To see the other types of publications on this topic, follow the link: Spatial orientation (Psychology).

Journal articles on the topic 'Spatial orientation (Psychology)'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the top 50 journal articles for your research on the topic 'Spatial orientation (Psychology).'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Browse journal articles on a wide variety of disciplines and organise your bibliography correctly.

1

Moraglia, Giampaolo. "Visual Search: Spatial Frequency and Orientation." Perceptual and Motor Skills 69, no. 2 (1989): 675–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1989.69.2.675.

Full text
Abstract:
Observers searched for a Gaussian-windowed patch of sinewave grating (Gabor pattern) through displays containing varying numbers of other such patterns (distractors). When the spatial frequencies of target and distractors differed by ± 2 octaves and their orientations by ± 60°, the search proceeded spatially in parallel irrespective of whether the target could be discriminated in terms of spatial frequency differences alone, orientation differences alone, or their combination. However, when target and distractors differed by only ±.5 octave in spatial frequency and by ± 15° in orientation, the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Matin, Ethel, Caroline Rubsamen, and Donna Vannata. "Orientation discrimination as a function of orientation and spatial frequency." Perception & Psychophysics 41, no. 4 (1987): 303–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03208230.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Carr, Walter, and Beverly Roskos-Ewoldsen. "Spatial orientation by mental transformation." Psychological Research 62, no. 1 (1999): 36–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s004260050038.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Merfeld, Daniel M. "Internal models and spatial orientation." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, no. 3 (2004): 410. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x04350090.

Full text
Abstract:
Aspects of “emulation theory” have been seminal to our understanding of spatial orientation for more than 50 years. Sometimes called internal models, existing implementations include both traditional observers and optimal observers (Kalman filters). This theoretical approach has been quite successful in helping understand and explain spatial orientation – successful enough that experiments have been guided by model predictions.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Presson, Clark C., Nina DeLange, and Mark D. Hazelrigg. "Orientation-specificity in kinesthetic spatial learning: The role of multiple orientations." Memory & Cognition 15, no. 3 (1987): 225–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03197720.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Sanders, Geoff, and Lynda Ross-Field. "Sexual orientation and visuo-spatial ability." Brain and Cognition 5, no. 3 (1986): 280–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(86)90032-1.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Lanca, Margaret, and David J. Bryant. "Effect of Orientation in Haptic Reproduction of Line Length." Perceptual and Motor Skills 80, no. 3_suppl (1995): 1291–98. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.80.3c.1291.

Full text
Abstract:
We investigated the accuracy of haptic reproduction of line length and whether accuracy is influenced by line orientation. 13 blindfolded subjects felt along different line lengths at various orientations in the horizontal plane, then reproduced the line lengths in the same orientation as that felt. Efforts were made to equate learning and reproductive scanning movements. Reproductions of line lengths were a nonveridical power function of their true lengths, but the power function exponents did not differ across spatial orientations. It was concluded that people can encode line lengths across
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Tamborello, Franklin P., Yanlong Sun, and Hongbin Wang. "Spatial Reasoning With Multiple Intrinsic Frames of Reference." Experimental Psychology 59, no. 1 (2012): 3–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000119.

Full text
Abstract:
Establishing and updating spatial relationships between objects in the environment is vital to maintaining situation awareness and supporting many socio-spatial tasks. In a complex environment, people often need to utilize multiple reference systems that are intrinsic to different objects (intrinsic frame of reference, IFOR), but these IFORs may conflict with each other in one or more ways. Current spatial cognition theories do not adequately address how people handle multi-IFOR reasoning problems. Two experiments manipulated relative orientations of two task-relevant objects with intrinsic ax
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Lackner, James R. "Spatial Orientation in Weightless Environments." Perception 21, no. 6 (1992): 803–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p210803.

Full text
Abstract:
Illusions of body inversion and of vehicle inversion can be evoked by exposure to weightlessness in the microgravity conditions of orbital and parabolic flight. Such illusions can involve all possible combinations of self-inversion and vehicle inversion. In the absence of any patterns of external stimulation, individuals may lose all sense of body orientation to their surroundings while retaining a sense of their overall body configuration and cognitive awareness of their actual position. Touch and pressure cues provide a perceptual ‘down’ in the absence of visual input. When vision is allowed
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Larish, John F., and George J. Andersen. "Active control in interrupted dynamic spatial orientation: The detection of orientation change." Perception & Psychophysics 57, no. 4 (1995): 533–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03213078.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
11

Hess, Robert F., and Sima Doshi. "Adaptation to Spatial Offsets." Perception 24, no. 12 (1995): 1407–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p241407.

Full text
Abstract:
After prolonged viewing of a three-element target in which the middle element is spatially offset, subsequent viewing of the same three elements in alignment results in the middle element appearing to be offset in the opposite direction. This adaptational aftereffect to a spatial offset was investigated with elements which were spatial-frequency narrowband and equidetectable to ascertain (a) the properties of the mechanisms involved and (b) the nature of the underlying computation. Evidence is presented in favour of an orientational-grouping, rather than a purely positional computation, underl
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
12

Neidhardt, Eva, and Michael Popp. "Spatial Tests, Familiarity with the Surroundings, and Spatial Activity Experience." Journal of Individual Differences 31, no. 2 (2010): 59–63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000010.

Full text
Abstract:
Spatial orientation as the ability to know the bearing to the origin of a walked path was investigated in two studies with ca. 140 preschool and primary school children who walked paths of about 1 km beginning at the familiar kindergarten or in a completely unknown territory. Path difficulty and familiarity with the surroundings influenced correctness of pointing. Spatial ability measured by test performance and spatial activity experience, i.e., children’s reports about unsupervised walks, effected pointing accuracy as well. The data emphasize that spatial activity experience may be an import
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
13

Burigo, Michele, and Simona Sacchi. "Object Orientation Affects Spatial Language Comprehension." Cognitive Science 37, no. 8 (2013): 1471–92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12041.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
14

Sun, Hong-Jin, George S. W. Chan, and Jennifer L. Campos. "Active navigation and orientation-free spatial representations." Memory & Cognition 32, no. 1 (2004): 51–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03195820.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
15

Chua, Fook Kee. "The processing of spatial frequency and orientation information." Perception & Psychophysics 47, no. 1 (1990): 79–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03208168.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
16

Wühr, Peter. "A Stroop Effect for Spatial Orientation." Journal of General Psychology 134, no. 3 (2007): 285–94. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/genp.134.3.285-294.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
17

Olzak, Lynn A., and Thomas D. Wickens. "Discrimination of Complex Patterns: Orientation Information is Integrated across Spatial Scale; Spatial-Frequency and Contrast Information are Not." Perception 26, no. 9 (1997): 1101–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p261101.

Full text
Abstract:
Real-world objects are complex, containing information at multiple orientations and spatial scales. It is well established that at initial cortical stages of processing, local information about an image is separately represented at multiple spatial scales. However, it is not yet established how these early representations are later integrated across scale to signal useful information about complex stimulus features, such as edges and textures. In the studies reported here, we investigate the scale-integration processes involved in distinguishing among complex patterns. We use a concurrent-resp
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
18

Ringach, D. L., and R. Shapley. "Centre-Surround Tuning of Orientation Detectors in Human Vision." Perception 26, no. 1_suppl (1997): 326. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v970176.

Full text
Abstract:
We present a new psychophysical method to study the tuning of orientation detectors in the human visual system. The stimulus consists of a sequence of sinusoidal gratings of random orientations and spatial phases (but of fixed spatial frequency) shown at a high presentation rate (30 Hz) in 60 s long trials. The gratings are seen through a circular aperture. The subject's task is to report, as fast as possible, by pressing a key, when the presence of a horizontal grating is seen embedded in the stimulus sequence. The data are analysed by calculating the distribution of orientations present in t
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
19

Ruggieri, Vezio, and Mila Ferreira Fernandez. "Gaze Orientation in Perception of Reversible Figures." Perceptual and Motor Skills 78, no. 1 (1994): 299–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1994.78.1.299.

Full text
Abstract:
We hypothesized that during perception of reversible figures the direction of gaze toward a specific perceptual focal point plays a determining role in the identification of the images, i.e., when subjects are asked by the experimenter to perceive one of the two images, a displacement of the eyes toward a specific spatial area of the figure occurs. For each image we think there is a particular point of the figure which acts as perceptual organizer. The stimuli were the Hill and Boring, Ehrenstein, Rubin, and Schroeder reversible figures. Subjects were 47 undergraduate psychology students (32 w
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
20

Bonnet, C., and A. Dufour. "Extracting Distributed Orientations in Different Contexts." Perception 25, no. 1_suppl (1996): 17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v96l0511.

Full text
Abstract:
It has been shown that discriminating the orientations of a target is influenced by the spatial distribution of the orientations of similar elements in the background. However, this effect appears to be essentially decisional (Dufour and Bonnet, 1995 Spatial Vision9 307 – 324). In the present experiment, we explored the accuracy with which subjects can discriminate relative proportions of orientations distributed over a surface (background). Stimuli were textures made of 100 segments with regular spacing. Each of the segments had one of four possible orientations. For each display, one of the
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
21

Morgan, M. J., R. M. Ward, and E. Castet. "Visual Search for a Tilted Target: Tests of Spatial Uncertainty Models." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 51, no. 2 (1998): 347–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713755766.

Full text
Abstract:
We report that spatial cueing of a parafoveal target in the presence of distractors enhances orientational acuity for that target. When no distractors were present, orientation thresholds were in the range 1–4°. For long exposure times, distractors increased threshold by the amount predicted from a winner-takes-all spatial uncertainty model. For short (100-msec) exposures followed by a random dot mask, the rise in threshold with distractors was considerably greater than that predicted from spatial uncertainty. For brief exposures the effect of distractors was greater when the target and distra
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
22

Funk, Johanna, Kathrin Finke, Hermann J. Müller, Kathrin S. Utz, and Georg Kerkhoff. "Effects of lateral head inclination on multimodal spatial orientation judgments in neglect: Evidence for impaired spatial orientation constancy." Neuropsychologia 48, no. 6 (2010): 1616–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.029.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
23

Babler, Timothy G., and Sheldon M. Ebenholtz. "Effects of peripheral circular contours on dynamic spatial orientation." Perception & Psychophysics 45, no. 4 (1989): 307–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03204945.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
24

Bremner, J. Gavin, Linda Knowles, and Gillian Andreasen. "Processes Underlying Young Children′s Spatial Orientation during Movement." Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 57, no. 3 (1994): 355–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1994.1017.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
25

Murias, Kara, Adam Kirton, Sana Tariq, Adrian Gil Castillejo, Andrea Moir, and Giuseppe Iaria. "Spatial Orientation and Navigation in Children With Perinatal Stroke." Developmental Neuropsychology 42, no. 3 (2017): 160–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2017.1306528.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
26

Hill, Oliver W., and M. Hadi Moadab. "Spatial Information and Temporal Representation in Memory." Perceptual and Motor Skills 81, no. 3_suppl (1995): 1339–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1995.81.3f.1339.

Full text
Abstract:
This study examined the effect of spatial cues on memory for temporal order. Improved performance for temporal order with added spatial cues was found only for certain directions and orientations. Dependent measures included memory for items, order, and response latency. The presence of a spatial cue had a significant effect on memory for order but not on memory for an item. For response latency, there were significant main effects for visual field and direction. There were also several significant interactions of visual field, direction, and orientation. The implications of these findings for
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
27

RAHMAN, QAZI, GLENN D. WILSON, and SHARON ABRAHAMS. "Sexual orientation related differences in spatial memory." Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 9, no. 3 (2003): 376–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s1355617703930037.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to investigate and extend previously reported sex differences in object location memory by comparing the performance of heterosexual and homosexual males and females. Subjects were 240 healthy, right-handed heterosexual and homosexual males and females. They were instructed to study 16 common, gender-neutral objects arranged randomly in an array and subsequently tested for object recall, object recognition and spatial location memory. Females recalled significantly more objects than males, although there were no group differences in object recognition. Decompositi
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
28

Bahrami, Bahador, David Carmel, Vincent Walsh, Geraint Rees, and Nilli Lavie. "Spatial Attention Can Modulate Unconscious Orientation Processing." Perception 37, no. 10 (2008): 1520–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p5999.

Full text
Abstract:
It has recently been suggested that visual spatial attention can only affect consciously perceived events. We measured the effects of sustained spatial attention on orientation-selective adaptation to gratings, rendered invisible by prolonged interocular suppression. Spatial attention augmented the orientation-selective adaptation to invisible adaptor orientation. The effect of attention was clearest for test stimuli at peri-threshold, intermediate contrast levels, suggesting that previous negative results were due to assessing orientation discrimination at maximum contrast. On the basis of th
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
29

Morgan, Michael J., Andrew Medford, and Philip Newsome. "The Orthogonal Orientation Shift and Spatial Filtering." Perception 24, no. 5 (1995): 513–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p240513.

Full text
Abstract:
A line abutting two tilted flanks is apparently shifted towards the orientation orthogonal to the flanks and at the same time is reduced in its apparent length. It has been suggested that both effects are caused by band-pass spatial filtering, followed by location of the end points of the line at the peaks in the filtered image. Here implications of the filtering explanation of these effects are explored further. In the first experiment, it was predicted that orientation thresholds (as opposed to biases) would be increased for short line lengths, and would be further increased by abutting bars
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
30

Postma, Albert, Sander Zuidhoek, Astrid M. L. Kappers, and Matthijs L. Noordzij. "Haptic spatial orientation processing and working memory." Cognitive Processing 7, S1 (2006): 181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10339-006-0139-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
31

Ito, Motoo, Tatsuya Sugata, and Hiroshi Kuwabara. "Visual Evoked Potentials to Geometric Forms: Effects of Spatial Orientation." Japanese Psychological Research 39, no. 4 (1997): 339–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1468-5884.00066.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
32

Ringach, D. L., M. Carandini, G. Sapiro, and R. Shapley. "Cortical Circuitry Revealed by Reverse Correlation in the Orientation Domain." Perception 25, no. 1_suppl (1996): 130. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/v96l0711.

Full text
Abstract:
We applied a novel ‘white-noise’-like stimulation technique to study the neural circuitry underlying the orientation tuning of simple cortical cells in cats and monkeys. We generate an image sequence (the stimulus) by selecting, at each refresh time, a random image from a finite set s of orthonormal images. For simple cells, we have shown that the above stimulus allows one to compute the projection of the receptive field onto the subspace spanned by the vectors in s (Ringach et al, 1996 ARVO Proceedings in press). The calculation is based on the cross-correlation between the input image sequen
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
33

Hynd, George W., Jeffrey Snow, and W. Grant Willis. "Visual-Spatial Orientation, Gaze Direction and Dichotic Listening Asymmetries." Cortex 22, no. 2 (1986): 313–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(86)80056-9.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
34

Ramful, Ajay, Thomas Lowrie, and Tracy Logan. "Measurement of Spatial Ability: Construction and Validation of the Spatial Reasoning Instrument for Middle School Students." Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 35, no. 7 (2016): 709–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0734282916659207.

Full text
Abstract:
This article describes the development and validation of a newly designed instrument for measuring the spatial ability of middle school students (11-13 years old). The design of the Spatial Reasoning Instrument (SRI) is based on three constructs (mental rotation, spatial orientation, and spatial visualization) and is aligned to the type of spatial maneuvers and task representations that middle-school students may encounter in mathematics and Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM)-related subjects. The instrument was administered to 430 students. Initially, a set of 15 items we
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
35

Lehnung, Maria, Bernd Leplow, Lars Friege, Arne Herzog, Roman Ferstl, and Maximilian Mehdorn. "Development of spatial memory and spatial orientation in preschoolers and primary school children." British Journal of Psychology 89, no. 3 (1998): 463–80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1998.tb02697.x.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
36

Graf, M., D. Kaping, and H. H. Bülthoff. "Orientation Congruency Effects for Familiar Objects." Psychological Science 16, no. 3 (2005): 214–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00806.x.

Full text
Abstract:
How do observers recognize objects after spatial transformations? Recent neurocomputational models have proposed that object recognition is based on coordinate transformations that align memory and stimulus representations. If the recognition of a misoriented object is achieved by adjusting a coordinate system (or reference frame), then recognition should be facilitated when the object is preceded by a different object in the same orientation. In the two experiments reported here, two objects were presented in brief masked displays that were in close temporal contiguity; the objects were in ei
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
37

Basso, Michael R., and Natasha Lowery. "Global-Local Visual Biases Correspond With Visual-Spatial Orientation." Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 26, no. 1 (2004): 24–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.1.24.23939.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
38

Yamamoto, Naohide, and Amy L. Shelton. "Orientation dependence of spatial memory acquired from auditory experience." Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16, no. 2 (2009): 301–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/pbr.16.2.301.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
39

Sagi, Dov. "The combination of spatial frequency and orientation is effortlessly perceived." Perception & Psychophysics 43, no. 6 (1988): 601–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03207749.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
40

Tolman, E. C., B. F. Ritchie, and D. Kalish. "Studies in spatial learning. I. Orientation and the short-cut." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121, no. 4 (1992): 429–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.429.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
41

Murray, Janice E., Esther Yong, and Gillian Rhodes. "Revisiting the Perception of Upside-Down Faces." Psychological Science 11, no. 6 (2000): 492–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00294.

Full text
Abstract:
In two experiments, the effect of orientation on face perception was assessed. Using a scale from 1 (normal) to 7 (bizarre), participants rated normal, unaltered faces and faces in which changes had been made to spatial-relational properties (eyes and mouth inverted or relative position of the eyes and mouth altered) or to component properties (eyes whitened and teeth blackened). For unaltered and component-distortion faces, bizarreness ratings increased linearly as orientation increased from 0° to 180°. For spatial-distortion faces, a discontinuity in the function relating orientation and biz
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
42

Hauptman, Anna, and John Eliot. "Contribution of Figural Proportion, Figural Memory, Figure-Ground Perception and Severity of Hearing Loss to Performance on Spatial Tests." Perceptual and Motor Skills 63, no. 1 (1986): 187–90. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.63.1.187.

Full text
Abstract:
348 adolescents, 176 male and 172 female, were administered six different spatial tests. Awareness of figural proportion, figural memory, and figure-ground perception contributed significantly to performance on spatial visualization tests but not to performance on spatial orientation tests. Similarly, severity of hearing loss was correlated with scores on visualization tests but not on spatial-orientation tests.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
43

Johnstone, Syren, and Peter Wenderoth. "Pattern Orientation, Not Motion, Determines the Two-Dimensional Tilt Illusion." Perception 18, no. 6 (1989): 729–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1068/p180729.

Full text
Abstract:
In the usual tilt illusion (TI) configuration, an inducing stimulus which has a single orientation is used psychophysically to explore orientation analysis in the human visual system. Recently, this approach has been extended to the use of inducing stimuli which have two orientations. Such a two-dimensional (2-D) stimulus permits investigation of the low-level analysis of visual patterns. Prior experimentation has left it unclear whether it is the spatial or the motion properties of a moving crossed-grating plaid which determine two-dimensional tilt illusions (2-D TIs) because these two parame
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
44

Dosher, Barbara Anne, and Zhong-Lin Lu. "Noise Exclusion in Spatial Attention." Psychological Science 11, no. 2 (2000): 139–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00229.

Full text
Abstract:
Precue validity affects the performance of perceptual tasks. These spatial attention effects have been variously attributed to facilitation of processing, capacity allocation, or noise reduction. We used a new attention-plus-external (stimulus)-noise paradigm and model to identify the mechanisms of attention in cue-validity paradigms. A new phenomenon is reported: a large effect of location cue validity in an orientation identification task that specifically occurs when the stimulus is embedded in external (environmental or stimulus) noise. This result identifies the mechanism of the effect as
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
45

Allahyar, Maryam, and Earl Hunt. "The Assessment of Spatial Orientation Using Virtual Reality Techniques." International Journal of Testing 3, no. 3 (2003): 263–75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327574ijt0303_5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
46

Frassinetti, Francesca, Barbara Magnani, and Massimiliano Oliveri. "Prismatic Lenses Shift Time Perception." Psychological Science 20, no. 8 (2009): 949–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02390.x.

Full text
Abstract:
Previous studies have demonstrated the involvement of spatial codes in the representation of time and numbers. We took advantage of a well-known spatial modulation (prismatic adaptation) to test the hypothesis that the representation of time is spatially oriented from left to right, with smaller time intervals being represented to the left of larger time intervals. Healthy subjects performed a time-reproduction task and a time-bisection task, before and after leftward and rightward prismatic adaptation. Results showed that prismatic adaptation inducing a rightward orientation of spatial attent
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
47

Yashar, Amit, Xiuyun Wu, Jiageng Chen, and Marisa Carrasco. "Crowding and Binding: Not All Feature Dimensions Behave in the Same Way." Psychological Science 30, no. 10 (2019): 1533–46. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797619870779.

Full text
Abstract:
Humans often fail to identify a target because of nearby flankers. The nature and stages at which this crowding occurs are unclear, and whether crowding operates via a common mechanism across visual dimensions is unknown. Using a dual-estimation report ( N = 42), we quantitatively assessed the processing of features alone and in conjunction with another feature both within and between dimensions. Under crowding, observers misreported colors and orientations (i.e., reported a flanker value instead of the target’s value) but averaged the target’s and flankers’ spatial frequencies (SFs). Interest
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
48

Daneri, M. Florencia, Emma Casanave, and Rubén N. Muzio. "Control of spatial orientation in terrestrial toads (Rhinella arenarum)." Journal of Comparative Psychology 125, no. 3 (2011): 296–307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024242.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
49

Grubb, Jefferson D., and Catherine L. Reed. "Trunk Orientation Induces Neglect-Like Lateral Biases in Covert Attention." Psychological Science 13, no. 6 (2002): 553–56. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00497.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to resolve a paradox in the literature on the effects of body orientation on spatial attention. Neuropsychological studies have found that real or simulated trunk rotation relieves contralesional inattention in patients with unilateral neglect, suggesting that trunk orientation affects how attention is allocated to space. However, in two previous studies, trunk orientation did not affect spatial attention in other populations. In this study, we investigated the effects of trunk orientation on the performance of a covert attention task by neurologically intact adul
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
50

Chouinard, Sylvie, Marie-Ève Brière, Constant Rainville, and Roger Godbout. "Correlation between evening and morning waking EEG and spatial orientation." Brain and Cognition 53, no. 2 (2003): 162–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00101-5.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!