To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Sentence Completion Test Integral.

Journal articles on the topic 'Sentence Completion Test Integral'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Sentence Completion Test Integral.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Kobayashi, Tetsuro. "SCT-B: An application of sentence completion test." Japanese journal of psychology 61, no. 5 (1990): 347–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.61.347.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Loevinger, Jane. "Revision of the Sentence Completion Test for ego development." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48, no. 2 (1985): 420–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.420.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Novy, Diane M., and David J. Francis. "Psychometric Properties of the Washington University Sentence Completion Test." Educational and Psychological Measurement 52, no. 4 (December 1992): 1029–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164492052004027.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Jantscher, S., U. Willinger, M. Schmoeger, C. Mueller, and E. Auff. "Validation of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test - German version & Stroop-Test." European Psychiatry 26, S2 (March 2011): 420. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0924-9338(11)72128-9.

Full text
Abstract:
The Hayling Sentence Competion Test (HSCT) consists of two sections and measures response initiation and inhibitory control. In the first section subjects are instructed to produce a single word to complete a sentence and in the response inhibition condition subjects are instructed to produce a contextually unconnected word. Several studies evidence that these abilities, which are associated with frontal lobe dysfunction and dysexecutive symptoms in evereyday life (Burgess & Shallice, 1996b), are impaired in psychiatric patients particularly in patients with schizophenia and major depression (Gohier et al., 2009; Chan et al., 2010; Joshua et al., 2009). The present study aims to examine the adapted German version of the HSCT by Willinger and Diendorfer (2009) upon a non-clinical sample and its relationship to Stroop-Test. The sample was composed of 74 adults (36 women and 38 men) ranging in age from 19 to 66. Reliability analyses of the HSCT yielded a Cronbachs's alpha of 0.45 (response initiation), and 0.92 (response inhibition). Correlation analyses between Stroop Cards 1 and 2 and HSCT initiation condition showed a medium effect (r = 0.378; p = 0.001), further correlation analyses indicated no assocation between Stroop Card 3 and HSCT inhibition condition (r = 0.070; p = 0.560). The high reliability regarding response inhibition seems to be promising. Further investigations considering executive functions such as word fluency are necessary.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

WATANABE, Masayuki, and Rika YAMAMOTO. "MAKING A TEST OF EGO DEVELOPMENT USING SENTENCE COMPLETION METHOD." Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology 37, no. 3 (1989): 286–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.5926/jjep1953.37.3_286.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Peled, Zimra. "The Structural Constituents of Sentence-Completion—Verbal Comprehension Test Items." Educational and Psychological Measurement 46, no. 1 (March 1986): 45–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164486461005.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Park, Heewoong, and Jonghun Park. "Assessment of Word-Level Neural Language Models for Sentence Completion." Applied Sciences 10, no. 4 (February 16, 2020): 1340. http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/app10041340.

Full text
Abstract:
The task of sentence completion, which aims to infer the missing text of a given sentence, was carried out to assess the reading comprehension level of machines as well as humans. In this work, we conducted a comprehensive study of various approaches for the sentence completion based on neural language models, which have been advanced in recent years. First, we revisited the recurrent neural network language model (RNN LM), achieving highly competitive results with an appropriate network structure and hyper-parameters. This paper presents a bidirectional version of RNN LM, which surpassed the previous best results on Microsoft Research (MSR) Sentence Completion Challenge and the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) sentence completion questions. In parallel with directly applying RNN LM to sentence completion, we also employed a supervised learning framework that fine-tunes a large pre-trained transformer-based LM with a few sentence-completion examples. By fine-tuning a pre-trained BERT model, this work established state-of-the-art results on the MSR and SAT sets. Furthermore, we performed similar experimentation on newly collected cloze-style questions in the Korean language. The experimental results reveal that simply applying the multilingual BERT models for the Korean dataset was not satisfactory, which leaves room for further research.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Mousavi, S., A. Massot Tarrus, F. Bihari, S. Hayman Abello, B. Hayman Abello, and S. Mirsattari. "fMRI for language: how can it replace the Wada test?" Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques 42, S1 (May 2015): S27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/cjn.2015.133.

Full text
Abstract:
Background: The goal of our project is to assess the feasibility of replacing the invasive Wada test considered as the gold standard with non-invasive fMRI test for assessment of language dominance preoperatively. Methods: fMRI test with three language paradigm tasks (verb generation, sentence completion and naming) were conducted on our cohort of patients. fMRI laterality indices (LI) were then defined as a ratio (L-R)/(L+R) between the number of activated voxels in the left and right ROIs for Anterior Language Area (ALA) and Posterior Language Area (PLA). fMRI results were divided into the right (LI < -0.2), left (LI > 0.2) or bilateral (-0.2 < LI <0.2) hemispheric language dominance and compared to the results of the Wada test. Results: 28 patients were studied. The concordance rate between Wada and fMRI tests for the ALA and PLA was 68.2% and 52.2% for sentence completion; 56% and 52% for verb generation and 25% and 35% for naming paradigm, respectively. Conclusions: Sentence completion and verb generation fMRI paradigms showed higher concordance with Wada test than naming paradigm. The higher discordance between the Wada test and fMRI was related to bilateral results suggestive of less stringent thresholds used for either test.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Bhattacharya, Snigdhasree, Uma Hirisave, and L. N. Suman. "Development of the NIMHANS Sentence Completion Test for Children and Adolescents." Indian Journal of Mental Health(IJMH) 2, no. 1 (January 1, 2015): 74. http://dx.doi.org/10.30877/ijmh.2.1.2015.74-79.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Potash, Herbert M., Andrea de Fileo Crespo, Sanjiv Patel, and Anita Ceravolo. "Cross-Cultural Attitude Assessment With the Miale-Holsopple Sentence Completion Test." Journal of Personality Assessment 55, no. 3-4 (December 1990): 657–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1990.9674100.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Potash, Herbert, Andrea de Fileo Crespo, Sanjiv Patel, and Anita Ceravolo. "Cross-Cultural Attitude Assessment With the Miale-Holsopple Sentence Completion Test." Journal of Personality Assessment 55, no. 3 (December 1, 1990): 657–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5503&4_22.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Kumano, Michiko. "Differences in sentence completion test responses based on degree of self-disclosure." Japanese journal of psychology 77, no. 4 (2006): 360–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.77.360.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Dhanya, V. S., Snigdhasree Bhattacharya, Uma Hirisave, and L. N. Suman. "Clinical Validity of the NIMHANS Sentence Completion Test for Children and Adolescents." Indian Journal of Mental Health(IJMH) 2, no. 2 (January 6, 2015): 182. http://dx.doi.org/10.30877/ijmh.2.2.2015.182-188.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Kimble, Matthew O., Milissa L. Kaufman, Leah L. Leonard, Paul G. Nestor, David S. Riggs, Danny G. Kaloupek, and Peter Bachrach. "Sentence completion test in combat veterans with and without PTSD: preliminary findings." Psychiatry Research 113, no. 3 (December 2002): 303–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00229-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Novy, Diane M. "Gender Comparability of Forms 81 of the Washington University Sentence Completion Test." Educational and Psychological Measurement 52, no. 2 (June 1992): 491–97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164492052002025.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Barton, Stephen, Stephen Morley, Gillian Bloxham, Catherine Kitson, and Simon Platts. "Sentence completion test for depression (SCD): An idiographic measure of depressive thinking." British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44, no. 1 (March 2005): 29–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466504x19794.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

HAYAMIZU, TOSHIHIKO. "SPONTANEOUS CAUSAL ATTRIBUTIONS: A CROSS-CULTURAL STUDY USING THE SENTENCE COMPLETION TEST." Psychological Reports 71, no. 7 (1992): 715. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.71.7.715-720.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Streich, Douglas D., and Clifford H. Swensen. "Response to Two Presentations of the Sentence Completion Test of Ego Development." Journal of Personality Assessment 49, no. 3 (June 1985): 285–88. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4903_14.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Hayamizu, Toshihiko. "Spontaneous Causal Attributions: A Cross-Cultural Study Using the Sentence Completion Test." Psychological Reports 71, no. 3 (December 1992): 715–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1992.71.3.715.

Full text
Abstract:
The hypothesis was that Japanese students use spontaneous causal attributions less frequently than American students because of their cultural background. To measure spontaneous causal attribution, an original sentence completion test was constructed. The subjects were 123 American and 111 Japanese undergraduate students. Analyzing the descriptions given on the test showed that, as hypothesized, Japanese students were less likely to make spontaneous causal attributions than American students. Instead, Japanese students described ways of resolution to overcome negative outcomes more frequently.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Kohli, Adarsh, Soumya Jhanda, and SusantaK Padhy. "The utility of modified version of sentence completion test for children and adolescents." Industrial Psychiatry Journal 27, no. 2 (2018): 231. http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/ipj.ipj_63_18.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Weiss, Daniel S., Nathan J. Zilberg, and Janice L. Genevro. "Psychometric Properties of Loevinger's Sentence Completion Test in an Adult Psychiatric Outpatient Sample." Journal of Personality Assessment 53, no. 3 (September 1989): 478–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5303_6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Blumentritt, Tracie, Diane M. Novy, John P. Gaa, and Dov Liberman. "Effects of Maximum Performance Instructions on the Sentence Completion test of Ego Development." Journal of Personality Assessment 67, no. 1 (August 1996): 79–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa6701_6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Mooijman, S., A. Vincent, E. De Witte, E. Visch-Brink, and D. Satoer. "OS07.5A Diagnostic Instrument for Mild Aphasia (DIMA): sensitive and valuable addition to standard language assessment in glioma patients." Neuro-Oncology 23, Supplement_2 (September 1, 2021): ii10—ii11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/neuonc/noab180.033.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract BACKGROUND Low-grade glioma (LGG) patients typically suffer from mild aphasia that often cannot be detected with standard aphasia tests. The Diagnostic Instrument for Mild Aphasia (DIMA) is the first standardized test-battery to assess mild language disorders. We investigate pre- and postoperative linguistic abilities of LGG and high-grade glioma (HGG) patients with the DIMA. METHODS The DIMA consists of subtests that tap phonology (word, compound, non-word, sentence repetition), semantics (odd-picture-out), and syntax (sentence completion). Additionally, we administered the Boston Naming Test, Category- and Letter Fluency, and the Token Test. Patients were assessed before awake surgery (T1, N=98), three-months (T2, N=69), and one-year (T3, N=30) postoperatively. DIMA performance was compared to healthy controls (N=214). Group differences were examined with parametric (t-test) and nonparametric (Mann-Whitney-U, Wilcoxon) tests. RESULTS DIMA: Preoperatively, patients deviated on sentence repetition and sentence completion (p&lt;0.05). HGG patients performed worse than LGG on word, non-word, and sentence repetition (p&lt;0.05). There was no effect of hemispheric tumor localization. At T2, compound repetition and odd-picture-out also became impaired (p&lt;0.05) and there was a decline compared to T1 on all repetition tasks (p&lt;0.05). At T3, only sentence completion remained impaired (p&lt;0.01) with a deterioration compared to T1 (p&lt;0.01). Standard tests: At T1, patients were impaired on BNT, Category- and Letter Fluency (p&lt;0.01). HGG patients performed worse than LGG patients on BNT and TT (p&lt;0.01). Patients with left-hemispheric tumors performed worse on BNT and Letter Fluency compared to right-hemispheric tumors (p&lt;0.05). At T2, TT also became impaired (p&lt;0.05) and patients declined compared to T1 on Verbal Fluency tests (p&lt;0.01). At T3, only BNT and Category Fluency remained impaired (p&lt;0.05), with no significant declines compared to T1. CONCLUSION The DIMA is the first test-battery to detect peri-operative impairments at different linguistic levels (phonology, semantics, syntax) in patients with left- or right-hemispheric gliomas. It even appeared more sensitive to detect surgical effects than standard tests: all phonological DIMA subtests captured short-term decline (T1-T2), in line with earlier evidence for the value of (non-)word repetition. DIMA sentence completion detected long-term decline (T1-T3), reflecting earlier spontaneous speech analyses. As expected, Verbal Fluency was also sensitive to short-term postoperative decline. Left-hemispheric tumor localization only affected standard test performance. HGG patients had more severe impairments than LGG on DIMA repetition and standard tests (BNT and TT). We advise adding the DIMA to standard language evaluation of glioma patients, as it allows for more detailed counseling about language outcome.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Mabry, Corinne Hay. "Gender Differences in Ego Level." Psychological Reports 72, no. 3 (June 1993): 752–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1993.72.3.752.

Full text
Abstract:
Entering college students (110 women and 63 men) completed a short form (12 items) of the Washington Sentence Completion Test of ego development. Using a Cramer's Phi, a significant gender difference on ego level scores was found, with women scoring higher.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Ravinder, Shash. "Loevinger's Sentence Completion Test of Ego Development: A Useful Tool for Cross-Cultural Researchers." International Journal of Psychology 21, no. 1-4 (January 1986): 679–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207598608247614.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Picano, James J., Robert R. Roland, Thomas J. Williams, and Kenneth D. Rollins. "Sentence Completion Test Verbal Defensiveness as a Predictor of Success in Military Personnel Selection." Military Psychology 18, no. 3 (January 2006): 207–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp1803_2.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

Endres, Johann. "The language of the psychopath: Characteristics of prisoners' performance in a sentence completion test." Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health 14, no. 3 (September 2004): 214–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cbm.588.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Oshodi, John Egbeazien. "The Construction of an Africentric Sentence Completion Test to Assess the Need for Achievement." Journal of Black Studies 30, no. 2 (November 1999): 216–31. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002193479903000205.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Jurich, Jack, and Robert R. Holt. "Effects of Modified Instructions on the Washington University Sentence Completion Test of Ego Development." Journal of Personality Assessment 51, no. 2 (June 1987): 186–93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa5102_3.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Aydin, Fatma, and Ozgur Yildirim. "A study on the morphological awareness of intermediate level adult Turkish EFL learners." JELE (Journal of English Language and Education) 3, no. 2 (December 19, 2017): 65. http://dx.doi.org/10.26486/jele.v3i2.261.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of the current study was to investigate morphological awareness of a group of intermediate level adult Turkish EFL learners. The participants of this study were 168 Turkish first year university students who were taking intermediate level English courses at a state university in Turkey. Morphological Awareness Test (MAT) was the data collection instrument of the study. MAT consists of two sections: Sentence Completion and Word Relation. The Sentence Completion section consists of 27 multiple-choice test items with the purpose of understanding whether the participants are aware of the grammatical and lexical functions of some common derivational suffixes in English. In the Word Relation section, the participants were provided with a list of 20 word pairs (real words), and asked whether the second word in each pair comes from the first word or not. For analyzing the data, first the descriptive statistics (mean scores and standard deviations) were calculated for the sections of MAT and for individual suffixes, then the comparisons between the sections and among the participants were made by using paired-samples t-tests and one-way repeated measures ANOVAs. Results of the study indicated that intermediate-level adult Turkish EFL learners participating in the present study had moderate to high morphological awareness; they performed better in the Word Relation task than in the Sentence Completion task; and they showed the best performance in the verb making suffixes.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Chun, Min-Ji, Mae-Hyang Hwang, and Hutchison Brian. "Development and Validation of the Career Development Sentence Completion Test for Elementary School Career Counseling." Institute for Education and Research Gyeongin National University of Education 1, no. 2 (December 31, 2018): 13–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.25020/joe.2018.1.2.13.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Ebrahimi, Bahman P. "Motivation to manage in Hong Kong: modification and test of Miner Sentence Completion Scale‐H." Journal of Managerial Psychology 12, no. 6 (September 1997): 401–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683949710176151.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Lowe, B. "The sentence completion test for depression can distinguish between people with and without major depressive disorder." Evidence-Based Mental Health 8, no. 4 (November 1, 2005): 99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/ebmh.8.4.99.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Hogansen, Jennifer, and Kevin Lanning. "Five Factors in Sentence Completion Test Categories: Toward Rapprochement between Trait and Maturational Approaches to Personality." Journal of Research in Personality 35, no. 4 (December 2001): 449–62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.2001.2331.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Shimonaka, Yoshiko, and Katsuharu Nakazato. "Personality development among the Japanese elderly: A ten-year longitudinal study on a sentence completion test." Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology 4, no. 4 (October 1989): 347–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00120578.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Picano, James J. "Automatic ogive scoring rules for the short form of the sentence completion test of ego development." Journal of Clinical Psychology 43, no. 1 (January 1987): 119–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-4679(198701)43:1<119::aid-jclp2270430119>3.0.co;2-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Martyr, Anthony, Elina Boycheva, and Aleksandra Kudlicka. "Assessing inhibitory control in early-stage Alzheimer's and Parkinson's disease using the Hayling Sentence Completion Test." Journal of Neuropsychology 13, no. 1 (June 20, 2017): 67–81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jnp.12129.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

KIM, HYUNWOO, and THERES GRÜTER. "Cross-linguistic activation of implicit causality biases in Korean learners of English." Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 22, no. 3 (June 4, 2018): 441–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1366728918000561.

Full text
Abstract:
This study investigates how the strength of referential biases associated with implicit vs explicit causality predicates in Korean affects Korean-speaking learners’ reference choices in English. Sentence-completion experiments with Korean (Experiment 1a) and English (1b) native speakers showed that Korean speakers referred to the subject more following predicates with explicit vs implicit causality marking, whereas English speakers showed no difference in referential bias for the English translation correspondents of these predicates, which did not contain explicit causality marking. In Experiment 2, Korean learners of English completed an English sentence-completion task, either preceded or followed by a translation task, to test whether strength of referential bias in Korean would affect their referential choices in English. After factoring in individual differences in cross-linguistic associations, results provided evidence that cross-language activation at the word level affects reference processing at a discourse level, with the predicted effect somewhat enhanced by translation priming.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Whalley, Heather, Susanna Flett, Enrico Simonotto, Nigel Goddard, Ian Marshall, David Cunningham-Owens, Klaus Ebmeier, Eve Johnstone, and Stephen Lawrie. "Functional MRI of the hayling sentence completion test in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia: preliminary results." NeuroImage 13, no. 6 (June 2001): 1117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1053-8119(01)92443-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Chan, Kevin K. S., J. Q. Xu, Kristy C. M. Liu, Christy L. M. Hui, Gloria H. Y. Wong, and Eric Y. H. Chen. "Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia: A three-year prospective study of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test." Schizophrenia Research 135, no. 1-3 (March 2012): 62–67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.12.022.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Bayard, Sophie, Marie-Christine Gély-Nargeot, Stéphane Raffard, Estelle Guerdoux-Ninot, Emmanuelle Kamara, Florent Gros-Balthazard, Jean-Pierre Jacus, and Christine Moroni. "French Version of the Hayling Sentence Completion Test, Part I: Normative Data and Guidelines for Error Scoring." Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 32, no. 5 (February 7, 2017): 585–91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acx010.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Montembeault, Maxime, Sabrina Sayah, Daisy Rinaldi, Benjamin Le Toullec, Anne Bertrand, Aurélie Funkiewiez, Dario Saracino, et al. "Cognitive inhibition impairments in presymptomatic C9orf72 carriers." Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 91, no. 4 (February 13, 2020): 366–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp-2019-322242.

Full text
Abstract:
ObjectiveTo investigate cognitive inhibition in presymptomatic C9orf72 mutation carriers (C9+) and its associated neuroanatomical correlates.MethodsThirty-eight presymptomatic C9orf72 mutation carriers (C9+, mean age 38.2±8.0 years) and 22 C9− controls from the PREV-DEMALS cohort were included in this study. They underwent a cognitive inhibition assessment with the Hayling Sentence Completion Test (HSCT; time to completion (part B−part A); error score in part B) as well as a 3D MRI.ResultsC9+ individuals younger than 40 years had higher error scores (part B) but equivalent HSCT time to completion (part B−part A) compared to C9− individuals. C9+ individuals older than 40 years had both higher error scores and longer time to completion. HSCT time to completion significantly predicted the proximity to estimated clinical conversion from presymptomatic to symptomatic phase in C9+ individuals (based on the average age at onset of affected relatives in the family). Anatomically, we found that HSCT time to completion was associated with the integrity of the cerebellum.ConclusionThe HSCT represents a good marker of cognitive inhibition impairments in C9+ and of proximity to clinical conversion. This study also highlights the key role of the cerebellum in cognitive inhibition.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Ma, Rui, Lynn E. Henrichsen, Troy L. Cox, and Mark W. Tanner. "Pronunciation’s role in English speaking-proficiency ratings." Journal of Second Language Pronunciation 4, no. 1 (May 31, 2018): 73–102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/jslp.00004.ma.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Although pronunciation is an integral part of speaking, the role pronunciation plays in determining speaking-proficiency levels is unclear (Higgs & Clifford, 1982; Kang, 2013). To contribute to our understanding of this area, the research reported here investigated the relationship between English as a Second Language (ESL) learners’ pronunciation ability and their speaking-proficiency ratings. At an intensive English program (IEP) in the United States, a speaking test was administered to 223 ESL students. Their speaking proficiency was rated using an oral proficiency assessment based on standardized guidelines. In addition, their pronunciation was rated in six categories (vowels, consonants, word stress, sentence stress, intonation, and rhythm) by 11 raters using a rubric specifically developed and validated for this study. Many-Facet Rasch Measurement (MFRM) was used to estimate the students’ pronunciation ability, which was then compared to their speaking ability. The study found that sentence stress, rhythm, and intonation accounted for 41% of the variance in the speaking-proficiency test scores with sentence stress being the most powerful factor.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Rusu, Adina C., and Dirk Hallner. "Idiographic measurement of depressive thinking: development and preliminary validation of the Sentence Completion Test for Chronic Pain (SCP)." Scandinavian Journal of Pain 18, no. 3 (July 26, 2018): 491–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2018-0059.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Background and aims Depression is a common feature of chronic pain, but there is only limited research into the content and frequency of depressed cognitions in pain patients. This study describes the development of the Sentence Completion Test for Chronic Pain (SCP), an idiographic measure for assessing depressive thinking in chronic pain patients. The sentence completion task requires participants to finish incomplete sentences using their own words to a set of predefined stems that include negative, positive and neutral valenced self-referenced words. In addition, the stems include past, future and world stems, which reflect the theoretical negative triad typical to depression. Complete responses are coded by valence (negative, positive and neutral), pain and health-related content. Methods A total of 89 participants were included in this study. Forty seven adult out-patients formed the depressed pain group and were compared to a non-clinical control sample of 42 healthy control participants. This study comprised several phases: (1) theory-driven generation of coding rules; (2) the development of a coding manual by a panel of experts (3) comparing reliability of coding by expert raters without the use of the coding manual and with the use of the coding manual; (4) preliminary analyses of the construct validity of the SCP. The internal consistency of the SCP was tested using the Kuder-Richardson coefficient (KR-20). Inter-rater agreement was assessed by intra-class correlations (ICC). The content and construct validity of the SCP was investigated by correlation coefficients between SCP negative completions, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) depression scores and the number of symptoms on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID). Results As predicted for content validity, the number of SCP negative statements was significantly greater in the depressed pain group and this group also produced significantly fewer positive statements, compared to the healthy control group. The number of negative pain completions and negative health completions was significantly greater in the depressed pain group. As expected, in the depressed pain group, the correlation between SCP negatives and the HADS Depression score was r=0.60 and the correlation between SCP negatives and the number of symptoms on the SCID was r=0.56. Conclusions The SCP demonstrated good content validity, internal consistency and inter-rater reliability. Implications Uses for this measure, such as complementing questionnaire measures by an idiographic assessment of depressive thinking and generating hypotheses about key problems within a cognitive-behavioural case-formulation, are suggested.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Chan, Raymond C., Li L. Song, Kui Wang, Eric F. Cheung, Simon S. Lui, and David Shum. "Poster #146 SEMANTIC INHIBITION PERFORMANCES IN BIPOLAR DISORDERS AND SCHIZOPHRENIA: A META-ANALYSIS OF HAYLING SENTENCE COMPLETION TEST." Schizophrenia Research 136 (April 2012): S143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0920-9964(12)70460-7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Riza, Lala Septem, Anita Dyah Pertiwi, Eka Fitrajaya Rahman, Munir Munir, and Cep Ubad Abdullah. "Question Generator System of Sentence Completion in TOEFL Using NLP and K-Nearest Neighbor." Indonesian Journal of Science and Technology 4, no. 2 (September 1, 2019): 294–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.17509/ijost.v4i2.18202.

Full text
Abstract:
Test of English as a Foreign Language (TOEFL) is one of learning evaluation forms that requires excellent quality of questions. Preparing TOEFL questions using a conventional way certainly spends a lot of time. Computer technology can be used to solve the problem. Therefore, this research was conducted in order to solve the problem of making TOEFL questions with sentence completion type. The built system consists of several stages: (1) input data collection from foreign media news sites with excellent English grammar quality; (2) preprocessing with Natural Language Processing (NLP); (3) Part of Speech (POS) tagging; (4) question feature extraction; (5) separation and selection of news sentences; (6) determination and value collection of seven features; (7) conversion of categorical data value; (8) target classification of blank position word with K-Nearest Neighbor (KNN); (9) heuristic determination of rules from human experts; and (10) options selection or distraction based on heuristic rules. After conducting the experiment on 10 news, it is obtained that 20 questions based on the results of the evaluation showed that the generated questions had a very good quality with percentage of 81.93% (after the assessment by the human expert), and 70% was the same blank position from the historical data of TOEFL questions. So, it can be concluded that the generated question has the following characteristics: the quality of the result follows the data training from the historical TOEFL questions, and the quality of the distraction is very good because it is derived from the heuristics of human experts.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Hammer, Carol Scheffner, Maria Pennock-Roman, Sarah Rzasa, and J. Bruce Tomblin. "An Analysis of the Test of Language Development—Primary for Item Bias." American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11, no. 3 (August 2002): 274–84. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2002/032).

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this research was to examine the Test of Language Development-P:2 (TOLD-P:2; Newcomer & Hammill, 1991) for item bias. The TOLD-P:2 was administered to 235 African American and 1,481 White kindergarten children living in the Midwest. Test items were examined for evidence of differential item functioning (DIF) using inferential and descriptive methods. Sixteen percent of all items of the TOLD-P:2 were found to have DIF. Of these items, 75% were found to be harder for the African American group. The percentages of items on the five core subtests identified as having DIF were as follows: Picture Vocabulary, 17%; Oral Vocabulary, 17%; Grammatic Understanding, 12%; Sentence Imitation, 20%; and Grammatic Completion, 13%. The implications of these findings are discussed in relation to the TOLD-P:3.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Mohamed, Ahmed Dahir, and Chris Roberts Lewis. "Modafinil Increases the Latency of Response in the Hayling Sentence Completion Test in Healthy Volunteers: A Randomised Controlled Trial." PLoS ONE 9, no. 11 (November 12, 2014): e110639. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110639.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Picano, James J., Robert R. Roland, Kenneth D. Rollins, and Thomas J. Williams. "Development and Validation of a Sentence Completion Test Measure of Defensive Responding in Military Personnel Assessed for Nonroutine Missions." Military Psychology 14, no. 4 (January 2002): 279–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327876mp1404_4.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Browning, Deborah L. "Ego development, authoritarianism, and social status: An investigation of the incremental validity of Loevinger's Sentence Completion Test (Short Form)." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53, no. 1 (July 1987): 113–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.113.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!

To the bibliography