Academic literature on the topic 'Speech disfluencies'

Create a spot-on reference in APA, MLA, Chicago, Harvard, and other styles

Select a source type:

Consult the lists of relevant articles, books, theses, conference reports, and other scholarly sources on the topic 'Speech disfluencies.'

Next to every source in the list of references, there is an 'Add to bibliography' button. Press on it, and we will generate automatically the bibliographic reference to the chosen work in the citation style you need: APA, MLA, Harvard, Chicago, Vancouver, etc.

You can also download the full text of the academic publication as pdf and read online its abstract whenever available in the metadata.

Journal articles on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Plexico, Laura W., Julie E. Cleary, Ashlynn McAlpine, and Allison M. Plumb. "Disfluency Characteristics Observed in Young Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Preliminary Report." Perspectives on Fluency and Fluency Disorders 20, no. 2 (2010): 42–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/ffd20.2.42.

Full text
Abstract:
This descriptive study evaluates the speech disfluencies of 8 verbal children between 3 and 5 years of age with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Speech samples were collected for each child during standardized interactions. Percentage and types of disfluencies observed during speech samples are discussed. Although they did not have a clinical diagnosis of stuttering, all of the young children with ASD in this study produced disfluencies. In addition to stuttering-like disfluencies and other typical disfluencies, the children with ASD also produced atypical disfluencies, which usually are not observed in children with typically developing speech or developmental stuttering. (Yairi & Ambrose, 2005).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Furini, Juliana, Luana Altran Picoloto, Eduarda Marconato, Anelise Junqueira Bohnen, Ana Claudia Vieira Cardoso, and Cristiane Moço Canhetti de Oliveira. "The role of auditory temporal cues in the fluency of stuttering adults." Revista CEFAC 19, no. 5 (2017): 611–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1982-021620171954417.

Full text
Abstract:
ABSTRACT Purpose: to compare the frequency of disfluencies and speech rate in spontaneous speech and reading in adults with and without stuttering in non-altered and delayed auditory feedback (NAF, DAF). Methods: participants were 30 adults: 15 with Stuttering (Research Group - RG), and 15 without stuttering (Control Group - CG). The procedures were: audiological assessment and speech fluency evaluation in two listening conditions, normal and delayed auditory feedback (100 milliseconds delayed by Fono Tools software). Results: the DAF caused a significant improvement in the fluency of spontaneous speech in RG when compared to speech under NAF. The effect of DAF was different in CG, because it increased the common disfluencies and the total of disfluencies in spontaneous speech and reading, besides showing an increase in the frequency of stuttering-like disfluencies in reading. The intergroup analysis showed significant differences in the two speech tasks for the two listening conditions in the frequency of stuttering-like disfluencies and in the total of disfluencies, and in the flows of syllable and word-per-minute in the NAF. Conclusion: the results demonstrated that delayed auditory feedback promoted fluency in spontaneous speech of adults who stutter, without interfering in the speech rate. In non-stuttering adults an increase occurred in the number of common disfluencies and total of disfluencies as well as reduction of speech rate in spontaneous speech and reading.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Temple, Liz. "Disfluencies in learner speech." Australian Review of Applied Linguistics 15, no. 2 (1992): 29–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/aral.15.2.03tem.

Full text
Abstract:
Abstract Disfluent phenomena such as pauses, hesitations and repairs, are investigated in a corpus of forty-two short samples of spontaneous speech of native French speakers and learners of French. Significant quantitative differences between native speakers and learners were found and interpreted in the light of Bialystok’s and McLaughlin’s theories of language processing and the two key concepts of automaticity and control. A greater requirement of processing time on the part of the language learners was attributed to their greater use of controlled processes. An analysis of lexical and syntactic selection and repair was carried out to determine where control was applied. While native speakers were attending to the construction of the referent, learners were more concerned with syntactic construction.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Gordon, Pearl A., and Harold L. Luper. "Speech disfluencies in nonstutterers." Journal of Fluency Disorders 14, no. 6 (1989): 429–45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0094-730x(89)90029-6.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Myers, Florence L., Klaas Bakker, Kenneth O. St. Louis, and Lawrence J. Raphael. "Disfluencies in cluttered speech." Journal of Fluency Disorders 37, no. 1 (2012): 9–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2011.10.001.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Boughaba, Hicham. "Speech Disfluencies in Simultaneous Interpretations of Spontaneous and Non-Spontaneous Speeches." International Journal of Linguistics, Literature and Translation 4, no. 7 (2021): 15–21. http://dx.doi.org/10.32996/ijllt.2021.4.7.3.

Full text
Abstract:
The present paper is an attempt to examine speech disfluencies in simultaneous interpretations of spontaneous and non-spontaneous speeches from English into Arabic. It focuses on the difference between the rate of disfluencies in renditions of spontaneous and non-spontaneous speeches. The data were collected from authentic sessions of professional interpreters from English into Arabic. The speeches and interpretations were transcribed. The data was divided into two different categories: ‘spontaneous source speeches and their renditions’ and ‘non-spontaneous speeches and their renditions’. Disfluencies in the source and target texts were analyzed and compared. The results of the analysis showed that the rate of disfluencies in spontaneous speeches is significantly higher than the renditions of non-spontaneous speeches. The analysis also demonstrated that silent pauses are the most frequent disfluency in both categories of interpretations, followed by prolongations and filled pauses.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Półrola, Paweł J., and Jolanta Góral-Półrola. "Speech disfluencies in Parkinson’s disease." Medical Studies 4 (2015): 267–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.5114/ms.2015.56668.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Lease, M., M. Johnson, and E. Charniak. "Recognizing disfluencies in conversational speech." IEEE Transactions on Audio, Speech and Language Processing 14, no. 5 (2006): 1566–73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/tasl.2006.878269.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Hubbard, Carol P., and Ehud Yairi. "Clustering of Disfluencies in the Speech of Stuttering and Nonstuttering Preschool Children." Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 31, no. 2 (1988): 228–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3102.228.

Full text
Abstract:
Cluster formation in speech disfluencies of 15 preschool stuttering children and an equal number of nonstuttering control subjects was studied and compared. Clustering was defined as the occurrence of two or more disfluencies on the same or adjacent words. A computer program, based on the Monte Carlo method, was used to generate sets of expected numbers of single disfluencies and disfluencies in various cluster sizes according to simulation models. Results indicate that disfluencies occurred in clusters more often than could have been expected by chance for both groups. The proportions of clustered disfluencies, as well as the size of clusters, in the speech of the stutterers were significantly greater than those of the nonstuttering subjects.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Befi-Lopes, Debora Maria, Ana Manhani Cáceres-Assenço, Suellen Fernanda Marques, and Marcely Vieira. "School-age children with specific language impairment produce more speech disfluencies than their peers." CoDAS 26, no. 6 (2014): 439–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20142014095.

Full text
Abstract:
PURPOSE: To compare the occurrence of speech disfluencies during narrative production in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and their age-matched peers. METHODS: The study included 60 children aged between 7 and 10 years, 40 with typical language development and 20 with SLI. For data collection, a series of 15 stories was used, each one represented by pictures composed of four scenes. Narratives were transcripted and the speech disfluencies presented on them were classified as stuttering-like disfluencies (part-word repetition, single-syllable word repetition, and dysrhythmic phonation - prolongations, blocks and broken words) or other disfluencies (interjection, revision/abandoned utterances, and multisyllable/phrase repetition). The disfluency categories were compared in each group and its occurrence was also compared between groups. RESULTS: The occurrence of stuttering-like and other disfluencies did not differ among children with typical language development, whereas children with SLI produced other disfluencies. Between-group comparison showed that children with SLI produced more disfluencies of both types than their age-matched peers. CONCLUSION: Children with SLI showed more speech disfluencies during narrative production than their age-matched peers, and the most common disfluencies used by them were not typical of people who stutter (interjection, revision/abandoned utterances, and multisyllable/phrase repetition).
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Dissertations / Theses on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Tseng, Shu-Chuan. "Grammar, prosody, and speech disfluencies in spoken dialogues." [S.l. : s.n.], 1999. http://deposit.ddb.de/cgi-bin/dokserv?idn=956351530.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Crowell, Susan Marto. "A longitudinal study of disfluencies in the speech of normal preschool children." PDXScholar, 1989. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/3870.

Full text
Abstract:
The process of differentially diagnosing a child who is experiencing temporary normal disfluency from one who is beginning to stutter could be made objective by the establishment of normative data on fluency development. To date, there are no standardized norms on the development of fluency in children. Current investigations have contributed greatly to expectations of certain types and amounts of disfluencies in preschool-age children. Most of the research, however, has focused on observing children at discrete age levels from 2- to 7-years-of-age. Only one longitudinal study to date has been reported. Additional longitudinal data of preschoolaged children would benefit the establishment of normative data. Observing the same children over time helps to expose the subtleties that could be missed when looking only at specific age levels. The present study sought to contribute to the investigation of normal childhood disfluency by comparing various types and amounts of disf luencies in 44- to 49-month-old-children to the results of the same group of children when they were 30- to 35-months-of-age.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Christianson, Pamela Paguia. "Disfluencies in normal three-year-old and five-year-old male children." PDXScholar, 1987. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/3737.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to compare the frequency of specific disfluencies in 3 year old and 5 year old normal male children in terms of part-word repetitions, word repetitions, phrase repetitions, interjections, revision-incomplete phrases, disrhythmic phonations and tense pauses. The disfluencies were observed while each child spontaneously interacted with an investigator in a clinical room. Two questions were addressed: 1. Do three-year-old male children exhibit a higher overall frequency of disfluencies than five-yearold male children? 2. Do three-year-old male children exhibit a greater frequency of certain disfluencies than five-year old male children?
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Cox, Mary B. "A longitudinal study of the disfluencies of four and six year old children." PDXScholar, 1989. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/3865.

Full text
Abstract:
Investigations into the speech of normal children have indicated that disfluencies are common. It is important for the Speech Language Pathologist to have knowledge of normal disfluencies for differential diagnosis, parent counseling, and in order to plan strategies for intervention. The purpose of this study was to compare the frequency of disfluencies in 4 year old and 6 year old normal male children to the frequency of disfluencies when they were 3 years old and 5 years old respectively.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

O'Connell, Annette Cameron. "Disfluencies in normal four-year-old Alaska Native and Caucasian children." PDXScholar, 1989. https://pdxscholar.library.pdx.edu/open_access_etds/3901.

Full text
Abstract:
The purpose of this study was to compare the frequency of occurrence of specific speech disfluencies in 4-year-old Alaska Native children to those of 4-year-old Caucasian children. Specifically, eight disfluency types were investigated: part-word repetition, word repetition, phrase repetition, interjection, revision-incomplete phrase, disrhythmic phonation, tense pause, and intrusive schwa. The questions addressed in the study were: 1. Do 4-year-old Alaska Native children exhibit a higher frequency of disfluencies than 4-year-old Caucasian children? 2. Do 4-year-old Alaska Native children exhibit a greater frequency of specific disfluencies, in terms of part-word repetition, word repetition, phrase repetition, interjection, revision-incomplete phrase, disrhythmic phonation, tense pause, or intrusive schwa than 4-year-old Caucasian children? 3. Do 4-year-old Alaska Native and Caucasian children exhibit a higher frequency of low risk disfluency types (word repetition, phrase repetition, interjection, and revision-incomplete phrase) when compared to high risk disfluency types (part-word repetition, disrhythmic phonation, tense pause, and intrusive schwa)?
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Brocklehurst, Paul Harrison. "Roles of speech errors, monitoring, and anticipation in the production of normal and stuttered disfluencies." Thesis, University of Edinburgh, 2011. http://hdl.handle.net/1842/5977.

Full text
Abstract:
In their Covert Repair Hypothesis (CRH), Postma and Kolk (1993) proposed that stuttering-like disfluencies arise, in both normal and stuttered speech, as a consequence of speakers‟ attempts to repair phonological-encoding errors before they start to speak. They posited that stutterers are particularly disfluent because they make larger numbers of such errors compared to normally-fluent speakers. To date, however, experimental research has provided little reliable evidence to support or counter this hypothesis. This thesis constitutes a systematic attempt to provide such evidence. Using a tongue-twister paradigm in conjunction with manipulations of auditory masking, it first documents (a) the vigilance with which normally-fluent speakers monitor for such errors; (b) the relative accuracy with which they detect them; and (c) the frequency with which they occur – in both inner and overt speech. A second set of experiments then extends the same investigation to a group of stutterers and matched controls and explores the relationship between the occurrence of participants‟ errors in the experimental paradigm and the frequency of their stuttering-like disfluencies in everyday speaking situations. Together, these experiments reveal that, compared to controls, participants who stutter monitor their speech with similar levels of vigilance; identify phonemic errors with similar degrees of accuracy; and, as predicted by the CRH, produce significantly more errors – in both their inner and overt speech. However, contrary to the predictions of the CRH, no relationship was found between the frequency of such errors in inner speech and the severity of participants‟ disfluencies. In a final set of experiments, a speech-recognition paradigm is employed to explore an alternative hypothesis: that stuttering-like disfluencies can be precipitated, in a speaker, by the mere anticipation that his words will result in communication failure. Results revealed that, for stutterers, stuttering decreased on words that were consistently followed by feedback implying correct recognition, but not on words followed by feedback implying incorrect recognition. For normally-fluent speakers, equivalent correlations were not found. The thesis concludes that slow or impaired phonological encoding may play a role in the development of the disorder. But, once established, the anticipation of communication failure may be a more important factor in determining where and when stuttering-like disfluencies actually occur. It then discusses implications of the experimental findings for hypotheses that posit a connection between phonological encoding and stuttering.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Williams, Mandy J. "Effects of frequency of disfluency and speaker acknowledgment of speech disfluencies on ratings of communicative competence /." abstract and full text PDF (UNR users only), 2000. http://0-gateway.proquest.com.innopac.library.unr.edu/openurl?url_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:dissertation&res_dat=xri:pqdiss&rft_dat=xri:pqdiss:1399564.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Guntupalli, V. K., S. Venkatesan, Saravanan Elangovan, and V. N. Dayalu. "The Relationship Between Auditory Processing Skills and Disfluencies under Delayed Auditory Feedback in Fluent Speakers." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2011. https://dc.etsu.edu/etsu-works/1564.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Venkatesan, Sundeepkumar. "The Relationship between Speech Disfluencies Produced under Delayed Auditory Feedback and Auditory Processing Skills in Fluent Speakers." Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University, 2010. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/1740.

Full text
Abstract:
Delayed auditory feedback (DAF) is known to produce speech disruptions in fluent speakers. The present study examined the relationship between individuals' susceptibility to DAF and their auditory processing skills. Forty participants (20 males and 20 females) read and produced monologue at no delay and 3 different delay levels of 100, 200, and 400 ms. Auditory processing skills were evaluated using dichotic digits test (DDT) and staggered spondaic word (SSW) test. Males produced significantly more Stuttering-Like Disfluencies (SLDs) under DAF than females. Significantly more SLDs were observed during conversation compared to reading. Overall, there was significant correlation between the frequency of SLDs and auditory performance on SSW test. Females scored significantly better on both SSW test and DDT compared to males. Scores on attention regulation questionnaire were not significantly different between genders. Fewer SLDs observed in females under DAF could be attributed to their superior control of auditory processing resources compared to males.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Silva, Thiago Quintanilha da. "Diagnóstico dos mecanismos disfluentes em orações relativas do português brasileiro." Universidade do Estado do Rio de Janeiro, 2012. http://www.bdtd.uerj.br/tde_busca/arquivo.php?codArquivo=4210.

Full text
Abstract:
Alguns estudos na área da Psicolinguística tratam sobre a interrupção da forma natural da fluência da fala. Essas suspensões são denominadas disfluências e podem ser de variados tipos como, por exemplo, repetições, substituições ou pausas. Entre alguns trabalhos sobre disfluências, destaca-se o realizado por Jaeger (2005), que tratou da omissão do relativizador that na língua inglesa e a possibilidade desse fator ser responsável pela criação de disfluências. Nessa mesma linha, objetivamos analisar as disfluências na camada dos relativizadores camada CP em língua portuguesa brasileira. Para isso, tomaram-se dados obtidos por Corrêa et al (2008) sobre produção de orações relativas. Nesse estudo, os autores, buscando verificar a complexidade da formulação de orações relativas, manipularam duas condições: plenamente planejada que reduziria a carga de processamento e, consequentemente, levaria à maior produção de orações relativas padrões e parcialmente planejada a qual aumentaria a carga de processamento e, consequentemente, maior produção de orações relativas cortadoras e resumptivas. Diante disso, sabendo que o planejamento de estruturas mais complexas pode ser um fator que gere mais sentenças disfluentes, resta saber em qual das condições, estipuladas por Corrêa et al (2008), haveria maior caso de disfluências. Acreditamos que a condição parcialmente planejada resultaria mais casos de disfluências em comparação à condição inteiramente planejada. Para verificação de tal hipótese, a presente pesquisa focou suas atenções no banco de dados de Corrêa et al (2008). Após determinados critérios, privilegiamos sentenças de orações relativas genitivas, orações relativas de objeto indireto funcional e orações relativas de objeto indireto lexical, pois tais seriam construções sintáticas mais complexas e passíveis de terem mais casos de disfluências. Identificadas e quantificadas todas as falas de interesse, criamos tabelas para melhor visualização das disfluências em localizações específicas na sentença como, por exemplo, antes da oração relativa (AOR), na oração relativa (NAOR) e depois da oração relativa (DPOR). Com tais dados analisados, verificamos que eles corroboravam a hipótese levantada: na condição parcialmente planejada há mais casos de disfluências do que na condição inteiramente planejada, principalmente quando se é focalizada a localização NAOR. Entre os principais resultados, percebemos que as disfluências do tipo pausas preenchidas aparecem em grande quantidade na localização NAOR, fator que revela uma característica especial da oração relativa. Essa disfluência, nesse trecho da sentença, revela que os falantes não somente fazem uma procura lexical, como também, um planejamento discursivo. Tais resultados nos motivaram a pensar em outras determinadas situações de pesquisa como, por exemplo, a análise das disfluências em sentenças de voz passiva<br>Some studies in Psycholinguistics area deal with the interruption of the natural form of fluency speech. These suspensions are called disfluencies and can be of various types, for example, repetitions, substitutions or pauses. Among some works about disfluency, the one made by Jaeger (2005) deserves attention. His research dealt with the omission of the relativizer that in English and the possibility of this factor being the responsible for the creation of disfluencies. With this in mind, we aimed to analyze disfluencies in the layer of relativizers - CP layer - in Brazilian Portuguese. For this, data were taken by Côrrea et al (2008) on production of relative clauses. In this study, the authors, trying to verify the complexity of the formulation of relative clauses, manipulated two conditions: totally planned which would reduce the processing load and, consequently, major production of standard relative clauses and partially planned which would increase the processing load and, therefore, major production of gap and resumptive relative clauses. So, knowing that the planning of more complex structures can be a factor that generates more disffluent sentences, the question in which of the conditions stipulated by Corrêa et al (2008), we would find major cases of disfluency. We believe that the condition partly planned results in more cases of disfluencies in comparison with the condition entirely planned. To investigate this hypothesis, this research has focused its attention on the database of Corrêa et al (2008). After certain criterions, were privileged sentences of genitive relative clauses, functional indirect object of relative clauses and lexical indirect object of relative clauses, because these would be syntactic constructions more complex and likely to have more cases of disfluencies. Once identified and quantified all kinds of speech that interested us, there were created tables in order to enable a better visualization of disfluencies in specific locations in sentences, for instance, before the relative clause (BRC), in relative clause (IRC) and after the relative clause (ARC). With such data analyzed, it was found that the data corroborate the hypothesis: in the partially planned condition there are more cases of disfluencies than there are in the totally planned condition, principally when the location IRC is focused. Among the main results, we noticed that the disfluencies such as filled pauses appear in large amounts in IRC location, a factor that reveals a special feature of the relative clause. This disfluency, in this stretch of the sentence, reveals that the speakers are not only looking for a lexical, but also a discursive planning. These results motivated us to think of some other research situations, for example, analysis of disfluencies in the passive voice sentences
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
More sources

Books on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Gilquin, Gaëtanelle. Errors and disfluencies in spoken corpora. John Benjamins Pub. Company, 2013.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Smagac, Melanie. The effect of age of target audience and verbal knowledge on disfluencies and speech characteristics. Laurentian University, 2005.

Find full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Book chapters on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Miltenberger, Raymond G., and Douglas W. Woods. "Speech Disfluencies." In Issues in Clinical Child Psychology. Springer US, 1998. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-5323-6_7.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Verkhodanova, Vasilisa, and Vladimir Shapranov. "Automatic Detection of Speech Disfluencies in the Spontaneous Russian Speech." In Speech and Computer. Springer International Publishing, 2013. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01931-4_10.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Pravin, Sheena Christabel, and M. Palanivelan. "Acousto-Prosodic Delineation and Classification of Speech Disfluencies in Bilingual Children." In Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing. Springer International Publishing, 2021. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-73689-7_59.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Mahesha, P., and D. S. Vinod. "Combining Cepstral and Prosodic Features for Classification of Disfluencies in Stuttered Speech." In Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing. Springer India, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-81-322-2012-1_67.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Szaszák, György, and András Beke. "Toward Exploring the Role of Disfluencies from an Acoustic Point of View: A New Aspect of (Dis)continuous Speech Prosody Modelling." In Text, Speech, and Dialogue. Springer International Publishing, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24033-6_42.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Conference papers on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Zayats, Vicky, Trang Tran, Richard Wright, Courtney Mansfield, and Mari Ostendorf. "Disfluencies and Human Speech Transcription Errors." In Interspeech 2019. ISCA, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2019-3134.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
2

Stouten, Frederik, and Jean-Pierre Martens. "Coping with disfluencies in spontaneous speech recognition." In Interspeech 2004. ISCA, 2004. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2004-570.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
3

Georgila, Kallirroi. "Using integer linear programming for detecting speech disfluencies." In Human Language Technologies: The 2009 Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics, Companion Volume: Short Papers. Association for Computational Linguistics, 2009. http://dx.doi.org/10.3115/1620853.1620885.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
4

Korotaev, N. A., V. I. Podlesskaya, K. V. Smirnova, and O. V. Fedorova. "DISFLUENCIES IN RUSSIAN SPOKEN MONOLOGUES: A DISTRIBUTIONAL ANALYSIS." In International Conference on Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies "Dialogue". Russian State University for the Humanities, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.28995/2075-7182-2020-19-454-466.

Full text
Abstract:
The paper addresses the overall distribution of speech disfluencies in Russian spoken monologic discourse: basing on corpus data, we investigate qualitatively and quantitatively how disfluencies of different types group (or do not group) with each other and how isolated disfluencies and their sequences are sandwiched with periods of fluent speech in the course of speech production. Self-repairs, filled and silent pauses, and instances of hesitation lengthening were annotated in a subcorpus of the “Russian Pears Chats and Stories” (RUPEX). A distribution-oriented typology of disfluencies was proposed that distinguishes between isolated disfluencies, disfluency clusters, and quasiclusters. We claim that disfluency tokens tend to cluster, as isolated occurrences are significantly less frequent in our data than it could have been expected basing on the relative frequency of tokens. This finding contradicts previous studies that treated disfluency clusters as a more marginal phenomenon, and emphasizes the importance of a distributional, rather than merely structural, approach to annotating disfluencies. Furthermore, individual types of disfluency tokens demonstrate significantly different distributional patterns. Compared to other types, self-repairs occur more often in isolation, while words with hesitation lengthening appear predominantly in clusters, and filled pauses most often group with silent pauses to form quasi-clusters.
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
5

Keszler, Borbála, and Judit Bóna. "Pausing and disfluencies in elderly speech – longitudinal case studies." In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech. ELTE Faculty of Humanities, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.21862/diss-09-018-kesz-bona.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
6

Jankovics, Julianna, and Luca Garai. "Disfluencies in mildly intellectually disabled young adults’ spontaneous speech." In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech. ELTE Faculty of Humanities, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.21862/diss-09-021-jank-gara.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
7

Betz, Simon, Petra Wagner, and David Schlangen. "Micro-structure of disfluencies: basics for conversational speech synthesis." In Interspeech 2015. ISCA, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/10.21437/interspeech.2015-129.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
8

Moniz, Helena. "Processing disfluencies in distinct speaking styles: Idiosyncrasies and transversality." In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech. ELTE Faculty of Humanities, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.21862/diss-09-001-moniz.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
9

Farag, Iva, and Heike Brock. "Learning Motion Disfluencies for Automatic Sign Language Segmentation." In ICASSP 2019 - 2019 IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP). IEEE, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1109/icassp.2019.8683523.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
10

Bakti, Maria. "Error type disfluencies in consecutively interpreted and spontaneous Hungarian speech." In The 9th Workshop on Disfluency in Spontaneous Speech. ELTE Faculty of Humanities, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.21862/diss-09-019-bakti.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles

Reports on the topic "Speech disfluencies"

1

Crowell, Susan. A longitudinal study of disfluencies in the speech of normal preschool children. Portland State University Library, 2000. http://dx.doi.org/10.15760/etd.5754.

Full text
APA, Harvard, Vancouver, ISO, and other styles
We offer discounts on all premium plans for authors whose works are included in thematic literature selections. Contact us to get a unique promo code!