cognitive processing style characterised by anxious and angry expectations of rejection, develops from experiences of rejection or acceptance by others. The purpose of this study of 417 children and early adolescents (age 9 to 13) was to examine how relationship experiences are directly and interactively associated with their rejection sensitivity. In a multivariate analysis, there was an association of rejection by parents and by peers with rejection sensitivity, with a stronger association between peer rejection and sensitivity than between parent rejection and sensitivity. Regarding interactive effects, peer rejection was found to have a strong association with rejection sensitivity among participants with low or high parent acceptance, and among those with high friendship satisfaction. Yet, there was evidence of a stronger association between peer rejection and rejection sensitivity among those with low parent acceptance or high friendship quality. This was because rejection sensitivity was highest when peer rejection was high and parent acceptance was low, and sensitivity was lowest when peer rejection was low and friendship quality was high. Findings show how young people's relationships in different domains uniquely co-vary with rejection sensitivity and interact in accounting for angry and anxious expectations of rejection by others.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Petereit, Pauline, Sarah Jessen, Tatiana Goregliad Fjaellingsdal, and Ulrike M. Krämer. "Social Context and Rejection Expectations Modulate Neural and Behavioral Responses to Social Feedback." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 34, no. 5 (March 31, 2022): 823–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01829.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract When meeting other people, some are optimistic and expect to be accepted by others, whereas others are pessimistic and expect mostly rejections. How social feedback is evaluated in situations that meet or do not meet these biases and how people differ in their response to rejection and acceptance depending on the social situation are unknown. In this study, participants experienced rejection and acceptance by peers in two different social contexts, one with high (negative context) and the other with low probability of rejection (positive context). We examined how the neural and behavioral responses to rejection are altered by this context and whether it depends on the individual's sensitivity to rejection. Behavioral results show that, on average, people maintain an optimistic bias even when mostly experiencing rejection. Importantly, personality differences in rejection sensitivity affected both prior expectations to be rejected in the paradigm and the extent to which expectations changed during the paradigm. The context also strongly modulated ERPs and theta responses to rejection and acceptance feedback. Specifically, valence effects on neural responses were enhanced in the negative context, suggesting a greater relevance to monitor social feedback in such a situation. Moreover, midfrontal theta predicted how expectations were changed in response to prediction errors, stressing a role for theta in learning from social feedback. Surprisingly, interindividual differences in rejection sensitivity did not affect neural responses to feedback. Our results stress the importance of considering the interaction between subjective expectations and the social context for behavioral and neural responses to social rejection.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Kraines, Morganne A., Lucas J. A. Kelberer, and Tony T. Wells. "Rejection sensitivity, interpersonal rejection, and attention for emotional facial expressions." Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 59 (June 2018): 31–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.11.004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Romero-Canyas, Rainer, Geraldine Downey, Kathy Berenson, Ozlem Ayduk, and N. Jan Kang. "Rejection Sensitivity and the Rejection-Hostility Link in Romantic Relationships." Journal of Personality 78, no. 1 (February 2010): 119–48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00611.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Nesdale, Drew, Melanie J. Zimmer-Gembeck, and Natalie Roxburgh. "Peer Group Rejection in Childhood: Effects of Rejection Ambiguity, Rejection Sensitivity, and Social Acumen." Journal of Social Issues 70, no. 1 (March 2014): 12–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12044.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Downey, Geraldine, Amy Lebolt, Claudia Rincón, and Antonio L. Freitas. "Rejection Sensitivity and Children's Interpersonal Difficulties." Child Development 69, no. 4 (August 1998): 1074–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06161.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

Tsirgielis, Constantina. "Rejection sensitivity in early adolescence." Thesis, University of British Columbia, 2015. http://hdl.handle.net/2429/54187.

Full text
Abstract:
Repeated rejection experiences may encourage a hypersensitivity to rejection stimuli and cues, defined as rejection sensitivity (RS): a dispositional pattern of responding in relationships with defensive expectations of rejection (Downey et al., 1998). Rejection sensitive youth perceive signs of rejection in situations others would consider neutral or ambiguous. Hypersensitivity to social rejection increases anxiety in social interactions where rejection is possible and leads to withdrawal, feelings of loneliness, social anxiety (SA), and depression. RS therefore can be considered a mechanism to explain the development of internalizing disorders such as SA. SA youth are also hypersensitive to cues of rejection, and tend to withdraw from their peer groups to avoid possible rejection, which leads to loneliness, depression, interpersonal problems, and increased SA (Sameroff & MacKenzie, 2003). Previous research reviewed the impact of RS on youth’s social and emotional wellbeing and have identified SA as a potential outcome of RS (Bowker et al., 2011, Marston et al., 2010, McDonald et al., 2010). There is limited research investigating the bidirectional relationship between RS-Anxiety and SA. Self-report data from grade 6 and 7 students (n=128) at a large, urban school district were collected. The goal of this research study was to 1) identify the relationship between RS-anxiety and SA, 2) determine if there are gender differences on the RS-Anxiety, and SA measures 3) investigate whether emotional symptoms influenced the relationship between RS-Anxiety and SA and 3) determine whether RS-Anxiety and SA predict peer problems in youth. Analyses performed include a bivariate correlational analysis, two-tailed independent samples t-test, a mediation analysis, and a hierarchical multiple regression. Results from the analyses revealed that there is a positive correlation between RS-Anxiety and SA, and emotional symptoms significantly mediated that relationship. SA also significantly predicted peer problems in RS-Anxious youth. However, no gender difference was found on the SA or RS-Anxiety measures.
Education, Faculty of
Educational and Counselling Psychology, and Special Education (ECPS), Department of
Graduate
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Bernstein, Michael J. "Rejection and Pain Sensitivity: Why Rejection Sometimes Hurts and Sometimes Numbs." Miami University / OhioLINK, 2010. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc_num=miami1269914149.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Bernstein, Michael Jason. "Rejection and pain sensitivity why rejection sometimes hurts and sometimes numbs /." Oxford, Ohio : Miami University, 2010. http://rave.ohiolink.edu/etdc/view?acc%5Fnum=miami1269914149.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Webb, Haley Jean. "The Role of Friends in Adolescent Appearance-Based Rejection Sensitivity: An Exploration of Individual Perceptions of the Friendship Context, and Interactions Within Best Friend Dyads and Friendship Groups." Thesis, Griffith University, 2013. http://hdl.handle.net/10072/367872.

Full text
Abstract:
When we consider society’s fixation with physical attractiveness coupled with adolescent concerns about social acceptance, it seems inevitable that some young people develop a tendency to expect social rejection based on the way they look. This tendency has recently been referred to as appearance-based rejection sensitivity (appearance-RS; Park, 2007). Building upon the existing theory and research on rejection sensitivity and body dissatisfaction, and on two recent studies that examined the role of social relationships on appearance-RS (Bowker et al., 2012; Park, DiRaddo, & Calogero, 2009), the primary purpose of the studies reported here were to examine the specific and multiple roles of friends in appearance-RS. Following a review of the literature and the development of a modified version of the Appearance-RS Scale (Park, 2007) suitable for adolescents, the Adolescent Appearance-RS Scale (AA-RSS), two studies were conducted.
Thesis (PhD Doctorate)
Doctor of Philosophy in Clinical Psychology (PhD ClinPsych)
School of Psychology
Griffith Health
Full Text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Jiang, Ruifang [Verfasser]. "Rejection Sensitivity : A Cross-cultural Perspective / Ruifang Jiang." Berlin : Freie Universität Berlin, 2016. http://d-nb.info/1121588077/34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

LaDuke, S. L., and Stacey L. Williams. "Rejection Sensitivity and Support Seeking Among the Stigmatized." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2014. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/8081.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

LaDuke, Sheri L., and Stacey L. Williams. "Rejection Sensitivity and Support Seeking Among the Stigmatized." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2014. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/8088.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Chandler, Sheri, and Stacey L. Williams. "Rejection Sensitivity and Direct and Indirect Support Seeking." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2010. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/8121.

Full text
Abstract:
Those high in rejection sensitivity (RS) have expectations of being rejected by others, and often construe vague interpersonal cues as rejection, and react to rejection whether it is true or perceived (Downy & Feldman, 1996). Williams and Mickelson (2008) suggest fear of rejection may guide individuals to seek support through indirect (rather than direct) behaviors, which are associated with low social support. Indeed, Brookings, Zembar and Hochstetler explored personalities of high-RS individuals and found individuals with high-RS likely avoid situations where rejection is possible and remain detached from others rather than attempting close relationships (2001). This study examines RS and the choice to disclose or not to disclose personally negative (i.e., stigmatizing) information to a potentially close individual (friend, family). While previous research has explored this relation using a crude measure of fear of rejection, we expanded that prior work by assessing RS with a well established scale. In addition, the current study explores specific characteristics of a stigmatizing identity (saliency, visibility, secrecy, and thinking about) and their relation with both RS and help seeking. Our hypotheses are that as RS increases direct help seeking will decrease and indirect help seeking will increase. In addition, we believe that the characteristics of a stigmatizing identity would be related to high levels of RS, as well as increased indirect seeking and decreased direct seeking. We collected data from 659 (69% female) students at a southeastern university through participation in an online survey. We tested our hypothesis with bivariate correlations and found that direct help seeking behaviors were negatively correlated with RS (r= -.166; p =.01) and indirect help seeking behaviors were positively correlated with RS (r=.183; p=.01). Secrecy was positively correlated with RS and indirect help seeking (r=.190 and r=.199 respectively; p =.01) and negatively correlated with direct help seeking (r= -.191; p =.01).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Godlee-Campbell, Georgia. "Online Dating Profiles of Rejection Sensitive and Introverted Individuals: Comparison Based on Rejection Explicitness." Scholarship @ Claremont, 2019. https://scholarship.claremont.edu/scripps_theses/1277.

Full text
Abstract:
Prior research has found a connection between dispositional factors such as rejection sensitivity and introversion and online dating behaviors including likelihood of use (Blackhart et al., 2014) and experience of use (Finkel et al., 2012; Whitty, 2008). The present study expands upon prior research to examine the relationship between these dispositional factors, and the impact of the possibility of explicit rejection on self-disclosure in participant-created dating app profiles. Adults between the ages of 18 and 60 will be introduced to an online dating app manipulated to contain either high or low potential for obvious rejection. Participants will then be asked to create a personal online profile. Participant perceptions of their own self-disclosure in the self-created profile as well as their disposition (introversion and rejection sensitivity) will be measured. It is hypothesized that rejection sensitive individuals as well as those rating lower in extraversion will report higher levels of self-disclosure in a non-explicit rejection dating app setting in comparison to an explicit rejection setting. The present research has implications for the field’s understanding of the experience of online dating app use for individuals as related to varying dispositional factors.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Rosenbach, Charlotte [Verfasser]. "Rejection Sensitivity - etiological aspects and psychopathological impact / Charlotte Rosenbach." Berlin : Freie Universität Berlin, 2014. http://d-nb.info/1051812224/34.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

Danquah, Ruth-Ellen. Rejection Sensitivity Journal for ADHD: A Journal for People with ADHD to Manage Rejection Sensitivity by Exploring Ways to Relieve the Level of Pain Associated with Rejection. Lulu Press, Inc., 2021.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

London, Bonita, and Lisa Rosenthal. The Social Stigma of Identity- and Status-Based Rejection Sensitivity. Oxford University Press, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195398700.013.0018.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Kelly, Megan M., and Mark Kent. The Relationship Between Body Dysmorphic Disorder and Social Anxiety Disorder. Edited by Katharine A. Phillips. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780190254131.003.0035.

Full text
Abstract:
Body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) and social anxiety disorder (SAD) are highly comorbid disorders that share high levels of social anxiety, social avoidance, and rejection sensitivity. In addition, in emotional processing studies, patients with BDD and SAD both show a heightened sensitivity to hostility. However, BDD and SAD differ in many important ways, including key phenomenologic and clinical differences as well as treatment approaches. This chapter reviews similarities and differences between BDD and SAD across demographic, clinical, biologic, and other domains. Future research directions for work that may further elucidate the relationship between these two disorders are also discussed.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Hart, Ashley S., and Martha A. Niemiec. Comorbidity and Personality in Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Edited by Katharine A. Phillips. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780190254131.003.0011.

Full text
Abstract:
Comorbidity is common in body dysmorphic disorder (BDD). Major depressive disorder, social anxiety disorder (social phobia), obsessive-compulsive disorder, and substance use disorders are the most frequently co-occurring Axis I conditions. Except for eating disorders (more common in women) and substance use disorders (more common in men), Axis I comorbidity rates in BDD appear similar across genders. Axis I comorbidity is associated with greater functional impairment and morbidity. Rates of comorbid personality disorders in BDD are high. Disorders from cluster C occur most frequently, with avoidant personality disorder the most common. Associated traits include low self-esteem and high levels of neuroticism, introversion, unassertiveness, social anxiety and inhibition, rejection sensitivity, and perfectionism. Research is needed on the relationship between BDD and psychiatric comorbidity, the causes and consequences of comorbidity in BDD, and the relationship between BDD and associated personality traits.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Simmons, Rachel A., and Katharine A. Phillips. Core Clinical Features of Body Dysmorphic Disorder : Appearance Preoccupations, Negative Emotions, Core Beliefs, and Repetitive and Avoidance Behaviors. Edited by Katharine A. Phillips. Oxford University Press, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/med/9780190254131.003.0006.

Full text
Abstract:
This chapter reviews core clinical features of body dysmorphic disorder (BDD) and presents two cases. Individuals with BDD are preoccupied with perceived flaws in their appearance that they view as ugly or abnormal (but that other people view as slight or nonexistent), to the point of experiencing significant distress or impairment in psychosocial functioning. The preoccupation with perceived ugliness triggers a cascade of negative emotions and behavioral responses that are intended to alleviate the appearance concerns but often do not. These responses include repetitive behaviors to check, fix, obtain reassurance about, or hide disliked body areas as well as avoidance of social situations. BDD is associated with high levels of emotional distress and a range of painful emotions, such as depression, anxiety, social anxiety, and shame. Core beliefs often focus on feeling unlovable, inadequate, or worthless. Associated features include high rejection sensitivity and perceived stress and low assertiveness.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Williamson, Timothy. Suppose and Tell. Oxford University Press, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198860662.001.0001.

Full text
Abstract:
The book argues that our use of conditionals is governed by imperfectly reliable heuristics, in the psychological sense of fast and frugal (or quick and dirty) ways of assessing them. The primary heuristic is this: to assess ‘If A, C’, suppose A and on that basis assess C; whatever attitude you take to C conditionally on A (such as acceptance, rejection, or something in between) take unconditionally to ‘If A, C’. This heuristic yields both the equation of the probability of ‘If A, C’ with the conditional probability of C on A and standard natural deduction rules for the conditional. However, these results can be shown to make the heuristic implicitly inconsistent, and so less than fully reliable. There is also a secondary heuristic: pass conditionals freely from one context to another under normal conditions for acceptance of sentences on the basis of memory and testimony. The effect of the secondary heuristic is to undermine interpretations on which ‘if’ introduces a special kind of context-sensitivity. On the interpretation which makes best sense of the two heuristics, ‘if’ is simply the truth-functional conditional. Apparent counterexamples to truth-functionality are artefacts of reliance on the primary heuristic in cases where it is unreliable. The second half of the book concerns counterfactual conditionals, as expressed with ‘if’ and ‘would’. It argues that ‘would’ is an independently meaningful modal operator for contextually restricted necessity: the meaning of counterfactuals is simply that derived compositionally from the meanings of their constituents, including ‘if’ and ‘would’, making them contextually restricted strict conditionals.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Collier, Jay T. Dilemmas at the Synod of Dort. Oxford University Press, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190858520.003.0003.

Full text
Abstract:
Chapter 3 analyzes the Synod of Dort and the British delegation’s participation in that famous international conference of Reformed churches. It investigates the delegation’s Collegiat Suffrage and their response to the draft canons to see how they handled the insistence of other Reformed churches to codify perseverance of the saints as a nonnegotiable Reformed distinctive. This chapter uncovers a consistent English strategy of conciliatory confessionalism, even when it was painfully inconvenient. Even though the delegates agreed theologically with the rest of the Synod on perseverance, their sensitivity to readings of Augustine prepared them to advocate unity among the Reformed churches in a way that would avoid unnecessary offense to those rejecting the prevailing view on the doctrine. And while the British delegation’s request was not granted, this episode demonstrates yet another attempt by Englishmen to use readings of Augustine to shape doctrinal standards within a Reformed context.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

London, Bonita, Gregory Thompson, Vanessa Anderson, and Elizabeth Velilla. "Rejection Sensitivity." In Encyclopedia of Adolescence, 2329–34. New York, NY: Springer New York, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-1695-2_231.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

London, Bonita, Gregory Thompson, Vanessa Anderson, and Elizabeth Velilla. "Rejection Sensitivity." In Encyclopedia of Adolescence, 3086–92. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-33228-4_231.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Downey, Geraldine, Lauren Irwin, Melissa Ramsay, and Ozlem Ayduk. "Rejection Sensitivity and Girls’ Aggression." In Girls and Aggression, 7–25. Boston, MA: Springer US, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8985-7_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Li, Xia. "The Relative Research on Rejection Sensitivity, Self-esteem, Social Support and Social Anxiety of Teenagers." In Communications in Computer and Information Science, 54–60. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-23065-3_9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Derevianko, Andrei, and Szymon Pustelny. "Global Quantum Sensor Networks as Probes of the Dark Sector." In The Search for Ultralight Bosonic Dark Matter, 281–303. Cham: Springer International Publishing, 2022. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-95852-7_10.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractMost dark matter searches to date employ a single sensor for detection. In this chapter, we explore the power of distributed networks in dark matter searches. Compared to a single sensor, networks offer several advantages, such as the ability to probe spatiotemporal signatures of the putative signal and, as a result, an improved rejection of false positives, better sensitivity, and improved confidence in the dark matter origin of the sought-after signal. We illustrate our general discussion with two examples: (1) the Global Network of Optical Magnetometers for Exotic physics searches (GNOME) and (2) the constellation of atomic clocks on board satellites of the Global Positioning System (GPS).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Hammer, C., D. Klanke, P. Dirschedl, B. M. Kemkes, B. Reichart, M. Gokel, and F. Krombach. "Specificity and sensitivity of the cytoimmunological monitoring (CIM): differentiation between cardiac rejection, viral, bacterial, or fungal infection." In Organ Transplantation 1990, 345–51. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands, 1991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-3386-9_44.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Bobier, Lacey. "The Sexualization of Menstruation: On Rape, Tampons, and ‘Prostitutes’." In The Palgrave Handbook of Critical Menstruation Studies, 303–17. Singapore: Springer Singapore, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-0614-7_24.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract This study contradicts extant research asserting that girls rarely connect menstruation with sexuality. Through interviews with post-menarcheal girls, Bobier demonstrates that they relate sexuality with menses, fluidly transitioning between subjects of menstruation and sex. Girls talk about tampons and Trojans in the same breath, discuss “waiting until you’re ready” to describe tampon usage, express a fear of “down there,” and consider “provocative girls” as their opposites. They associate birth control pills with “prostitutes” and express broad apprehension about sexual activity. These attitudes, coupled with their rejection of the pill and tampons, reveal girls’ sensitivity to the taboo surrounding female sexuality and demonstrate their desire to be “good girls.” At the same time, they are aware that they exist within a set of gender and power relations that limit their ability to define their bodies and their sexuality. This is underscored by concerns about rape and resulting pregnancy as an implication of menarche.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Levy, Sheri R., Ozlem Ayduk, and Geraldine Downey. "The Role of Rejection Sensitivity in People’s Relationships with Significant Others and Valued Social Groups." In Interpersonal Rejection, 250–89. Oxford University Press, 2006. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195130157.003.0010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

"Rejection sensitivity and non-melancholic depression." In Modelling and Managing the Depressive Disorders, 183–92. Cambridge University Press, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511544194.022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Downey, Geraldine, Cheryl Bonica, and Claudia Rincón. "Rejection Sensitivity and Adolescent Romantic Relationships." In The Development of Romantic Relationships in Adolescence, 148–74. Cambridge University Press, 1999. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781316182185.008.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

Yu, Yeong Ran, Gi Young Youk, and Sung Hee Lee. "Factors Affecting Rejection Sensitivity in University Students." In 10th International Workshop on Healthcare and Nursing 2016. Global Vision School Publication, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.21742/asehl.2016.7.15.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

"Disturbance Rejection Controller Design Based on Sensitivity Function Notching." In Emirates Research Publishing. Emirates Research Publishing, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.17758/er715201.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Eskew, R. T., C. F. Stromeyer, A. Chaparro, and R. E. Kronauer. "Why is chromatic sensitivity greater than luminance sensitivity?" In OSA Annual Meeting. Washington, D.C.: Optica Publishing Group, 1992. http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/oam.1992.ms3.

Full text
Abstract:
At ARVO 1992, we reported that when detection thresholds for luminance and red–green chromatic spots are expressed in cone contrast energy (the space–time integral of the squared contrast produced in either the L or M cones), sensitivity for the optimal chromatic spot was 5–8× higher than for the optimal luminance spot. For an ideal observer of chromatic signals, the noise level in P ganglion cells would not be limiting, suggesting that limits are imposed at subsequent stages. The large chromatic sensitivity advantage could result from (a) lower detection uncertainty in the red–green mechanism; (b) common mode rejection (removal of correlated noise) in the opponent pathway; (c) reductions in luminance gain, relative to chromatic gain, at cortical levels; or (d) greater spatial and temporal integration in the chromatic pathway.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Zheng, Jinchuan, Minyue Fu, Chunling Du, Youyi Wang, and Lihua Xie. "Sensitivity loop shaping for disturbance rejection in hard disk drives." In 2009 IEEE International Conference on Control and Automation (ICCA). IEEE, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icca.2009.5410584.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

GOMEZ MALUENDA, Hector. "Background rejection and sensitivity for new generation Ge detectors experiments." In Identification of Dark Matter 2010. Trieste, Italy: Sissa Medialab, 2011. http://dx.doi.org/10.22323/1.110.0051.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Zhang, Mingji, Siu Wing Or, and Yiu Man Yip. "Magnetoelectric intrinsic gradiometer with high detection sensitivity and ambient noise rejection." In 2016 IEEE SENSORS. IEEE, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icsens.2016.7808505.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Fister, J. C., L. M. Davis, S. C. Jacobson, and J. M. Ramsey. "High Sensitivity Detection on Microchips." In Laser Applications to Chemical and Environmental Analysis. Washington, D.C.: Optica Publishing Group, 1996. http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/lacea.1996.lwd.6.

Full text
Abstract:
High efficiency separations coupled with rapid response times have been demonstrated on capillary electrophoresis devices micromachined on glass substrates. [1-3] Although detection of single organic dyes has been achieved with fluorescence detection in capillaries, [4] such detection limits have not yet been demonstrated in micromachined channels. Efficient, single molecule detection is desirable for many potential applications such as rapid DNA sequencing. The structure of microfabricated separation channels, however, does not readily facilitate a 90° fluorescence excitation/collection geometry which has been used to achieve high sensitivities in capillaries. [4] This optical geometry allows efficient spatial rejection of scattering at the capillary solution interfaces. Confocal detection in which the excitation source is introduced through the collection optics provides a means of achieving both high axial resolution and high collection efficiency.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Jurisic, D., and N. Mijat. "Low-sensitivity, low-noise, band-rejection and all-pass active-RC filters." In Proceedings of the IEEE International Symposium on Industrial Electronics. IEEE, 2005. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/isie.2005.1529091.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lee, Sung Hee, and Su Jeong Song. "Parent-Child Intimacy, Adult Attachment, Rejection Sensitivity, and Empathy in University students." In 10th International Workshop on Healthcare and Nursing 2016. Global Vision School Publication, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.21742/asehl.2016.7.26.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Jamshidi, Babak, Robert G. Azevedo, Anand V. Jog, and Albert P. Pisano. "Enhanced Cross-Axis Rejection Capacitive Strain Gauge." In ASME 2007 International Mechanical Engineering Congress and Exposition. ASMEDC, 2007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1115/imece2007-43168.

Full text
Abstract:
A novel bending plate capacitive strain gauge is designed, fabricated, and tested to measure strain in the range of 1 to 1000 με. This silicon-based strain sensor uses a unique structural design to increase the on-axis gain through the use of a bending beam structure while attenuating signals due to cross-axis strain. A differential capacitive measurement is used to improve the output, reduce the parasitic capacitance, and eliminate the capacitance measurement error due to temperature. The device is fabricated using silicon-on-insulator (SOI) technology. Experimental results exhibit an on-axis sensitivity of 50 aF/με and attenuation of the cross-axis sensitivity to shear strain to less than 10 percent of the applied shear strain. A detailed mechanical analysis of the suspension and deflection-amplifying bent-beam capacitor will be presented. Furthermore, the capacitive plate analytical model is compared to finite element simulations and verified with experimental results. In addition, a noise assessment of the device shows the electronics noise dominates the Brownian noise.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Rejection sensitivity"

1

Fishman, George S. Sensitivity Analysis Using the Monte Carlo Acceptance-Rejection Method. Fort Belvoir, VA: Defense Technical Information Center, September 1988. http://dx.doi.org/10.21236/ada201261.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography