To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Selectivity of memory.

Journal articles on the topic 'Selectivity of memory'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Selectivity of memory.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Eron, C. "Neuron Selectivity: Down Memory Lane." Science News 133, no. 26 (1988): 407. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3972200.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Joseph, Abhinav, Joana S. Rodrigues Alves, Carlos E. S. Bernardes, M. Fátima M. Piedade, and Manuel E. Minas da Piedade. "Tautomer selection through solvate formation: the case of 5-hydroxynicotinic acid." CrystEngComm 21, no. 13 (2019): 2220–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1039/c8ce02108b.

Full text
Abstract:
Different 5-hydroxynicotinic acid tautomers were selectively captured through solvate formation. The selectivity is lost once the memory of solvation is erased by removing the solvent from the crystal lattice.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Master, D. R., C. Thompson, G. Dunn, and W. A. Lishman. "Memory selectivity and unilateral cerebral dysfunction." Psychological Medicine 16, no. 4 (1986): 781–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s003329170001179x.

Full text
Abstract:
SynopsisThe relative speed of recall of pleasant and unpleasant experiences was investigated in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and after unilateral temporal lobectomy. Indications have been obtained that right, but not left, temporal lobe dysfunction may impair hedonic aspects of memory selectivity.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Bor, Daniel, and Adrian M. Owen. "Working Memory: Linking Capacity with Selectivity." Current Biology 16, no. 4 (2006): R136—R138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.02.002.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Zhai, F., J. Chen, and J. Lu. "Selectivity in fear extinction learning and memory." Sleep Medicine 64 (December 2019): S438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2019.11.1229.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Saverino, Cristina, Zainab Fatima, Saman Sarraf, Anita Oder, Stephen C. Strother, and Cheryl L. Grady. "The Associative Memory Deficit in Aging Is Related to Reduced Selectivity of Brain Activity during Encoding." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 28, no. 9 (2016): 1331–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00970.

Full text
Abstract:
Human aging is characterized by reductions in the ability to remember associations between items, despite intact memory for single items. Older adults also show less selectivity in task-related brain activity, such that patterns of activation become less distinct across multiple experimental tasks. This reduced selectivity or dedifferentiation has been found for episodic memory, which is often reduced in older adults, but not for semantic memory, which is maintained with age. We used fMRI to investigate whether there is a specific reduction in selectivity of brain activity during associative e
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Chen, Ying-Chen, Yifu Huang, Sumant Sarkar, John Gibbs, and Jack Lee. "Direct-Grown Helical-Shaped Tungsten-Oxide-Based Devices with Reconfigurable Selectivity for Memory Applications." Journal of Low Power Electronics and Applications 12, no. 4 (2022): 55. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/jlpea12040055.

Full text
Abstract:
In this study, a direct-grown helical-shaped tungsten-oxide-based (h-WOx) selection device is presented for emerging memory applications. The selectivity in the selection devices is from 10 to 103 with a low off-current of 0.1 to 0.01 nA. In addition, the selectivity of volatile switching in the h-WOx selection devices is reconfigurable with a pseudo RESET process on the one-time negative voltage operations. The helical-shaped selection devices with the glancing angle deposition (GLAD) method show good compatibility, low power consumption, good selectivity, and good reconfigurability for next-
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Hill, Paul F., Danielle R. King, and Michael D. Rugg. "Age Differences In Retrieval-Related Reinstatement Reflect Age-Related Dedifferentiation At Encoding." Cerebral Cortex 31, no. 1 (2020): 106–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhaa210.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Age-related reductions in neural selectivity have been linked to cognitive decline. We examined whether age differences in the strength of retrieval-related cortical reinstatement could be explained by analogous differences in neural selectivity at encoding, and whether reinstatement was associated with memory performance in an age-dependent or an age-independent manner. Young and older adults underwent fMRI as they encoded words paired with images of faces or scenes. During a subsequent scanned memory test participants judged whether test words were studied or unstudied and, for word
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Suthana, Nanthia A., Neelroop N. Parikshak, Arne D. Ekstrom, et al. "Specific responses of human hippocampal neurons are associated with better memory." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112, no. 33 (2015): 10503–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1423036112.

Full text
Abstract:
A population of human hippocampal neurons has shown responses to individual concepts (e.g., Jennifer Aniston) that generalize to different instances of the concept. However, recordings from the rodent hippocampus suggest an important function of these neurons is their ability to discriminate overlapping representations, or pattern separate, a process that may facilitate discrimination of similar events for successful memory. In the current study, we explored whether human hippocampal neurons can also demonstrate the ability to discriminate between overlapping representations and whether this s
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Garcia, Sarah, and Sara M. Moorman. "College Selectivity and Later-Life Memory Function: Evidence From the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study." Research on Aging 43, no. 1 (2020): 14–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0164027520927137.

Full text
Abstract:
Research has shown a consistent association between college completion and laterlife cognition. We extend this work by examining whether college selectivity—the achievement level required to gain admission to a college—is associated with memory functioning more than 50 years later. We analyze data from 10,317 participants in the 1957–2011 Wisconsin Longitudinal Study to examine the relationship between college selectivity and later-life memory. Models control for childhood, midlife socioeconomic status, and later-life health and adjust for selection bias. Selective college attendance was assoc
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Le Donne, Anthony. "The Problem of Selectivity in Memory Research: A Response to Zeba Crook." Journal for the Study of the Historical Jesus 11, no. 1 (2013): 77–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/17455197-01101005.

Full text
Abstract:
Zeba Crook argues that there is an emerging consensus that the Gospels are reliable historical narratives by those to have applied ‘memory’ theories to historical Jesus research. Crook argues that this emerging consensus betrays a selective reading of research done on ‘memory distortion’ in interdisciplinary study. This essay demonstrates that Crook misunderstands and misrepresents social memory theory both in and outside Jesus studies. A better understanding would have properly represented the spectrum from theoretical ‘presentism’ to ‘continuitism’ in memory applications/adaptations.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

McKeefry, D. J., and M. P. Burton. "Speed selectivity in visual short term memory for motion." Journal of Vision 6, no. 13 (2010): 23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/6.13.23.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Littlefield, L. "Working with semantic memory: Frontal lobe selectivity and judgement." Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 13, no. 1 (1998): 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6177(98)90524-8.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

McKeefry, D. J., M. P. Burton, and C. Vakrou. "Speed selectivity in visual short term memory for motion." Vision Research 47, no. 18 (2007): 2418–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.011.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Littlefield, L. M., G. Ledakis, and C. Armstrong. "Working with semantic memory: Frontal lobe selectivity and judgement." Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 13, no. 1 (1998): 88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/13.1.88a.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Su, Wei-Chia, and Chien-Hong Lin. "Enhancement of the angular selectivity in encrypted holographic memory." Applied Optics 43, no. 11 (2004): 2298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1364/ao.43.002298.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

РОДІНА, Катерина Миколаївна. "ХАРАКТЕРИСТИКА ОБРАЗНОЇ ПАМ'ЯТІ В ЮНАКІВ ІЗ РІЗНИМ РІВНЕМ ЗДАТНОСТІ ДО ІМАГІНАЦІЇ". Психологія і особистість 2, № 12 (2017): 149–58. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.853445.

Full text
Abstract:
The article presents an analysis of the concepts of the ability to imagination, visual memory and investigates the relationship between them. Levels of the ability to make imaginations in adolescence are revealed. It is shown that a high level of imagination ability is characterized by a high level of involuntary memorization, selectivity, completeness and accuracy of memorization of visual material.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Kornyushchenko-Ermolaeva, N. S. "Features of Semantic Translation of Collective Historical Memory." Tempus et Memoria 3, no. 2 (2022): 30–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.15826/tetm.2022.3.035.

Full text
Abstract:
The article is devoted to the methodological problem of the interaction of individual and collective in the phenomenon of historical memory. The identification of specific features of collective memory, the nature and dynamics of collective memories allows, on the one hand, to conceptualize collective memory, to substantiate its ontological status, on the other, to reveal the features of the mechanisms of collective memories. The article analyzes such features of collective memory as conventionality, tradition, repeatability, selectivity, etc. The thesis about the ambivalent nature of collecti
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Hirel, C., Y. Lévêque, L. Fornoni, N. Nighoghossian, B. Tillmann, and A. Caclin. "Verbal and musical memory: Selectivity of auditory disorders after stroke." Annals of Physical and Rehabilitation Medicine 58 (September 2015): e69-e70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rehab.2015.07.170.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Kessler, B. S., and A. S. Arrott. "Improved selectivity in magnetic random access memory arrays using hysterons." Journal of Applied Physics 97, no. 10 (2005): 10C502. http://dx.doi.org/10.1063/1.1846971.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Siegel, Alexander L. M., and Alan D. Castel. "Age-related differences in metacognition for memory capacity and selectivity." Memory 27, no. 9 (2019): 1236–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1645859.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Kojima, Satoshi, and Allison J. Doupe. "Song Selectivity in the Pallial-Basal Ganglia Song Circuit of Zebra Finches Raised Without Tutor Song Exposure." Journal of Neurophysiology 98, no. 4 (2007): 2099–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.00916.2006.

Full text
Abstract:
Acoustic experience critically influences auditory cortical development as well as emergence of highly selective auditory neurons in the songbird sensorimotor circuit. In adult zebra finches, these “song-selective” neurons respond better to the bird's own song (BOS) than to songs of other conspecifics. Birds learn their songs by memorizing a tutor's song and then matching auditory feedback of their voice to the tutor song memory. Song-selective neurons in the pallial-basal ganglia circuit called the anterior forebrain pathway (AFP) reflect the development of BOS. However, during learning, they
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Savic, Miroslav M., Dragan I. Obradovic, Nenad D. Ugrešic, and Dubravko R. Bokonjic. "Memory Effects of Benzodiazepines: Memory Stages and Types Versus Binding-Site Subtypes." Neural Plasticity 12, no. 4 (2005): 289–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/np.2005.289.

Full text
Abstract:
Benzodiazepines are well established as inhibitory modulators of memory processing. This effect is especially prominent when applied before the acquisition phase of a memory task. This minireview concentrates on the putative subtype selectivity of the acquisition-impairing action of benzodiazepines. Namely, recent genetic studies and standard behavioral tests employing subtype-selective ligands pointed to the predominant involvement of two subtypes of benzodiazepine binding sites in memory modulation. Explicit memory learning seems to be affected through theGABAAreceptors containing theα1andα5
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Pasternak, Tatiana, and Duje Tadin. "Linking Neuronal Direction Selectivity to Perceptual Decisions About Visual Motion." Annual Review of Vision Science 6, no. 1 (2020): 335–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-121219-081816.

Full text
Abstract:
Psychophysical and neurophysiological studies of responses to visual motion have converged on a consistent set of general principles that characterize visual processing of motion information. Both types of approaches have shown that the direction and speed of target motion are among the most important encoded stimulus properties, revealing many parallels between psychophysical and physiological responses to motion. Motivated by these parallels, this review focuses largely on more direct links between the key feature of the neuronal response to motion, direction selectivity, and its utilization
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Reinert, Sandra, Mark Hübener, Tobias Bonhoeffer, and Pieter M. Goltstein. "Mouse prefrontal cortex represents learned rules for categorization." Nature 593, no. 7859 (2021): 411–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03452-z.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThe ability to categorize sensory stimuli is crucial for an animal’s survival in a complex environment. Memorizing categories instead of individual exemplars enables greater behavioural flexibility and is computationally advantageous. Neurons that show category selectivity have been found in several areas of the mammalian neocortex1–4, but the prefrontal cortex seems to have a prominent role4,5 in this context. Specifically, in primates that are extensively trained on a categorization task, neurons in the prefrontal cortex rapidly and flexibly represent learned categories6,7. However,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Furey, M. L. "Cholinergic Enhancement and Increased Selectivity of Perceptual Processing During Working Memory." Science 290, no. 5500 (2000): 2315–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.290.5500.2315.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Barlow, Horace. "Intraneuronal information processing, directional selectivity and memory for spatio-temporal sequences." Network: Computation in Neural Systems 7, no. 2 (1996): 251–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0954-898x/7/2/004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Barlow, Horace. "Intraneuronal information processing, directional selectivity and memory for spatio-temporal sequences." Network: Computation in Neural Systems 7, no. 2 (1996): 251–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1088/0954-898x_7_2_004.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Li, Juan, Lars-Goran Nilsson, and Zhenyun Wu. "Effects of age and anxiety on episodic memory: Selectivity and variability." Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 45, no. 2 (2004): 123–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2004.00387.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Markov, V. B. "Holographic memory based on the angular speckle-selectivity of volume holograms." Technical Physics Letters 24, no. 4 (1998): 284–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1134/1.1262085.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Middlebrooks, Catherine D., Kou Murayama, and Alan D. Castel. "The value in rushing: Memory and selectivity when short on time." Acta Psychologica 170 (October 2016): 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.06.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Toulopoulou, Timothea, Robin G. Morris, Sophia Rabe-Hesketh, and Robin M. Murray. "Selectivity of verbal memory deficit in schizophrenic patients and their relatives." American Journal of Medical Genetics 116B, no. 1 (2002): 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.10027.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Gottlieb, Alice B. "Alefacept is Well Tolerated in Patients with Chronic Plaque Psoriasis." Journal of Cutaneous Medicine and Surgery 8, no. 2_suppl (2004): 14–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/12034754040080s204.

Full text
Abstract:
Traditional systemic treatments for moderate to severe chronic plaque psoriasis are often poorly tolerated and are associated with safety concerns that restrict their long-term use. Alefacept is a fully human fusion protein that selectively targets memory T cells, and it is expected to provide enhanced safety over traditional nonselective agents. The safety and tolerability profile of alefacept is reviewed using data from the clinical development program. The most common adverse events were similar among alefacept and placebo groups. As expected from its mechanism of action, alefacept reduced
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Peelen, Marius V., Domenica Romagno, and Alfonso Caramazza. "Independent Representations of Verbs and Actions in Left Lateral Temporal Cortex." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24, no. 10 (2012): 2096–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00257.

Full text
Abstract:
Verbs and nouns differ not only on formal linguistic grounds but also in what they typically refer to: Verbs typically refer to actions, whereas nouns typically refer to objects. Prior neuroimaging studies have revealed that regions in the left lateral temporal cortex (LTC), including the left posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), respond selectively to action verbs relative to object nouns. Other studies have implicated the left pMTG in action knowledge, raising the possibility that verb selectivity in LTC may primarily reflect action-specific semantic features. Here, using functional neuro
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

A. Majcherek, Janusz. "Critical Pedagogy in the Face of the Arbitrariness of Places of Memory and Oblivion." Lubelski Rocznik Pedagogiczny 44, no. 1 (2025): 27–41. https://doi.org/10.17951/lrp.2025.44.1.27-41.

Full text
Abstract:
Introduction: Location, territorial assignment, is an element of identity, both individual and collective They are expressed by such terms as Tarnovian (Tarnovians), Widzewian (Widzewians), Silesian (Silesians), New Yorker (New Yorkers), Alsatian (Alsatians), etc. Since the place of origin and residence significantly determines identity, its past becomes important for its formation, and places preserved and distinguished in individual and collective memory become special objects of care, often cult. However, as they are critical for the formation of identity, they are exposed to manipulation,
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Arrott, Anthony S. "Solving the selectivity problem in magnetic random access memories using configurations that form C-states." International Journal of Materials Research 93, no. 10 (2002): 963–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/ijmr-2002-0167.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract The selectivity problem in magnetic random access memory is caused by processing problems that are metallurgical in origin. In honor of the 70th birthday of Professor Kronmüller, who has made so many contributions to magnetism and metallurgy, a solution of the selectivity problem is presented in which the metallurgical problems are circumvented by the design of the patterned thin film element to make it insensitive to processing problems. The solution is theoretical, using micromagnetics, the limitations of which call for experiments to support the conclusions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Simchoni, Noa, and Charlotte Cunningam-Rundles. "Selectivity of Toll-like receptor stimulation in human B cells (HUM8P.350)." Journal of Immunology 192, no. 1_Supplement (2014): 185.25. http://dx.doi.org/10.4049/jimmunol.192.supp.185.25.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract B cells activated by nucleic-acid sensing Toll-like receptor 7 and TLR9 proliferate and secrete immune globulin. Memory B cells are more responsive due to higher TLR expression levels, but further selectivity remains largely unknown. In this study, human B cells stimulated by TLR ligands, with or without IFNα, or by T cell mimic CD40L plus IL-21 were examined to identify differentially responsive subsets, defined phenotypically or through BCR characteristics. Both stimuli induced CD27hiCD38hi plasmablasts, with TLRs inducing more IgM+ plasmablasts and IgM+ CD27hiCD38lo pre-plasmablast
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Sakai, K., Y. Naya, and Y. Miyashita. "Neuronal tuning and associative mechanisms in form representation." Learning & Memory 1, no. 2 (1994): 83–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.1.2.83.

Full text
Abstract:
We examine the hypothesis that the form representation in the anterior inferotemporal (AIT) cortex is acquired through learning. According to this hypothesis, perceptual aspects of the temporal association area are closely related to its visual representation, in that the response selectivity of AIT neurons can be influenced by visual experience. On the basis of the neurophysiological evidence, we summarize two neuronal mechanisms that subserve the acquisition of form selectivity in AIT neurons. The first mechanism is neuronal tuning to particular stimuli that were learned in a cognitive task.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Tong, Tao, Shujuan Wang, Jie Zhao, Baochang Cheng, Yanhe Xiao, and Shuijin Lei. "Erasable memory properties of spectral selectivity modulated by temperature and bias in an individual CdS nanobelt-based photodetector." Nanoscale Horizons 4, no. 1 (2019): 138–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1039/c8nh00182k.

Full text
Abstract:
Single CdS nanobelt-based photodetectors show an ultra-high response to light close to the bandgap energy at large bias, and moreover their spectral response range selectivity to above- and below-bandgap light can show an reversable memory by temperature-writing and bias-erasing.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Ge, Jie, Yi Yang, Xiao-Ning Li, and Tianling Ren. "Mechanisms for plasma etching of RRAM SiO2 with diblock copolymer selectivity." Modern Physics Letters B 28, no. 18 (2014): 1450149. http://dx.doi.org/10.1142/s0217984914501498.

Full text
Abstract:
To minimize the critical dimension of resistive switching random access memory (RRAM), good anisotropy and selectivity with diblock copolymer are required for silicon dioxide etching. Inductively coupled plasma (ICP) etcher using CHF 3/ H 2 mixture is used for effective etching of SiO 2. In this paper, a commercial software CFD-ACE+ was used to simulate reactor scale and feature scale model of SiO 2, diblock copolymer and Pt . Etch properties of SiO 2 at different chamber conditions were discussed. It was found that etch rate increased at the expense of selectivity as ICP power increased, whic
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Eldar, Eran, Yael Niv, and Jonathan D. Cohen. "Do You See the Forest or the Tree? Neural Gain and Breadth Versus Focus in Perceptual Processing." Psychological Science 27, no. 12 (2016): 1632–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797616665578.

Full text
Abstract:
When perceiving rich sensory information, some people may integrate its various aspects, whereas other people may selectively focus on its most salient aspects. We propose that neural gain modulates the trade-off between breadth and selectivity, such that high gain focuses perception on those aspects of the information that have the strongest, most immediate influence, whereas low gain allows broader integration of different aspects. We illustrate our hypothesis using a neural-network model of ambiguous-letter perception. We then report an experiment demonstrating that, as predicted by the mod
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Smith, A. P. "Effects of Time of Day, Introversion and Neuroticism on Selectivity in Memory and Attention." Perceptual and Motor Skills 74, no. 3 (1992): 851–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.74.3.851.

Full text
Abstract:
Two experiments examined the effects of time of day, introversion and neuroticism on selectivity in memory and attention. The first experiment showed that none of these factors interacted with task priority, which suggests that such variables do not produce the same changes in selectivity as do exogenous factors such as noise. In Exp. 2, colour names were read more quickly in the late afternoon than in the early morning, whereas patches of colour were named more quickly in the morning. Results from the Stroop interference condition showed that subjects classified as extraverts on the basis of
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Staffell, Simon. "The Mappe and the Bible: Nation, Empire and the Collective Memory of Jonah." Biblical Interpretation 16, no. 5 (2008): 476–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156851508x341238.

Full text
Abstract:
AbstractThis article uses the work of the English cartographer John Speed as a way to explore the role of the collective memory of Jonah in social and political discourses during the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries. The paper engages with debates concerning nationalism during the early modern period. Collective memory theory is also used to consider how Jonah became a reified site of memory. By placing Speed's writing alongside the works of his forebears and examining the function of the Jonah text within three sermons, the evolving collective memory of the biblical text, and its imagined
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

An, Dongchen, Guilherme Salgado Carrazoni, Ben-Hur Souto das Neves, Rudi D’Hooge, Steve Peigneur, and Jan Tytgat. "The Sobering Sting: Oleoyl Serotonin Is a Novel Stephanoconus Snail Venom-Derived Antagonist of Cannabinoid Receptors That Counteracts Learning and Memory Deficits." Biomedicines 12, no. 2 (2024): 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines12020454.

Full text
Abstract:
Cannabinoid receptors (CB1 and CB2) are promising targets for a better understanding of neurological diseases. Nevertheless, only a few ligands of CB have reached clinical application so far. Venoms are considered as interesting sources of novel biologically active compounds. Here, we describe an endocannabinoid-like molecule, oleoyl serotonin (OS), present in the venom of Stephanoconus snails. Using electrophysiological assays, it was shown that OS inhibits CB1 and CB2. Structure–activity relationship studies using a chimeric CB1/2 revealed that the domain encompassing the transmembrane helix
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Wilson, Donald A. "Binaral Interactions in the Rat Piriform Cortex." Journal of Neurophysiology 78, no. 1 (1997): 160–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/jn.1997.78.1.160.

Full text
Abstract:
Wilson, Donald A. Binaral interactions in the rat piriform cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 78: 160–169, 1997. Single-unit recordings were made from layer II/III anterior piriform cortex (aPCX) neurons in adult Wistar rats to examine odor response patterns to unilaterally and bilaterally delivered stimuli. Isoamyl acetate odor stimulation was presented either unilaterally through tubes inserted into the external nares, or bilaterally during unilateral olfactory bulb lidocaine infusions. Olfactory bulb multiunit or slow-wave activity was recorded simultaneously bilaterally to monitor selectivity of uni
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Westö, Johan, Patrick J. C. May, and Hannu Tiitinen. "Memory Stacking in Hierarchical Networks." Neural Computation 28, no. 2 (2016): 327–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/neco_a_00803.

Full text
Abstract:
Robust representations of sounds with a complex spectrotemporal structure are thought to emerge in hierarchically organized auditory cortex, but the computational advantage of this hierarchy remains unknown. Here, we used computational models to study how such hierarchical structures affect temporal binding in neural networks. We equipped individual units in different types of feedforward networks with local memory mechanisms storing recent inputs and observed how this affected the ability of the networks to process stimuli context dependently. Our findings illustrate that these local memories
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Adnan, Areeba, Anthony J. W. Chen, Tatjana Novakovic-Agopian, Mark D’Esposito, and Gary R. Turner. "Brain Changes Following Executive Control Training in Older Adults." Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 31, no. 10-11 (2017): 910–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1545968317728580.

Full text
Abstract:
Background. While older adults are able to attend to goal-relevant information, the capacity to ignore irrelevant or distracting information declines with advancing age. This decline in selective attention has been associated with poor modulation of brain activity in sensory cortices by anterior brain regions implicated in cognitive control. Objective. Here we investigated whether participation in an executive control training program would result in improved selective attention and associated functional brain changes in a sample of healthy older adults (N = 24, age 60-85 years). Methods. Part
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Macken, William J., Sébastien Tremblay, Robert J. Houghton, Alastair P. Nicholls, and Dylan M. Jones. "Does auditory streaming require attention? Evidence from attentional selectivity in short-term memory." Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29, no. 1 (2003): 43–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.1.43.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Azmitia, Margarita, William E. Merriman, and Marion Perlmutter. "A Life-Span Study of the Interaction of Selectivity and Knowledge in Memory." Child Development 58, no. 1 (1987): 276. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130308.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Sutterer, David, Joshua Foster, Kirsten Adam, Edward Vogel, and Edward Awh. "Spatial selectivity of alpha band activity declines with increasing visual working memory load." Journal of Vision 17, no. 10 (2017): 332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/17.10.332.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!